Abstract
Spontaneous and sensory-evoked cortical activity is highly state-dependent, yet relatively little is known about transitions between distinct waking states. Patterns of activity in mouse V1 differ dramatically between quiescence and locomotion, but this difference could be explained by either motor feedback or a change in arousal levels. We recorded single cells and local field potentials from area V1 in mice head-fixed on a running wheel and monitored pupil diameter to assay arousal. Using naturally occurring and induced state transitions, we dissociated arousal and locomotion effects in V1. Arousal suppressed spontaneous firing and strongly altered the temporal patterning of population activity. Moreover, heightened arousal increased the signal-to-noise ratio of visual responses and reduced noise correlations. In contrast, increased firing in anticipation of and during movement was attributable to locomotion effects. Our findings suggest complementary roles of arousal and locomotion in promoting functional flexibility in cortical circuits.
- LFP
- Local Field Potential
- V1
- primary visual cortex
- PD
- pupil diameter
- FS
- fast spiking
- RS
- regular spiking
- FR
- firing rate
- L-on
- locomotion onset
- L-off
- locomotion offset
- L
- locomotion
- LE
- early locomotion
- LL
- late locomotion
- Q
- quiescence
- QE
- early quiescence
- QM
- middle quiescence
- QL
- late quiescence
- SNR
- signal-to-noise ratio
- ITI
- Inter-trial-interval
- LV
- local coefficient of variation
- ISI
- inter-spike-interval
- s.e.m.
- standard error of the mean
- SD
- standard deviation
- ID
- Isolation Distance
Introduction
Patterns of cortical activity differ dramatically across behavioral states, such as sleeping, anesthesia and waking1–4. Likewise, neural responses to sensory inputs depend strongly on ongoing patterns of internally generated activity5–7. The generation of multiple activity patterns associated with sleep and anesthesia states has been examined in great detail2–4, 8–10. However, relatively little is known about transitions between distinct waking states, such as quiescence, arousal, and focused attention.
Recent studies in rodents have contrasted inactive vs. active behavioral states, in particular quiescent vs. whisking11–13 or running14–22, and found profound differences in cortical activity patterns that resemble the effects of focused spatial attention in primates23–27. In mouse primary visual cortex (V1), locomotion is accompanied by altered firing rates, a reduction in low-frequency fluctuations in the membrane potential and local field potential (LFP), and an increase in LFP gamma-band oscillations15–18, 22. Enhanced firing rates during locomotion are particularly prominent in inhibitory interneurons15, 16, 19, 20, 22. Locomotion is also associated with an increase in the gain of visual responses15, 16, 19, 22.
Because the most commonly studied active states involve a substantial motor component, it remains unclear whether the associated changes in cortical activity patterns are specific to motor output or more generally attributable to changes in global arousal. Recordings during manipulations of the visual environment suggest that much of the change in firing rates during locomotion is consistent with multimodal processing of visual and motor signals17, 18. The integration of locomotor and visual signals in V1 may thus represent elements of predictive coding or play a role in spatial navigation. However, locomotion-associated changes in cortical activity have been replicated by noradrenergic and cholinergic manipulations in the absence of motor output16, 20, 28. Changes in V1 activity during locomotion may therefore result from recruitment of neuromodulatory systems that regulate global arousal levels.
Wakefulness comprises states of low and high arousal, but the relationship between changes in arousal and cortical activity remains poorly understood. The functional impact of motor feedback signals to sensory cortex is likewise only beginning to be explored13, 21, 29, 30. Here we used behavioral state monitoring and manipulation to dissociate the roles of locomotion and arousal in regulating neural activity in mouse V1. We recorded from V1 in mice head-fixed on a wheel to measure locomotion and monitored pupil diameter to assess arousal22, 31–33. To disentangle the effects of motor activity and arousal, we first took advantage of naturally occurring state transitions where mice initiated or finished a bout of locomotion. We examined the precise time course of changes in the LFP, single-unit firing rates, and pupil diameter around locomotion onset and offset. We further compared epochs of high and low arousal during quiescent states. In a second set of experiments, we causally manipulated behavioral state and induced a shift from low to high arousal in the absence of locomotion by delivering an air puff to the animal’s body. We find that arousal mediates most state-dependent changes in LFP activity, whereas increases in overall firing rates are attributable to locomotion. In contrast, enhancement of visual encoding during locomotion is associated with increased arousal, rather than motor activity.
RESULTS
Dissociating locomotion and arousal
To separate the contributions of locomotion and arousal to V1 activity and visual encoding, we performed simultaneous recordings of isolated single units and LFPs from multiple sites throughout layers 2-6 of V1 in awake mice (Fig. 1; n=88 sessions in 28 mice). Mice were head-fixed on a spring-mounted wheel apparatus (Fig. S1a) and recordings were made during both baseline and visual stimulation periods. Behavioral state was assessed by continuous monitoring of arousal and locomotion. To monitor arousal, we measured pupil dilation in the eye that was contralateral to the visual stimulus (Fig. 1a, Supplementary Movie 1). Transition points were defined as shifts from quiescence to locomotion or from locomotion to quiescence and were identified by detecting significant changes in the statistics of the locomotion speed signal from the wheel with high temporal resolution (Fig. 1b).
Experimental paradigm to separate the contributions of arousal and locomotion to neural activity and visual encoding in V1. (a) Example data from one experiment session. Video frame images of the mouse’s eye (1-6) are shown where acquired at the times indicated in the pupil recording trace. Pupil diameter was recorded on video and extracted posthoc via a fitted ellipse (cyan). The average pupil diameter in pixel units is shown as a function of time. Locomotion is shown as a linearized version of the wheel position. Locomotion onset point is shown in the inset panel. The locomotion period is indicated by green shading. LFP recording is shown as a raw broadband LFP signal for a superficial electrode, together with the 1-4 Hz filtered signals. Thresholded multi-unit traces and spike densities (1 s Gaussian smoothing kernel with SD [Standard Deviation] of 0.25 s) are shown for a superficial (MU1) and deep (MU2) electrode, respectively, together with a single unit trace that was isolated from MU1. Grey shadings indicate visual stimuli at 100% contrast and varying orientations. (b) Locomotion speed around locomotion onset (green) and offset (red), shown as mean ± s.e.m (across sessions). (c) Population average pupil diameter, normalized to PD at 20-25 s point after locomotion offset, as a function of time around locomotion onset (green) and offset (red), shown as mean ± s.e.m.
We found complex temporal relationships between pupil diameter, locomotion, LFP dynamics, and firing rates of V1 neurons (Fig. 1a). At locomotion onset, speed rapidly increased, reaching a plateau after about 2.5 s and remaining at ∼10-15 cm/s until just prior to locomotion offset (Fig. 1b). Locomotion speed and pupil diameter were consistently correlated at transition points. Pupil diameter increased prior to the onset of locomotion, suggesting that arousal reliably preceded movement. Pupil diameter reached a plateau after about 2.5 s, and remained elevated until locomotion offset (Fig. 1c). Locomotion offset was followed by a gradual decrease in pupil diameter that did not reach baseline values until 40 s later (Fig. 1c), indicating a substantial period of elevated arousal in the absence of locomotion. Periods of high arousal and locomotion thus occurred both together and separately, allowing us to discriminate their roles in V1 activity.
LFP modulation by behavioral state transition
To dissociate LFP changes associated with locomotion and arousal, we computed time-frequency representations of LFP signals around locomotion onset and offset in the absence of visual stimuli. Locomotion onset was preceded by a sharp increase in LFP gamma oscillations (55-65 Hz) and a decrease in low-frequency LFP fluctuations (1-4 Hz; Fig. 2a). Gamma power decreased gradually over time after locomotion onset, whereas low-frequency fluctuations remained suppressed throughout the locomotion period (Fig. 2a). In contrast, locomotion offset was followed by a gradual increase in low-frequency LFP power and a gradual decrease in LFP gamma power that lasted up to 40-50 s (Fig. 2b). The time courses of LFP low-frequency and gamma power were strongly correlated (Spearman correlation) with the time course of pupil dynamics, but not with the time course of locomotion speed (Fig. 2c). The correlations between LFP power and pupil diameter time courses were strongly linear (gamma and pupil: Pearson’s R = 0.94, p=2.4×10−6; 1-4 Hz and pupil: Pearson’s R = −0.93, p=4.1×10−5), suggesting that arousal, rather than locomotion, mediates most of the observed change in LFP patterns.
Contributions of arousal and locomotion to V1 LFP rhythms. (a) Left: Time-frequency representation of LFP signals around locomotion onset (L-on), showing base-10 log-transformed relative power, i.e. power at time t divided by average power in the [−4.5,−4] s quiescent interval preceding L-on. Power was computed in sliding windows of ±2 s using short-term Fourier Transform. Log power ratios are shown as percentages. Right: Line plots of gamma (55-65 Hz) and low-frequency (1-4 Hz) power across n=58 sites in 11 mice, shown as mean ± s.e.m. Dashed lines at 2 s before zero represent the effective time at which data after L-on would be included in computation, revealing spectral changes before this point. Note the appearance of an 8-10 Hz theta-band that interrupts the reduction in low-frequency LFP fluctuations, presumably due to volume conduction from the hippocampus. (b) Time-frequency representation of V1 LFP signals around locomotion offset (L-off). Relative power was computed by dividing by the average power in the [−3,-2.5] s epoch before locomotion offset, for n=133 sites in 18 mice. (c) Spearman’s correlation (rho2) between either average pupil diameter (PD) (open bars) or locomotion speed (closed bars) with average gamma and delta time courses for quiescence period, using data >−3 s before locomotion offset till 40 s after locomotion offset.***: p<0.001. Shown mean ± s.e.m. n=133 sites in 18 mice. (d) Average power spectra during visual stimulation period (left) and ITI (right), separately for locomotion and quiescent periods, for n = 196 sites in 23 mice. Power spectra were computed using multitapering of 500 ms windows, with a smoothing window of ±4 Hz.
We also recorded LFP activity in V1 while presenting visual stimuli. All stimuli were drifting gratings on a mean luminance background (see Online Methods). We found that the increase in LFP gamma-band power and decrease in low-frequency power during locomotion periods were observed for both visual stimulation and inter-trial interval (ITI) epochs (Fig. 2d). Increased gamma oscillations during locomotion are thus largely the result of behavioral state transition, rather than visual stimulation.
Cell-type specific modulation of firing rates
Next, we investigated the contributions of locomotion and arousal to V1 firing rates in the absence of visual stimuli. We compared firing rates between epochs of locomotion and quiescence and within each type of epoch to examine the temporal dynamics of changes in firing around transition points (Fig. 3a). Individual V1 neurons demonstrated a broad range of relationships between spontaneous firing rate and behavioral state (Fig 3a, Fig. S2). To characterize firing patterns across the population, we classified well-isolated single units into 34 fast spiking (FS), putative inhibitory, and 157 regular spiking (RS), putative excitatory cells based on action potential waveform characteristics (Fig. S3).
Contributions of arousal and locomotion to spontaneous firing activity in V1. (a) Schematic showing division of data into epochs for analysis. LE: early locomotion period (-1 to 1 s around L-on). QE: early quiescent period (1 to 5 s after L-off). QM: middle quiescent period (5 to 20 s after L-off). QL: late quiescent period (>40 s after L-off). Plots show average wheel speed and firing rate for an example FS cell during locomotion onset (L-on, left) and locomotion offset (L-off, right). Rasters show individual transition points for the same cell. Gray shadings indicate L-off and L-on during individual trials. (b) Upper: Population average change in RS (blue) and FS (orange) cells’ firing rates relative to a quiescent baseline period (taken −6 to −3 s before L-on) as a function of time around L-on, shown as mean ± s.e.m. Lower: Change in firing rates relative to a baseline locomotion period (defined −3 to −1 seconds before L-off) as a function of time around L-off. (c) Upper: Overview of all recorded RS (left) and FS (right) cells, sorted by firing rate difference between quiescent baseline and the [-1,1] s interval around L-on. Color coding corresponds to Z-scored firing rates. Lower: As in the upper panel, but as a function of time around L-off and sorted on Spearman’s correlation between time after L-off (>3 s) and quiescence. (d-e) Mean ± s.e.m. of firing rate differences (d) and firing rate modulation (e) between LE and LL epochs. Statistical testing using two-sided Rank-Wilcoxon tests. #,*,**,*** correspond to p<0.1, 0.05, 0.01, 0.001. RS: n=106/17, FS: n=18/9. (f-g) As in (d-e), but now for QE vs. QL and QM vs. QL. RS: 101/18, 101/18 (#cells/#mice), FS: 21/10, 18/9. Statistical comparison of QM vs. QL rate modulation: RS, FS: p=7.1×10−4, 8.6×10−4.
Both FS and RS cells’ firing rates increased before locomotion onset, peaked around the time of locomotion onset, and declined over time throughout the locomotion period (Fig. 3b-c). To quantify the anticipatory effect for individual cells, we computed the Pearson cross-correlation coefficient between locomotion speed and instantaneous firing rate in the interval around locomotion onset. Most cells exhibited a strong linear correlation between locomotion speed and firing rate that was forward-shifted in time, indicating that increases in RS and FS firing rates preceded increases in locomotion speed (Fig. S4).
Both FS and RS firing rates were significantly higher during the early period around locomotion onset (within ±1 s of locomotion onset; LE) than during the late locomotion period (>10 s after locomotion onset; LL) (Fig. 3d, Fig. S5a). To directly compare state-dependent changes between RS and FS cells, we computed a rate modulation value (FRb – FRa) / (FRa + FRb), which normalizes for absolute rate differences. RS and FS cells exhibited similar degrees of rate modulation during the early vs. the late locomotion period (p=0.48; Fig. 3e).
We next examined whether increases in firing rate during locomotion might be explained by the associated change in arousal. To separate the effects of locomotion and arousal, we examined the trajectories of firing rates during quiescence following locomotion offset, when pupil diameter showed substantial variation in the absence of movement (Fig. 1b-c). For both RS and FS cells, locomotion offset was accompanied by a rapid decrease in firing rates, followed by a subsequent gradual increase over time (Fig. 3b).
To directly compare firing rates across epochs within the quiescent period, we analyzed early quiescence (1-5 s after locomotion offset; QE), middle quiescence (5-20 s after locomotion offset; QM) and late quiescence (>40 s after locomotion offset; QL) (Fig. 3a). Pupil diameter was significantly decreased across the QE to QM to QL intervals after locomotion offset (Fig. S6). In contrast, FS and RS firing rates significantly increased across the QE to QM to QL intervals after locomotion offset (Fig. 3f-g). We found no significant difference in rate modulation between FS and RS cells across quiescent epochs (Fig 3f-g, Fig. S5a).
To further quantify the trajectory of V1 firing rates over time, we computed Spearman correlation coefficients between firing rate and time after locomotion offset (using only data >3 s after locomotion offset; Online Methods). Many cells showed a significant association between firing rate and time (FS: 10/23, RS: 37/80, at p<0.05), and the average correlation was higher than zero (Fig. S5b, mean ± s.e.m. of Spearman’s rho = 0.04±0.024, p=0.034; Rank-Wilcoxon test). However, we also found subpopulations of cells whose firing rate either decreased or increased over time (Fig. 3c, Fig. S5b, FS positive: 6/23, negative: 4/23; RS positive: 20/80, negative: 17/80, p<0.05), suggesting diverse firing rate trajectories across the V1 population.
Together, these results suggest that both RS and FS cells in V1 demonstrate increased spontaneous activity in anticipation of and during locomotion. However, a state of heightened arousal, as indicated by maintained pupil dilation after the cessation of movement, is accompanied by decreased RS and FS firing. In contrast to the spectral changes in LFP signals, elevated firing during movement is thus specific to locomotion periods and unlikely to result from associated changes in global arousal.
Modulation of visual encoding by state transition
To determine the impact of locomotion and arousal on visual encoding, we recorded LFP and unit activity in V1 while presenting visual stimuli. Visual stimulation during locomotion caused an increase in gamma band power at frequencies above 60 Hz, with a spectral peak around 75 Hz (Fig. S7a-b). During quiescent periods, visual stimulation instead caused an increase in gamma-band power at a broader range above 20 Hz, with a relatively shallow spectral peak around 30 Hz (Fig. S7a-b). We further found that contrast tuning of LFP gamma-band power was affected by locomotion, with increased and decreased gain at high and low gamma frequencies, respectively (Fig. S7c-f).
The robust differences in visually evoked LFP dynamics between periods of locomotion and quiescence suggest that visual stimulation may likewise differentially affect firing rates during these states. We therefore computed the mean peak firing rate of RS cells in response to stimuli presented during locomotion and quiescence (Fig 4a-b). To separate the effects of locomotion and arousal, we divided the quiescence period into separate epochs as in Figure 3. The exact durations of the epochs were adjusted to account for the duration and timing of stimulus presentation. We analyzed early quiescence (3-20 s after locomotion offset; QE), when the pupil was relatively dilated, and late quiescence (>40 s after locomotion offset; QL), when the pupil was relatively constricted. We used the signal-to-noise (SNR) ratio, defined as (FRstim - FRITI) / (FRstim + FRITI), to assess visual encoding by V1 neurons during each epoch (Online Methods). We found an overall increase in the SNR of visual encoding during locomotion in comparison to quiescence for RS, but not FS, cells (Fig. 4c). A finer analysis of time periods found that the SNR in early quiescence (QE), during high arousal, was larger than in late quiescence (QL), during low arousal, for both RS and FS cells (Fig. 4c). We found that spontaneous, but not visually evoked, RS firing rates were significantly higher in the late than in the early quiescence period (Fig. S8a-b). The observed increase in SNR during high arousal states thus largely resulted from the differential effects of arousal on spontaneous and evoked firing rates.
Contributions of locomotion and arousal to visual encoding and noise correlations in V1. (a,b) Raster plots of the visual responses of two example RS cells with associated firing rate density (computed using ±0.025 s Gaussian kernels with SD of 0.0125 s) for all locomotion (L), all quiescence (Q), early quiescence (QE; 3 to 20 s after locomotion offset) and late quiescence (QL; >40 s after locomotion offset). Gray shading and sinusoid indicate visual stimulation. (c) Mean ± s.e.m. of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR), defined as (FRstim-FRITI) / (FRstim+FRITI), for each period. #,*,**,*** correspond to p<0.1, 0.05, 0.01, 0.001, two-sided Rank-Wilcoxon test. RS: n=88/20, 101/20 (#cells/#mice). FS: n=23/10, 24/11. (d) Mean ± s.e.m of noise correlation of firing rates during different behavioral periods. n=155/9, 289/11 (#pairs/#mice). *,**,***: p<0.05, p<0.01, p<0.001, two-sided Rank-Wilcoxon test. (e) Left: Noise correlation for locomotion vs quiescence. Circles correspond to cell pairs. FS-FS: n=9/3, RS-RS: 90/7, FS-RS (black): n=56/5. Right: as left, but for QE vs. QL. FS-FS: 10/4, RS-RS: 206/7, FS-RS: n=73/7.
The SNR is a measure of the encoding of visual stimuli by individual neurons. However, visual stimuli are likely to be encoded by patterns of V1 population activity34, 35. We therefore calculated noise correlations, which may determine the efficiency of population coding36, 37, for each epoch (Online Methods). Overall, noise correlations were significantly lower during periods of locomotion than during quiescence (Fig. 4d-e). Because low-frequency and gamma-band LFP power showed gradual changes over time after locomotion offset, we hypothesized that noise correlations would increase with time after locomotion offset. Indeed, noise correlations were significantly elevated during late quiescent periods, when arousal was low, as compared to early quiescent periods (Fig. 4d-e).
These results suggest that visual encoding is enhanced during locomotion as a result of increased SNR and decorrelation of visually evoked activity. However, these changes are mainly attributable to arousal, rather than locomotion, as comparable changes in SNR and noise correlations were observed during periods of heightened arousal in the absence of movement.
Isolating the cortical effects of arousal
The analyses of naturally occurring state transitions described above suggest separable effects of locomotion and arousal on firing rates, LFP activity, and visual encoding in V1. However, it is possible that the period of heightened arousal following locomotion offset represents an unusual form of arousal or is affected by long-lasting modulation of V1 activity by motor signals. To more causally test the role of arousal in regulating neural activity, we induced a state of enhanced arousal by puffing air on the back of the mouse (Fig. S1b, Online Methods). In a subset of sessions (n=20 sessions in 10 mice), we administered brief air puffs to the mouse while monitoring pupil diameter and performing simultaneous recordings of V1 LFPs and isolated single cells (Fig. 5; n = 61 cells in 10 mice).
Causal induction of arousal without locomotion. (a) Example data from one experiment session. Video images captured of eye are shown for several time points (1-7). Average pupil diameter trace in pixels is shown as a function of time around air puff onset. Locomotion is shown as a linearized version of the wheel position. LFP recording is shown for one electrode, together with the 1-4 Hz filtered signal. A multiunit trace (MU) and associated spike density plot are shown for the same electrode. Single-unit traces (SU1-6) were isolated from a total of three electrodes in L5/6 during this recording session. Shaded yellow area indicates period during which pupil was dilated, for visualization purposes. (b) Average locomotion speed as a function of time around the air puff. (c) Average pupil diameter, normalized to pre-air-puff interval (-10 to 1 s before air puff), as a function of time. The s.e.m.’s were computed across sessions (n=20 in 10 mice).
Delivery of the air puff reliably induced both arousal, as measured by pupil dilation, and changes in unit activity in the absence of locomotion (Fig. 5a, Supplementary Movie 2). Across sessions (n=20 sessions in 10 mice), air puffs caused an average 1.6-fold increase in pupil diameter in the absence of locomotion (Fig. 5b-c). This increase was similar in magnitude to the average increase in pupil diameter observed during locomotion (Fig. 1c), suggesting a comparable level of arousal.
LFP modulation by induced arousal
Our analysis of arousal periods following locomotion offset (Fig. 2) predicts that a shift to an aroused state in the absence of locomotion should be accompanied by a decrease in low-frequency LFP power and an increase in gamma-band power. Indeed, we found that after the air puff the LFP showed a long-lasting, 2.5 fold decrease in low-frequency fluctuations comparable in magnitude to the locomotion effect (Fig. 6a-b). The induced arousal also caused a 1.25-fold increase in gamma-band power. The pupil diameter time course showed a strong positive correlation with gamma-band power, and a negative correlation with low-frequency power (Fig. 6c). These findings indicate that a shift to a state of heightened arousal without locomotion causes a decrease in low-frequency fluctuations and an increase in gamma-band fluctuations in V1.
Arousal causes a frequency shift in V1 LFP activity. (a) Time-frequency representations of LFP signals around the air puff. Base-10 log-transformed relative power, i.e. power at time t divided by average power in the [-6,-1] s interval preceding the air puff. n = 45 sites/10 mice. (b) Line plots of gamma and delta power, with shading representing s.e.m. across sites, around time of air puff. (c) Correlation of pupil diameter time course with LFP time course at various frequencies. Gray horizontal bars indicate significance at p<0.05, linear regression analysis.
Rate modulation by induced arousal
We next investigated how firing rates were affected by the causal manipulation of arousal levels. Overall, we found that the air puff caused a significant, long-lasting decrease in spontaneous firing rates (Fig. 7a,b). However, a small subpopulation of RS and FS cells showed elevated firing rates after the air puff (Fig. 7a,c). Air puffs were particularly suppressive for RS cells if they had a high propensity to engage in irregular, burst firing (Fig. 7d; Online Methods), but a similar relationship was not observed for FS cells (Fig. 7d).
Arousal suppresses firing rates in V1. (a) Raster plots and spike density traces (1 s windows with Gaussian kernel and SD of 250 ms) from two example (left: FS, right: RS) cells are shown as a function of time. (b) Average modulation of firing rate induced by the air puff for RS (blue) and FS (orange) cells. Mean ± s.e.m of modulation in [1,4] s interval for RS = −0.19±0.046, p=4.6×10−4, n=51/9 (#cells/#mice). FS: −0.14±0.074, p=0.084, n=10/6. Difference FS and RS cells not significant (Rank-Wilcoxon test; p=0.87). (c) Rate modulation scores for all FS (upper) and RS (lower) cells as a function of time, relative to 6 s prior to air puff. (d) Air puff modulation index, comparing [1,4] s post air puff to 6 s prior to air puff, as a function of local variation coefficient, a measure of firing regularity. High local variation values correspond to bursty cells and a local variation coefficient of 1 indicates Poisson-like firing (Online Methods). RS cells: R = −0.57, p=1×10−5, n=51/9 (#cells/#mice), FS cells: R=0.13, p=0.71, n=10/6. (e) Values on the x-axis indicate air puff modulation index. Values on the y-axis indicate airpuff-arousal modulation, calculated as the Spearman correlation of time after locomotion (>3 s) and firing rate. R = −0.47, p = 4.15×10−4, NS: R = −0.32, p=0.36, difference RS and FS n.s., Fisher Z-test. (f) Air puff modulation index vs. correlation of pupil diameter and firing rate across trials. Correlation of pupil and firing rate was defined over trials using the [1,4] s interval after the air puff. Pearson’s R = 0.52, p=3.3×10−4, n=45/7.
Individual neurons should show similar effects in response to naturally occurring and induced arousal. We therefore predicted that cells whose firing rates were suppressed by the induced arousal would likewise show suppression after locomotion offset, followed by gradual increases in firing rate over time as arousal levels decreased (Fig. 3). For this analysis, we selected only locomotion epochs that did not occur within 50 s of an air puff. We first defined a locomotion-arousal modulation as the correlation between time after locomotion offset and firing, with positive values indicating that a cell was suppressed by arousal. We also defined the airpuff-arousal modulation in the interval after the air puff. We found that the locomotion-arousal and airpuff-arousal modulations were significantly negatively correlated, demonstrating consistent effects of arousal on firing rate (Fig. 7e).
The coordinated effects of induced arousal on firing rate and pupil diameter also predict that fluctuations in arousal should correlate on a trial-by-trial basis with firing rates. We predicted (more) negative correlations between pupil diameter and firing rates across trials for cells whose firing was on average suppressed by the air puff. Likewise, we predicted (more) positive correlations for cells whose firing was enhanced by the air puff. These predictions were supported by trial-by-trial correlation analysis (Fig. 7f). Together, these results suggest that global arousal is associated with decreased spontaneous firing rates.
Arousal and visual encoding
To isolate the role of arousal in modulating single cell and population visual encoding, we administered air puffs randomly in combination with presentation of visual stimuli in a subset of experiments (Online Methods). We then compared visual responses in the 10 s before the air puff with those in the 10 s after the air puff. Across the population of cells, we found a significant increase in SNR following the induced arousal (Fig. 8a,b). We also observed that induced arousal significantly decreased noise correlations (Fig. 8a,c). These results indicate that arousal alone replicates the effects of locomotion on visual encoding, leading to an increase in the salience of visual responses by individual cells and decorrelating activity across the population.
Arousal enhances visual encoding and decreases noise correlations in V1. (a) Raster plots of the visual responses of an example cell with associated firing rate density (computed using ±0.025 s Gaussian kernels with SD of 0.0125) before (left) and after (right) air puff (AP). Gray shading and sinusoid indicate visual stimulation. (b) Plot of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) for 10 s interval before air puff (y-axis) compared to 10 s interval after air puff (x-axis). Population SNR shown at right, as mean ± s.e.m (n=25/8, #cells/#pairs). (c) Noise correlation in the 10 s after air puff compared to the 10 s before air puff. Circles correspond to cell pairs (red = FS, blue = RS, black = FS/RS). Population average noise correlation is shown at right. (b-c) *, **: p<0.05, p<0.01, n=22/4 (#pairs/#mice), two-sided Rank-Wilcoxon test.
Discussion
We examined the distinct contributions of arousal and locomotion to V1 activity and visual encoding using extracellular recording in combination with behavioral state monitoring and manipulation. Our data revealed that much of the change in V1 LFP and unit activity associated with locomotion is the result of increased arousal. We found that heightened arousal in the absence of locomotion has several cortical impacts: 1) a change in the temporal patterning of neural activity, comprising a reduction in low-frequency oscillations and an increase in gamma synchronization, 2) a net suppression of firing rates, and 3) a change in visual encoding at both the single-cell and population levels. In contrast, locomotion specifically contributes to an increase in RS and FS cell firing rates in anticipation of and during movement. Arousal state and motor activity thus have distinct roles in regulating activity in primary visual cortical circuits.
We found substantial differences in V1 activity between periods of quiescence and locomotion. This is consistent with several studies that used electrophysiology or calcium imaging in awake head-fixed rodents15–20, 22, 28. However, previous results suggested that elevated RS cell firing during locomotion periods was not associated with locomotion, but was rather a modulation of visual responses15, 19. By focusing on state transition points, we were able to show that RS firing was strongly increased in anticipation of locomotion and during movement, even in the absence of visual stimulation. In contrast, causal induction of a state of high arousal caused a suppression of firing rates, indicating that motor and arousal signals may have opposing effects on spontaneous spiking activity in V1. Studies in auditory and barrel cortex have found that firing rates of both RS and FS cells are suppressed by locomotion and whisking, respectively12, 14, 21, 38. Thus, increased RS and FS firing during locomotion is likely specific to visual cortex and may be related to the necessity for integration of motor and visual signals in order to faithfully represent the outside world with respect to the animal’s position17, 18.
Firing rates and LFP power in the low (1-4Hz) and gamma (55-65Hz) ranges showed changes well in advance of the onset of locomotion, as did pupil diameter. These anticipatory changes indicate that increased arousal levels and motor-related activity in V1 reliably precede the execution of motor output. Increased firing in anticipation of locomotion, consistent with motor planning or predictive coding17, 18, could rely on top-down inputs from fronto-parietal circuits, in which many cells fire in anticipation of saccades and other movements39, 40. Indeed, recent work has highlighted the existence of long-range inputs from frontal to striate cortex41 and motor cortical areas may also project to V1 or other visual areas42, 43.
Distinct V1 cell populations exhibited different modulation around transition points. We observed enhanced signal-to-noise during locomotion for visual responses only in RS, putative excitatory cells but not in FS, putative inhibitory cells, potentially indicating cell-type specific regulation of visual encoding. Recent work has highlighted distinct patterns of state-dependent recruitment of different interneuron classes14, 16, 20, 22, 38. Notably, we found diversity within both the RS and FS cell populations in the trajectory of state-dependent changes in firing patterns at behavioral state transition points. Whereas most RS and FS cells were suppressed by arousal, a subset of both cell types instead showed enhanced firing. Firing rate suppression with arousal was particularly evident in bursting RS cells.
We took advantage of direct manipulations of behavioral state to probe the dynamic range of behavioral state-dependent cortical activity. Causal induction of arousal with the air puff stimulus initiated a shift from low to high frequencies in the cortical LFP. We found that induced arousal replicated the 2.5 fold reduction in 1-4 Hz fluctuations observed during locomotion periods. Low frequency fluctuations increased only gradually after locomotion offset, with a strong linear relationship to the pupil diameter. Arousal alone therefore appears to account for the reduction in 1-4 Hz LFP power during locomotion periods. The observed linear relationship between pupil diameter and 1-4 Hz LFP power differs from recent work in which pupil diameter did not correlate with low-frequency membrane potential power, likely because only very small and short-lasting pupil fluctuations were considered22. LFP and firing rate changes were dissociated from one another during arousal periods, as the gradual increase in LFP low-frequency power and decrease in LFP gamma-band power were accompanied by a net increase in firing rates over the same time period.
The effect of arousal on gamma-band LFP power in the absence of locomotion (∼20% increase) was smaller than the increase in gamma-band LFP power during locomotion periods (∼40% increase). Gamma-band power showed a similar adaptation during the locomotion period as firing rates, in contrast to 1-4 Hz power. A change in arousal might therefore not exclusively account for the increase in gamma-band power during locomotion periods, suggesting that locomotion might further amplify gamma-band oscillations through the increased drive of local RS and FS cells44–47. Previous work has linked noise correlations to intrinsic low-frequency fluctuations and a decrease in gamma-band oscillations25, 27, 48, 49. The effects of arousal observed here thus overlap with those of focused spatial attention in primates, where attention is associated with increased temporal patterning in the gamma band and decreased noise correlations23, 25–27.
State-dependent changes in visual encoding were highly correlated with arousal level across a wide range, suggesting extensive flexibility in the sensory processing operations of cortical circuits. An increase in the SNR of visual responses in V1 has previously been reported for waking vs. sleeping states2, 7, indicating possible regulation of visual sensitivity by overall arousal levels. In agreement with this idea, we found that states of high arousal were associated with increased SNR and decreased noise correlations, indicating enhanced encoding of visual stimuli at both the single-cell and population levels. We found a strong trial-by-trial correlation between arousal, as measured by pupil diameter, and both measures of visual encoding, suggesting a dynamic system in which changes in arousal fine-tune the gain of visual responses in V1 on a moment-to-moment basis.
Lesion and stimulation studies have supported causal roles for several major neuromodulatory systems in controlling sleep-wake transitions and the temporal pattern of cortical activity, including norepinephrine (NE) and acetylcholine (ACh)28, 50–55. Like arousal, neuromodulatory action desynchronizes the cortex, promotes gamma oscillations, and changes sensory encoding4, 20, 25, 51, 53, 54. Noradrenergic blockade of awake mouse V1 eliminates the depolarization and elevated firing associated with locomotion16. Recent evidence also points to the involvement of the mesencephalic locomotor region, a cholinergic brainstem nucleus, in the control of locomotion and regulation of V1 activity patterns and visual responses28. In addition, cholinergic afferents from the basal forebrain nucleus of the diagonal band of Broca selectively target V1 interneurons involved in locomotion-related changes in firing rates20. Arousal effects observed in V1 during waking state transitions are thus likely involve complex interactions between multiple neuromodulatory systems.
In summary, our data show that activity in mouse visual cortex during wakefulness is differentially regulated by arousal and motor signals. We find a complex interaction between internally generated cortical states and visual inputs. Arousal restructures spontaneous cortical activity and promotes fast gamma-band oscillations, which may be optimal for synchronized, bottom-up routing of sensory signals23, 48, 56, 57 This shift in the mode of cortical operation also strongly increases the signal-to-noise ratio of visual representations. The interplay between arousal level, motor activity, and sensory input may contribute to the functional flexibility of cortical circuits.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by a NARSAD Young Investigator award, an Alfred P. Sloan Fellowship award, a Whitehall grant, a Klingenstein fellowship award, a McKnight Scholar award, and NIH/NEI grants R00 EY018407 and R01 EY022951 to JAC, a Rubicon Grant (Netherlands Organization for Science) to MV, a Jane Coffin Childs Fund fellowship award to RBB, and a NARSAD Young Investigator award to UK. We thank James Mossner and Hyun Lee for technical support. We also thank the main developers of M-Clust (D. Redish), KlustaKwik (K. Harris) and FieldTrip (R. Oostenveld) for free use of their software. We thank M.J. Higley for comments on the manuscript.
Author contributions
MV, RBB, UK and JAC designed research. RBB, MV and JAC conducted experiments. UK and JAC developed hardware. MV analyzed data. MV and JAC wrote the manuscript. MV, RBB, and UK contributed equally to this study.