Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

On the relationship between psychopathy and general intelligence: A meta-analytic review

Olga Sanchez de Ribera, Nicholas Kavish, Brian Boutwell
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/100693
Olga Sanchez de Ribera
aInstitute of Criminology, University of Cambridge, Sidgwick Avenue, Cambridge, CB3 9DA United Kingdom email:
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: olgasrdc@gmail.com
Nicholas Kavish
bDepartment of Psychology and Philosophy, Sam Houston State University, 1901 Avenue I, Suite 390, Huntsville, TX 77340, USA email:
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: nak012@shsu.edu nak012@shsu.edu
Brian Boutwell
cDepartment of Criminology and Criminal Justice and Department of Epidemiology, College for Public Health and Social Justice, Saint Louis University, St. Louis, MO 63103, USA email:
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: boutwellb@slu.edu
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

Over recent decades, a growing body of research has accumulated concerning the relationship between indicators of general intelligence and the personality construct known as psychopathy. Both traits represent key correlates of life outcomes, predicting everything from occupational and economic success, to various indicators of prosocial behavior (including avoiding contact with the criminal justice system). The findings to date regarding the association of the two traits, however, have been somewhat inconsistent. Thus, there remains a need for a more systematic investigation of the extant empirical literature. The current study reports a meta-analysis conducted to evaluate the direction and overall effect size of the relationship between these two constructs. Our analyses revealed a small, but significant, negative effect of intelligence on psychopathy. The results and impact of possible moderating variables such as type of intelligence test used are discussed. Finally, the study limitations, and possible directions for further research on this issue are detailed prior to concluding.

Introduction

Psychopathy and intelligence represent two psychological constructs that have been studied extensively over the last several decades. Large bodies of psychometric work have consistently supported the reliability and validity of both concepts (Carrol, 1993; Hare et al., 1990; Kranzler & Jensen, 1991; Salekin, Rogers, & Sewell, 1996). General intelligence is one of the most studied traits in all of psychology and has nearly a century of research related to its measurement, development, and etiological underpinnings (Gottfredson, 2002; Ritchie, 2015). Psychopathy, while representing a more recently defined psychological construct (Cleckley, 1941), is also psychometrically robust, and research continues to shed light on its etiology and development across the life course.

Of particular interest to the current study, however, is a more recent line of research examining the association of general intelligence with indicators of psychopathy. The last decade or so has seen a sharp increase in studies examining the effect of general intelligence on psychopathy, with some evidence suggesting that lower IQ scores are associated with increased psychopathic tendencies (DeLisi, Vaughn, Beaver, & Wright, 2010; Vitacco, Neumann, & Wodeshuk, 2008). To date, however, the results gleaned from this growing body of research have been somewhat mixed (with some evidence of an inverse relationship between the two variables, and other failing to find such an effect). Thus, the goal of the current study is to systematically review the literature in order to better understand the pattern of findings emerging in the literature to date. To the extent that psychopathy covaries with intelligence (regardless of the direction of the effect), it may provide insight into the development of both outcomes. Prior to progressing to the results, we discuss both constructs in more detail.

Psychopathy

Unlike most clinical disorders which are characterized by a set of symptoms, psychopathy is commonly described as a cluster of relatively stable personality traits (Cleckley, 1941; Hare & Vertommen, 1991). The traits most often associated with psychopathy are callousness, remorselessness, lack of empathy, grandiosity, impulsivity, deceitfulness, and manipulativeness (Blair, 2007; Cleckley, 1941; Hare & Vertommen, 1991). Additionally, the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised Edition (PCL-R; Hare & Vertommen, 1991), generally viewed as the best diagnostic tool for measuring psychopathy, includes the previously mentioned traits plus superficial charm, pathological lying, failure to accept responsibility, need for stimulation, parasitic lifestyle, early behavior problems, lack of long term planning or goals, and promiscuous sexual behavior (Hare & Vertommen, 1991; Cooke & Michie, 2001).

While some debate remains, the general consensus among scholars is that psychopathy represents a confluence of traits that predict a host of antisocial outcomes (Cooke & Michie, 2001; Hare, 1996; Patrick et al., 2006). Because some of the studies being analyzed also look at Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD) it is noted that while psychopathy and ASPD do correlate, they are not fungible. ASPD is a clinical disorder with specific diagnostic criteria, whereas psychopathy is a non-clinical construct generally measured on a continuum.

Intelligence

General intelligence, commonly referred to as g, is arguably the best measured trait in all of psychology and research from across a variety of disciplines has repeatedly found that it is immensely important in most areas of life (Gottfredson, 2002; Ritchie, 2015). Researchers have been studying and refining the concept of g since Spearman (1904) first proposed it in the beginning of the 20th century as a way to conceptualize overall mental ability rather than a specific ability (e.g. verbal ability) (Gottfredson, 1997; 2002).

Intelligence is a (relatively) stable trait (Plomin & Deary, 2015). Yet, some mental abilities do change over time. Research into the stability of intelligence often divides the construct into two categories: fluid intelligence and crystallized intelligence. Fluid intelligence, which broadly contains abilities such as memory for novel and abstract information as well as processing speed, has been found to decline with age (Salthouse, 2004; Schaie, 2005). Crystallized intelligence, on the other hand, consists of abilities such as vocabulary and general knowledge and shows significantly less decline with age, even persisting into the early stages of dementia (McGurn et al.,2004). Nevertheless, differences in cognitive ability become stable across childhood (Gottfried, Gottfried, & Guerin, 2009) and remain stable throughout most of adulthood (Deary et al., 2000). In fact, Deary and colleagues (2000) found that scores on one measure of intelligence at age 11 correlated at about 0.73 with scores on the same test at age 77.

Similar to psychopathy, intelligence has consistently been linked to important life outcomes. IQ predicts socioeconomic status (Kanazawa, 2006; Strenze, 2007), educational achievement (Deary, Strand, Smith, & Fernandes, 2007; Gottfredson, 1997; Lynn & Mikk, 2009; Strenze, 2007), occupational status and job success (Gottfredson, 2002; Strenze, 2007), mating success (Greengross & Miller, 2011), physical and mental health (Batty, Der, Macintyre, & Deary, 2006; Deary, Weiss, & Batty, 2010; Der, Batty, & Deary, 2009; Gottfredson & Deary, 2004) and longevity (Beaver et al., 2016; Deary, Weiss, & Batty, 2010; Gottfredson & Deary, 2004). In other words, having a high IQ has been found to be a predictor of completing more years of education (Deary, Strand, Smith, & Fernandes, 2007; Gottfredson, 1997; Lynn & Mikk, 2009; Strenze, 2007),gaining a higher status career (Gottfredson, 2002; Strenze, 2007) and living longer (Deary, Weiss, & Batty, 2010; Gottfredson & Deary, 2004). At the macro level, estimates of the mean IQs of a state (Kanazawa, 2006) or country (Lynn & Vanhanen, 2002) predict differences in per capita GSP and GDP, respectively.

With regards to health and longevity, higher levels of g have been found to correlate with lower levels of a wide variety of physical and mental health problems as well as levels of intentional injury. For instance, Hart and colleagues (2004) found that an IQ score 1 SD below the mean was linked to an 11% increase in the risk of hospitalization or death. Additionally, Gale, Batty, Tynelius, Deary, and Rasmussen (2010) found in a longitudinal study of more than 1 million men that lower intelligence was associated with an increased risk for all psychiatric disorders, increased rates of psychiatric hospital admissions, and greater rates of comorbid disorders. Higher levels of intelligence on the other hand, have been found to correlate with better health and longevity, with a 1 SD advantage on IQ being linked to about a 50% decrease in risk for homicide victimization (Batty, Deary, Tengstrom, & Rasmussen, 2008).

Intelligence and Psychopathy

Intelligence and personality researchers have often explored the possibility that IQ might be related to personality traits (Furnham, Moutafi, & Chamorro-Premuzic, 2005; Higgins, Peterson, Pihl, & Lee, 2007). The results of such research have been mixed and what correlations have been found are often very small. Higgins and colleagues (2007) found only a small positive correlation between Openness and IQ (r=.28), while another study found a small negative relationship between intelligence and conscientiousness (r=-.21) (Furnham, Moutafi, & Chamorro-Premuzic, 2005). Other researchers have found a positive relationship between intelligence and extroversion (r=.08) and openness (r=.33; Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997).

Despite a rather ambiguous relationship between intelligence and other broad personality traits, researchers have uncovered a somewhat consistent phenotypic link between intelligence and psychopathic personality styles. One of the largest behavioral overlaps between psychopathy and low intelligence is the increased propensity to violent and criminal involvement. Numerous studies and reviews have found a robust inverse relationship between intelligence and delinquency in adolescents and juveniles (Hernstein & C. Murray, 1994; Hirschi & Hindelang, 1977; Wilson & Hernstein, 1985). As might be expected based on the stability of intelligence over time, the relationship between intelligence and antisocial behavior continues into adulthood with lower IQ being a significant risk factor for criminal behavior (Hernstein & C. Murray, 1994; J. Murray et al., 2010). Lower levels of intelligence have also been found to predict longer criminal careers (Piquero & White, 2003) and higher rates of violence among incarcerated individuals (Diamond, Morris, & Barnes, 2012). And on the opposite end of the spectrum, a meta-analysis of IQ and crime found that higher IQ was a protective factor against offending (Ttofi et al., 2016).

Similarly, psychopathy has been repeatedly associated with antisocial behavior and criminal activity (Hemphill, Hare, & Wong, 1998; Porter, Brinke, & Wilson, 2009; Salekin, Rogers, & Sewell, 1996). A meta-analysis of 53 studies totalling over 10,000 participants determined that psychopathy was a significant predictor of juvenile delinquency and assessment of psychopathy as a predictor of violence was valid as early as middle childhood (Asscher et al., 2011). Additionally, psychopaths tend to commit more violent crime (Porter, Brinke, & Wilson, 2009), more violence in prison (Hare, 1999), and recidivate at much higher rates (Hemphill, Hare, & Wong, 1998; Langevin & Curnoe, 2011).

Given the overlap in outcomes that correlate with both low intelligence and psychopathy, researchers have recently become interested in directly testing the link between the two phenotypes. The findings of this line of research have been relatively equivocal with many non-significant results, several of which suggested a positive relationship between intelligence and psychopathy (Hare & Jutai, 1988; Pham,Philippot, & Rime, 2000). Others found non-significant negative relationships (Dolan & Park, 2002) as well as significant negative relationships (Dolan & Anderson, 2002).The ambiguity of these results and the common limitation of very small samples necessitates review and meta-analysis to further elucidate the possible link between intelligence and psychopathy.

Aims

The aim of this meta-analytic review is twofold: first, synthesize and analyze the empirical evidence (published and unpublished) on whether intellectual functioning (IQ) relates to psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) using the meta-analytical technique; and second to identify which variables moderate this association to understand the current divergent results in the literature.

METHOD

Inclusion criteria

To be included in this meta-analysis, the studies had to meet all the following criteria to assure the quality of the study and that sufficient information would be provided to allow the calculation of the effect sizes and to avoid publication biases: (1) the studies had to be written in English, published in a peer-reviewed journal, or published as a dissertation; (2) a psychometric instrument for psychopathy or ASPD had to be administered (e.g. the PCL- R (Psychopathy Checklist—revised), the PPI-R (Psychopathic Personality Inventory-Revised for psychopathy, and the ICD-9, ICD-10, DSM-III, DSM-III-R, DSM-IV or DSM-V for the ASPD); (3) measures of premorbid-full, verbal and performance IQ (e.g. WAIS, SILS, NART) had to be included; (4) and studies had to include a control group or a measure of correlation between IQ and psychopathy or ASPD. Neuroimaging studies that included an IQ measure were also included. Conversely, studies that used factorial analyses were excluded; (5) the antisocial sample included psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder, also in their violent form (see search strategy); (6) we limited our search to male adults. Studies that included mixed samples (males and females) without splitting the groups were excluded in order to reduce heterogeneity.

Case reports, or patients with antisocial symptoms were excluded. Participants were not selected from populations systematically diagnosed with mental illness (psychosis and depression) or substance related disorders (alcohol, drugs). The decision for inclusion was taken by consensus between authors.

Moderator analyses

We assumed that the individual effect sizes were not likely to be homogeneous so we estimated a series of moderator analyses. Meta-regression was used as quality analysis, that is, to investigate moderating factors that might interfere with the results. Thus we investigated the relationship of years of education, age, and number of participants with the magnitude of the effect size of intellectual functioning. Conversely, we used subgroup analyses for examining the publication type, use of PCL-R, settings, group of comparison, data used to calculate the effect sizes and IQ measures. For all categorical variables, moderator analyses were conducted using the analog to the ANOVA (with random effects), whereas the moderator analyses for the continuous variables were investigated by using fixed effect regression.

Search strategy

The search for candidate studies to be included in the meta-analysis was conducted using keywords relevant to antisocial behavior AND IQ/intelligence (see Appendix 1) in four electronic indices (PubMed, PsycINFO, ISI Web of Science, Google Scholar) for published English language studies and dissertations between January 1941 and June 2015. 1941 was selected as starting date because Cleckley was the first author who listed 16 criteria for psychopathy in his book “The Mask of Sanity” in 1941. In addition, all of the reference lists of the studies included for analysis, as well as several review articles on the relation of cognition to psychopathy and antisocial behavior were reviewed (e.g. Brennan & Raine, 1997; Fitzgerald & Demakis, 2007; Kiehl, 2006; Maes & Brazil, 2013; Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000; Ogilvie et al. 2011; Reidy et al. 2011).

Coding the studies

The following information was extracted from each study. Moderator variables are noted with an asterisk: (1) reference data (i.e. first name of the author and year of publication or dissertation); (2) publication type*; (3) sample type and size*; (4) age of the sample*; (5) IQ measures*; (6) assessment method criteria for psychopathy and ASPD*; (7) psychopathy cut off; (8) establishment recruitment*; (9) data reported to calculate the effect size* and (10) effect size and its significance (see Table 1). One investigator independently coded each eligible study. There was no double coding of categories because they were objective.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Characteristics of the studies included in the meta-analysis

Meta-analysis procedure

Meta-analyses were performed using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis, Version 3, Biostat, Englewood NJ (Borenstein et al., 2005). The majority of the studies provided the means and standard deviations necessary for calculating the effect sizes, however at times t-values, f-values, p-values, and r-values were used to calculate effect sizes (see Lipsey & Wilson, 2001 for effect sizes calculations). The effect size used was Cohen´s d (defined as the difference between means divided by the pooled standard deviation (see Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). This has been proposed as the most appropriate measure for neuropsychological research because it accounts for the variability observed in patients with neuropsychological disorders (Zakzanis, 2001). Negative effect sizes in the present meta-analysis mean that the control group (non-antisocial) had higher scores in the IQ tests than antisocial groups.

Initially, a single effect size for all IQ measures within a study was calculated, that is, a weighted mean effect size for all tests was computed within a study to be entered in the meta-analysis. When two dependent variables were indicators of IQ, a pooled effect size was computed. For instance, a combined d value was computed in the overall analysis for the same studies that reported information on different tests. If a test reported two dependent variables (e.g. verbal score and performance score), both were considered separately. Furthermore, in studies where data was reported for several subgroups (e.g., violent ASPD and, or low/high anxiety criminals and low/high anxiety psychopaths), data were considered separately to compute the effect size for each subgroup instead of being pooled. Finally, extreme group contrasts were used to calculate effect sizes (i.e. the low and high group scores).

For each meta-analysis, homogeneity (Q and I2) tests were performed to determine whether the studies can reasonably be described as sharing a common effect size, that is, the variation among study outcomes was due to random chance (Q test) and the percentage of variation across the studies due to significant heterogeneity (I2 test) (Hedges and Olkin, 1985, Higgins et al., 2003). Generally, I2 up to 40% represents relatively inconsequential, 30%-60% moderate, 50%-90% substantial, and 75%-100% considerable heterogeneity (Higgins et al., 2003). A random effects model was considered due to diversity of methods of measurement used in each analysis. According to the classification adopted by Cohen, small, medium and large effect sizes were defined by Cohen’s d values of 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8, respectively (Cohen, 1988). Publication bias was assessed using both Egger’s regression (Egger et al., 1997) and the Fail-Safe N tests to evaluate whether the available literature was biased towards non-published studies.

RESULTS

Literature search

Following an initial identification of over 7,179 hits1 (this number is the total hits from database searches (7,095) + 84 records identified through other sources), we sorted through the titles and abstracts and removed any that were inconsistent with the inclusion criteria. This process reduced the number of potentially relevant studies to 187 studies. These 187 studies were thoroughly reviewed and final coding decisions were made as to whether the study conformed to each of the inclusion criteria. Ninety studies were excluded because of the inclusion of females in the sample, inclusion of other neuropsychological domains, antisocial personality disorder was mixed with other personality disorders, did not report the measures of psychopathy but just violent offenders, did not report data for IQ, the sample was youth or psychiatric patients or the papers were not accessible or in another language (see also Figure 1).

Characteristics of Studies Included in Meta-Analysis

Table 1 present a series of descriptive statistics characterizing the 97 included studies, as well as the procedures used to narrow down eligible studies. Ninety one percent of the studies were published in peer-reviewed journals and 37% were non-published dissertations. A total of 9,010 participants were included in the analysis (4,321 antisocial and 4,689 control) with a mean age of 32.2 (SD=7.64) for antisocial groups and 31.7 (SD=7.61) for control groups. The antisocial group was mainly composed of criminal psychopaths (57%) and anxious/non-anxious psychopaths (12%), whereas 9% were diagnosed with ASPD and 5% were a combination of ASPD and psychopaths/non-psychopaths. Other studies used specific samples such as violent psychopaths (4%), violent ASPD (8%), and successful/unsuccessful psychopaths (3%). Most of the psychopaths and ASPD were recruited from prisons (62%) and high security hospitals (16%) or a combination of both (9%). A small proportion of studies recruited the antisocial group from forensic centers (5%), and temporary employment agencies and universities (6%). The antisocial groups were mainly compared to criminal non-psychopaths (47%) and the general population (16%). The other half of the studies used high/low anxious non-psychopaths (12%) and a combination of several control groups (16%) including general population/non-violent schizophrenics/violent schizophrenics; non-psychopaths/general population; drug users/general population; and violent offenders/non-violent offenders/non-psychopaths/ criminals drug users/criminal non-drug users. Only two studies (2%) used mental disorders and one study (1%) used patients with and without frontal dysfunction as control groups. Furthermore, five studies (5%) did not report a control group because they simply examined the correlation between psychopathy and intellectual functioning.

The majority of the studies (83%) used the PCL-R score to assess psychopathy and subjects receiving a score of 30, 25, 18 or higher were classified as psychopaths in 48%, 6% and 4% of the studies, respectively. Twenty one percent of the studies used diverse cut offs such as 26, 17, 19, 23.63, 33, 28.5, 31.5, 12, 28 and 23, whereas 12% of the studies did not report cut offs. Conversely, 13% of the studies used clinical assessment, the DSM-IV and other scales to assess the antisocial groups. The measures to assess the intellectual functioning of these participants were mainly the full score of the WAIS (27.08%), the SILS (25%), the NART (10.4%) and the Raven´s Progressive Matrices (8.3%). Fifteen percent of studies used a combination of tests such as the NART and the WAIS or the NART and the Quick Test. Effect sizes were computed by calculating Cohen’s d from the available information, which were predominantly means and standard deviations (84.5%), whereas 5% was the correlation and sample size, 4% was the t-value, 2% was the mean and p-value, d and variance; and 1% was the correlation. The last column of the Table 1 shows the Cohen´s d for individual effect sizes in each study and its statistical significance. Twelve studies (12%) reported a significant small and medium effect size (range: -0.28 to -0.75). There were three studies which reported outliers (-1.66, -0.93, 0.93) (see sensitivity analysis) and one out of five correlations was significant (0.36). Keeping in mind that most of these studies used IQ to match the samples, it appears that the effect of the association between psychopathy and intellectual functioning was robust.

Types of groups

The summary effect size yielded by the meta-analysis was d = -0.15, 95% CI: -0.21, -0.09; p < .001, indicating that antisocial groups show small but significant impairment in intellectual functioning. Consistent with our first assumption, we used a random-effects analytic approach, as the Q test rejected the null hypothesis of homogeneity of variance Qtotal (205) = 455.80, p < .001. The I2 test indicate moderate heterogeneity (I2 = 55.02). As seen in Table 2, when splitting those with ASPD from psychopaths, effect sizes were significantly negative (d=-0.40 and d=-0.12, respectively), suggesting that these two groups performed worse than controls on the measures of IQ. Both the ASPD and psychopathy groups had significant heterogeneity, but the psychopathy group (I2 = 30.40) had lower heterogeneity than the ASPD group (I2 = 78.72). Importantly, neither violent groups nor the combination of ASPD and psychopathy reported significant impairments, which might may be due to the small number of comparisons in the case of violent psychopaths (k=6) and violent ASPD combined with psychopathy (k=1).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2.

Psychopathic and Antisocial Personality Disorder Effect Sizes for IQ

Type of comparisons

Studies used both different antisocial groups and comparison groups. In this meta-analysis we wanted to examine two things: the differences between several comparisons, and if the level of homogeneity increased. As seen in Table 3 most of the studies employed criminal psychopaths compared to criminal non-psychopaths (k=67) followed by antisocial personality disorder with the general population (k=21). The significant negative effect size (d=-0.11) showed that criminal psychopaths obtained lower IQ scores than criminal non-psychopaths. The effect size was larger for the comparison of those with ASPD to the general population (d=-0.53). Additionally, other comparisons reported significant small and medium negative effect sizes: ASPD+psychopathy/general population (GP) (d=-0.24); Violent ASPD-psychopathy/GP (d=-0.29); Successful psychopaths/GP (d=-0.43); Unsuccessful psychopaths/GP (d=-0.55); Psychopaths/Criminal Non-Drugs Users (d=-0.58); Violent ASPD/GP (d=-0.62); High negative affective psychopathy/High negative affective non-psychopaths (d=-0.64); Violent ASPD+psyschopathy/GP (d=-0.58); Psychopathy/Non-violent offenders (d=-0.63). The ASPD/Personality disorders group was the only group with a large effect size (d=-1.86). However, some of these effects must be viewed cautiously since they are based on only one study.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 3.

Effect Sizes by Groups

Overall it seems that when antisocial groups (i.e. antisocial personality disorder, antisocial personality disorder with psychopathy, successful and unsuccessful psychopaths, and violent antisocial personality disorder with and without psychopathy) are compared to the general population the effect size is larger than when they are compared to non-psychopathic offenders. Conversely, the comparison between psychopaths and non-violent offenders (P/NVO) showed a larger significant negative effect size but is based on only one study.

The heterogeneity between the studies was significant (Qbetween=138.77, p<.001) indicating that the type of comparison has a significant effect on results. The largest heterogeneity was reported for ASPD/Personality disorders (Q (4) = 75.12, p <.001, I2 =94.67) followed by ASPD/GP (Q (21) = 61.93, p <.001, I2 =67.70). This is unsurprising since the control group in these studies was comprised of participants with mental/personality disorders and included a diverse array of disorders such as schizophrenia, borderline personality disorder, narcissistic personality disorder, etc. However, some significant comparisons showed low levels of heterogeneity such as incarcerated Psychopaths/Non-Psychopaths (I2 =1.08) and ASPD/Violent schizophrenics (I2 =15.61). Finally, some heterogeneity was accounted for by the type of comparison used across studies, which in turn moderated the association between the key variables (Qbetween = 138.77, p< .001).

Type of IQ measures

Although, it is assumed that IQ measures correlate, it is worth examining whether some of the heterogeneity is due to different IQ measures used across the studies. As seen in Table 4, most of the studies employed the full score of the WAIS (k=54) and the SILS (k=51). Both reported similar effect sizes (d=-0.12 and -0.07,respectively). Similarly, the NART and the Quick Test reported similar effect sizes (d=-0.21 and -0.24, respectively), but the effects were larger than for the WAIS and SILS. Furthermore, the Quick Test was used in fewer studies (k=12) than the NART (k=45). Conversely, the Raven´s Progressive Matrices, which measures abstract reasoning and the non-verbal aspect of fluid intelligence, did not show a significant effect. Finally, the largest effect size was for the CFT20-R (d=-0.42) but was based on two comparisons.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 4.

Main Effect Sizes by Outcome Measure

With regards to the subtests, none of them reported significant effects, except for the verbal and performance score of the WAIS. The former showed a very large significant effect (d=-1.03), whereas the latter showed a small but significant effect (d=- 0.37), larger than full scores of the SILS, WAIS and Quick Test. This very large effect size might be due to the effect of an outlier. Importantly, the information subscale of the WAIS was the only significant positive effect size (d=0.36), indicating that psychopaths performed better than controls. However, it is only based on a single study.

The heterogeneity test was significant for the WAIS (Q (53) = 148.99, p < .001) and the verbal subscale combination of measures (Q (6) = 140.01, p < .001), whereas the rest of the measures remain homogenous. However, the type of intelligence measure used across the studies was an important moderator since the results of the analog to the ANOVA analyses were significant (Qbetween = 38.39, p < .001).

Moderator analyses

Due to the significant heterogeneity of the effect sizes, we explore potential moderating variables. We selected a number of potential categorical moderators that may be particularly relevant including: whether the study was published (yes/no), whether the study used the PCL-R to assess psychopathy (yes/no), whether the antisocial group was recruited from: prisons, in maximum security hospitals, both, universities or temporary employment agencies or forensic assessments units; the control group used (criminal no psychopaths, general population, violent mental disordered, non-violent mental disordered, no control group/correlation, other), and data reported (mean/SD, correlation, t value, sample size/p value, d/variance). The results of the ANOVA analyses (with random effects) investigating possible moderators of the IQ effect sizes reported that the publication type (Qbetween (1)= 1.19, p=0.16) and settings (Qbetween (4) = 3.91, p=0.41) did not affect the effect sizes, whereas the use of PCL-R score of the antisocial sample (Qbetween (1) = 5.71, p=0.01), the control group (Qbetween (5) = 47.95, p=0.0001) (violent groups positive vs GP), IQ measure (Qbetween (15) = 38.39, p=0.001) and the type of data reported (Qbetween (1) = 7.75, p=0.005) moderated the effect size.

We also included two continuous measures as moderators: sample size and age of the participants. There were not enough studies that reported level of education of the sample to include education as a moderator. The analyses of fixed-effect regression showed that effect size was not moderated by the sample size (b = -0.0006, p=0.20) or the age of the participants (b = 0.01, p=0.20).

Sensitivity analysis

In addition to moderator analyses, we carried out an outlier analysis to test the robustness of the results. Few studies (Dolan & Anderson, 2002; Kiehl et al. 2000; Kumari et al. 2006; Raine & Venables, 1988) contained extreme effect sizes. However, the exclusion of these outliers slightly decreased the overall effect size (d = -0.12, 95%CI: -0.16, -0.07, p=.0001), but the model remained significantly negative. Therefore, this difference is small, and suggests that our overall effect is robust.

Publication bias

In consonance with the moderator analysis, the Egger’s tests showed no indication of significant publication bias (d= 0.11, 95%CI: -0.66, 0.42; p=0.67), as well as the Fail-Safe N test. The result for this test was that 2,953 studies would have been required to render the mean effect statistically non-significant. It is highly unlikely that in our literature search we may have overlooked that large amount of studies.

Discussion

The results of the current meta-analysis produced a small, but significant effect size (d = -0.15) suggesting that individuals who score higher on measures of psychopathic traits tend to score lower on measures of IQ. Moderator analyses determined that the type of control group, type of IQ test, and type of data reported did moderate the effect size. For instance, the effect size is larger when psychopaths are compared to a general population sample than when they are compared to non-psychopathic offenders. However, fixed effect analyses of the age of participant and sample size did not moderate the relationship. Additionally, sensitivity analyses confirmed that although the effect size is small, it appeared to be robust. The IQ-Psychopathy relationship varied by measure of IQ, with the WAIS verbal score exhibiting the strongest relationship (d = -1.03) in line with previous research suggesting that low verbal intelligence is particularly related to psychopathic traits (DeLisi, Vaughn, Beaver, & Wright, 2010).

It is noted that a relatively large number of studies reported non-significant effect sizes including a few in the opposite direction of the overall effect size. The results demonstrated that heterogeneity in aspects of intelligence and especially in psychopathy may play a role in dampening the effect. While there is a substantial body of empirical support for the concept of general intelligence and scores on various measures of intelligence (e.g. verbal, working memory, processing speed, etc) are highly correlated, the correlations are not perfect and as shown in the moderator analyses the type of IQ test utilized did moderate the relationship.

In fact, there seems to be an interaction between the facet of IQ being measured and the facet of psychopathy being measured. As demonstrated by the moderator analyses, and in line with previous research, verbal intelligence tests seem to show the strongest relationship of any intelligence type with overall scores on measures of psychopathy (DeLisi et al., 2010). However, numerous studies have found that the relationship between verbal intelligence and psychopathy is highly variable when examining the facets that comprise psychopathy. Salekin and colleagues (2004) found that individuals exhibiting higher scores in the superficial and deceitful interpersonal style facet of psychopathy actually possessed greater ability in the verbal domain. Vitacco, Neumann and Jackson (2005) also found a positive relationship between verbal IQ and the interpersonal facet of psychopathy. Interestingly, they found a negative relationship between verbal IQ and the behaviour and lifestyle components (Vitacco, Neumann, & Jackson, 2005).

It is becoming increasingly clear that both general intelligence and psychopathy are highly complex traits that contain a large degree of heterogeneity. Psychopathy in particular, has been divided into a variety of subtypes in recent research. These subtypes, while they have not been fully adopted by the psychopathy researcher community, have received a degree of empirical support. The two most common subdivisions are into primary and secondary psychopathy and successful and unsuccessful psychopathy. Research that has utilized such subtypes has found support for discrete differences between groups and raises the possibility that each subtype might be differentially related to intelligence and other important traits.

Despite a lack of agreement over the exact definition of primary and secondary subtypes, there is significant evidence that both are captured under the umbrella of psychopathy, yet exhibit distinct patterns of traits (Ross et al., 2007; Vaughn et al., 2009). Ross and colleagues found evidence to suggest differences between primary and secondary psychopaths’ behavioral inhibition systems (BIS). Primary psychopathy was negatively associated with the BIS (i.e. displayed less sensitivity to punishment, less anxiety, and reduced behavioral inhibition), while secondary psychopathy was not significantly associated with the BIS. In a sample of incarcerated juveniles, Vaughn and associates (2009) found that secondary psychopathy was associated with higher levels of depression, anxiety, and suicidal ideation whereas primary psychopathy was associated with lower levels of distress, similar to the level of non-psychopathic controls.

Psychopathy has also been subdivided between those who have been involved in the criminal justice system (“unsuccessful” or “criminal” psychopaths) and those who have managed to avoid legal troubles (“successful” psychopaths; Cleckley, 1941; Gao & Raine, 2010). Successful psychopaths exhibit the same core features of the psychopathic personality, but owing to other traits manage to avoid contact with the criminal justice system. They are thought to be prevalent in higher numbers in certain professions such as business (Babiak & Hare, 2006) where psychopathic traits such as manipulativeness and a lack of empathy can be particularly beneficial. Unsuccessful psychopaths on the other hand, have the core features of psychopathy, but are more impaired than their successful counterparts leading to more criminal behavior and easier detection by law enforcement. Despite the growing popularity of subdividing psychopathic samples into primary/secondary and successful/unsuccessful, researchers have rarely distinguished between such subtypes in studies of how psychopathy is related to intelligence. Future research will need to be mindful of the growing evidence for heterogeneity when assessing psychopathy and its correlates.

The current study analyzed research examining bivariate relations between psychopathy and intelligence, yet other researchers have found intelligence to be a mediator between psychopathic traits and antisocial behavior (Muñoz et al., 2008; Salekin et al., 2010). Kandel et al. (1988) found that higher intelligence is a protective factor against offending generally (i.e. not looking specifically at psychopaths). Conversely, Muñoz et al. (2008) found higher intelligence to be a risk factor for increased violent offending among psychopaths. Salekin and colleagues (2010) found no relationship between IQ and offending among psychopaths. Thus, it remains unclear how much of a role intelligence plays as a mediator of behavior in psychopathy, however, these studies and the small effect sizes found in the meta-analysis suggest a degree of heterogeneity of intelligence within psychopathy. Nevertheless, the existence of a significant overall effect size suggests that there is a degree of overlap between intelligence and psychopathy.

Limitations and Future Directions

The studies available for analysis were not without limitation. As mentioned earlier in the discussion, there is a paucity of research on the psychopathy/intelligence relationship that examined how different factors of psychopathy are related to IQ and how different facets of IQ are related to psychopathy. Another limitation is that the majority of the studies used inmate samples. Additional research is needed to further elucidate which facets of each of the two constructs drives the relationship and to further examine the relationship in non-incarcerated populations, women, and adolescents. Finally, some studies included in this review were correlational, that is to say they lacked a control group.

One of the most important gaps in the literature to date concerns the degree to which psychopathy and intelligence share etiological factors. Neuroscience research has already provided some evidence of shared neuroanatomical and neuroconnective abnormalities between psychopathy and low intelligence. Specifically, similar deficits in the volume and connectivity of the prefrontal cortex have been reported in both psychopathy (Yang et al., 2005; Motzkin et al., 2011) as well as instances of lower intelligence (Cole et al., 2012; Haier et al., 2004; Thompson et al., 2001; Toga & Thompson, 2005). Genetically sensitive methods (e.g. twin studies) are needed to evaluate the degree to which common genetic and environmental factors overlap between psychopathy and IQ. Overall, psychopathy and intelligence will continue to be important constructs in the behavioral sciences. Further understanding the nature of the connection between the two traits remains an important endeavour for psychological research.

APPENDIX I: LITERATURE SEARCH: Keywords

  • Psychopathy and Antisocial personality disorder (ASPD)

    Antisocial personality disorder/ASPD, antisocial behavior, psychopath, psychopathy, psychopathic, violent, violence, aggressive, aggression, offender, criminal. Terms such as violence, aggression and criminal were included because the relationship between psychopathy and instrumental violence has been well substantiated (see Reidy et al. 2011). Conversely, terms such as conduct disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, disruptive behavior disorders were not included because they refer to children and teens.

  • Cognitive domains

    Prefrontal cortex/PFC, executive functioning/EF, planning, cognitive flexibility/set shifting, motor regulation, response inhibition, decision making, abstraction, concept formation, cognitive control, frontal function, Intelligence/IQ, memory, short-term memory, long-term memory, spatial memory, verbal memory, episodic memory, explicit memory, verbal recall, visual recall, attention, sustained attention, language, verbal expression, academic skills/reading, semantic processing, knowledge.

  • Neuropsychological tests/tasks

    Stroop task, D2 test, Go-NoGo task, CPT (Continuous Performance Test), Category test, Raven´s Advanced Matrices, WAIS (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale), D-KEFS (The Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System), CANTAB (Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery), NART (National Adult Reading Test), WCST (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test), TMT-A & B (Trail Making Test Part A & B), Proverbs, Porteus Maze Test, Cognitive Estimation test, verbal fluency, COWAT (Controlled Oral Word Association Test)/FAS, WMS (Wechsler Memory Scale), Digit Span, CVLT (California Verbal Learning Test), Verbal Learning Test, RAVLT (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test), ToL (Tower of London), SILS (Shipley Institute of Living Scale), IGT (Iowa Gambling Task).

Footnotes

  • ↵1 This search included other cognitive/neuropsychological domains (e.g., Iowa Gambling Task), which will be presented in a different article (in progress) (see Appendix I). Here we only considered the IQ domain.

REFERENCES

Articles with asterisk (*) were included in the meta-analysis

  1. ↵
    Ackerman, P. L., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and interests: evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological bulletin, 121(2), 219.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  2. *.
    Arnett, P. A., Howland, E. W., Smith, S. S., & Newman, J. P. (1993). Autonomic responsivity during passive avoidance in incarcerated psychopaths. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 173–184.
    OpenUrl
  3. *.
    Arnett, P. A., Smith, S. S., & Newman, J. P. (1997). Approach and avoidance motivation in psychopathic criminal offenders during passive avoidance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1413–1428.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  4. ↵
    Asscher, J. J., van Vugt, E. S., Stams, G. J. J., Deković, M., Eichelsheim, V. I., & Yousfi, S. (2011). The relationship between juvenile psychopathic traits, delinquency and (violent) recidivism: A meta-analysis. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 52(11), 1134–1143.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  5. ↵
    Babiak, P., & Hare, R. D. (2006). Snakes in suits: When psychopaths go to work. New York, NY: Regan Books.
  6. *.
    Bagshaw; R., Gray, N.S., & Snowden, R.J. (2014). Executive function in psychopathy: The Tower of London, Brixton Spatial Anticipation and the Hayling Sentence Completion Tests. Psychiatry Research, 220, 483–489.
    OpenUrl
  7. *.
    Bagley, A. D., Abramowitz, C. S., & Kosson, D. S. (2009). Vocal affect recognition and psychopathy: Converging findings across traditional and cluster analytic approaches to assessing the construct. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 388–398.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  8. *.
    Barkataki, I., Kumari, V., Das, M., Taylor, P., & Sharma, T. (2006). Volumetric structural brain abnormalities in men with schizophrenia or antisocial personality disorder. Behavioral Brain Research, 169, 239–247.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  9. *.
    Barkataki, I., Kumari, V., Das, M., Sumich, A., Taylor, P., & Sharma, T. (2008). Neural correlates of deficient response inhibition in mentally disordered violent individuals. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 26, 51–64.
    OpenUrl
  10. *.
    Barkataki, I., Kumaria, V., Das, M., Hill, M., Morris, R., O’Connell, P., Taylor, P., Sharma, T. (2005). A neuropsychological investigation into violence and mental illness. Schizophrenia Research, 74, 1–13.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  11. ↵
    Batty, G. D., Deary, I. J., Tengstrom, A., & Rasmussen, F. (2008). IQ in early adulthood and later risk of death by homicide: cohort study of 1 million men. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 193(6), 461–465.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. ↵
    Batty, G. D., Der, G., Macintyre, S., & Deary, I. J. (2006). Does IQ explain socioeconomic inequalities in health? Evidence from a population based cohort study in the west of Scotland. Bmj, 332(7541), 580–584.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. ↵
    Beaver, K. M., Schwartz, J. A., Connolly, E. J., Said Al-Ghamdi, M., Kobeisy, A. N., Barnes, J. C., & Boutwell, B. B. (2016). Intelligence and early life mortality: Findings from a longitudinal sample of youth. Death studies, 40(5), 298–304.
    OpenUrl
  14. *.
    Beggs, S.M., & Grace, R.C. (2008). Psychopathy, intelligence, and recidivism in child molesters. Evidence of an interaction effect. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 35, 683–695.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  15. *.
    Blair, R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57, 1–29.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  16. *.
    Blair, R., Sellars, C., Strickland, I., Clark, F., & et al. (1995). Emotion attributions in the psychopath. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 431–437.
    OpenUrl
  17. *.
    Blair, R., Sellars, C., Strickland, I., Clark, F., Williams, A., Smith, M. (1996). Theory of mind in the psychopath. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 7, 15–25.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  18. *.
    Blair, R., Jones, L., Clark, F., & Smith, M. (1997). The psychopathic individual: A lack of responsiveness to distress cues? Psychophysiology, 34, 192–198.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  19. *.
    Blair, R., Mitchell, D., Richell, R. A., Leonard, A., Scott, S. K., Newman, C. (2002). Turning a deaf ear to fear: Impaired recognition of vocal affect in psychopathic individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 682–686.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  20. *.
    Blair, R., Mitchell, D., Peschardt, K., Colledge, E., Leonard, R., Shine, J. (2004). Reduced sensitivity to others’ fearful expressions in psychopathic individuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1111–1122.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  21. *.
    Blair, R., Mitchell, D., Leonard, A., Budhani, S., Peschardt, K., & Newman, C. (2004). Passive avoidance learning in individuals with psychopathy: Modulation by reward but not by punishment. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1179–1192.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  22. *.
    Blair, K., Richell, R., Mitchell, D., Leonard, A., Morton, J., & Blair, R. (2006). They know the words, but not the music: Affective and semantic priming in individuals with psychopathy. Biological Psychology, 73, 114–123.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  23. *.
    Blair, K., Newman, C., Mitchell, D., Richell, R., Leonard, A., Morton, J. (2006). Differentiating among prefrontal substrates in psychopathy: Neuropsychological test findings. Neuropsychology, 20, 153–165.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  24. *.
    Blair, K., Morton, J., Leonard, A., & Blair, R. (2006). Impaired decision-making on the basis of both reward and punishment information in individuals with psychopathy. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 155–165.
    OpenUrl
  25. ↵
    Blair, R. J. R. (2007). The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex in morality and psychopathy. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11(9), 387–392.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  26. Blair, R. J. R., & Mitchell, D. G. V. (2009). Psychopathy, attention and emotion. Psychological medicine, 35(04), 543–555.
    OpenUrl
  27. ↵
    Borenstein, M., Ledges, L.V., Higgins, J.P.T., & Rothstein, H.R. (Eds.). (2005). Introduction to meta-analysis. London: Wiley.
  28. ↵
    Brennan, P.A., & Raine, A. (1997). Biosocial bases of antisocial behavior: psychophysiological, neurological, and cognitive factors. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 589–604.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  29. *.
    Brinkley, C. A., Schmitt, W. A., & Newman, J. P. (2005). Semantic processing in psychopathic offenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1047–1056.
    OpenUrl
  30. *.
    Budhani, S., Richell, R. A., & Blair, R. (2006). Impaired reversal but intact acquisition: Probabilistic response reversal deficits in adult individuals with psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 552–558.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  31. ↵
    Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. Cambridge University Press.
  32. *.
    Cima, M., Tonnaer, F., & Hauser, M. D. (2010). Psychopaths know right from wrong but don’t care. Social Affective and Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 59–67.
    OpenUrl
  33. ↵
    Cleckley, H. (1941) The mask of sanity: an attempt to reinterpret the so-called psychopathic personality. Oxford, England: Mosby.
  34. Cleckley, H. (1976). The mask of sanity (5th ed.). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
  35. ↵
    Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  36. ↵
    Cole, M. W., Yarkoni, T., Repovš, G., Anticevic, A., & Braver, T. S. (2012). Global connectivity of prefrontal cortex predicts cognitive control and intelligence. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(26), 8988–8999.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (2001). Refining the construct of psychopathy: towards a hierarchical model. Psychological assessment, 13(2), 171–188.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
    1. C.J. Patrick
    Cooke, D. J., Michie, C., & Hart, S. D. (2006). Facets of Clinical Psychopathy. In C.J. Patrick (Ed.) Handbook of Psychopathy, (pp. 91–106). New York, NY: The Guildford Press.
  38. *.
    Craig, M. C., Catani, M., Deeley, Q., Latham, R., Daly, F., Kanaan, R. (2009). Altered connections on the road to psychopathy. Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 1–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Deary, I. J., Strand, S., Smith, P., & Fernandes, C. (2007). Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence, 35(1), 13–21.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  40. ↵
    Deary, I. J., Weiss, A., & Batty, G. D. (2010). Intelligence and personality as predictors of illness and death how researchers in differential psychology and chronic disease epidemiology are collaborating to understand and address health inequalities. Psychological science in the public interest, 11(2), 53–79.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    Deary, I. J., Whalley, L. J., Lemmon, H., Crawford, J. R., & Starr, J. M. (2000). The stability of individual differences in mental ability from childhood to old age: follow-up of the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey. Intelligence, 28(1), 49–55.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  42. *.
    de Brito, S.A., Viding, E., Kumari, V., Blackwood, N., Hodgins, S. (2013). Cool and hot executive function impairments in violent offenders with antisocial personality disorder with and without psychopathy. PLoS ONE 8, e65566. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0065566
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    Der, G., Batty, G. D., & Deary, I. J. (2009). The association between IQ in adolescence and a range of health outcomes at 40 in the 1979 US National Longitudinal Study of Youth. Intelligence, 37(6), 573–580.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  44. *.
    de Tribolet-Hardy, F., Vohs, K., Mokros, A., & Habermeyer, E. (2014). Psychopathy, intelligence, and impulsivity in German violent offenders. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 37, 238–244.
    OpenUrl
  45. ↵
    DeLisi, M., Vaughn, M. G., Beaver, K. M., & Wright, J. P. (2010). The Hannibal Lecter myth: psychopathy and verbal intelligence in the MacArthur violence risk assessment study. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32(2), 169–177.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  46. ↵
    Diamond, B., Morris, R. G., & Barnes, J. C. (2012). Individual and group IQ predict inmate violence. Intelligence, 40(2), 115–122.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  47. *.↵
    Dolan, M. & Anderson, I.A. (2002). Executive and memory function and its relationship to trait impulsivity and aggression in personality disordered offenders. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 13, 503–526.
    OpenUrl
  48. *.
    Dolan, M., & Park, I. (2002). The neuropsychology of antisocial personality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 32, 417–427.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  49. *.
    Dolan, M.C., Deakin, J.F.W., Roberts, N., & Anderson, I.M. (2002). Quantitative frontal and temporal structural MRI studies in personality-disordered offenders and control subjects. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 116, 133–149.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  50. *.
    Dolan, M., & Fullam, R. (2004). Theory of mind and mentalizing ability in antisocial personality disorders with and without psychopathy. Psychological Medicine, 34, 1093–1102.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. *.
    Dolan, M., & Fullam, R. (2005). Memory for emotional events in violent offenders with antisocial personality disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1657–1667.
    OpenUrl
  52. *.
    Dolan, M., & Fullam, R. (2006). Face affect recognition deficits in personality-disordered offenders: Association with psychopathy. Psychological Medicine, 36, 1563–1569.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  53. *.
    Dolan, M. (2012). The neuropsychology of prefrontal function in antisocial personality disordered offenders with varying degrees of psychopathy. Psychological Medicine, 42, 1715–1725.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. *.
    Drugge, J. E. (1998). Psychopathy and inhibitory control. (doctoral dissertation), University of Toronto, Toronto, ON.
  55. ↵
    Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 13, 629–34.
    OpenUrl
  56. ↵
    Fitzgerald, K.L., & Demakis, G.J. (2007). The neuropsychology of antisocial personality disorder. Disease-a-month (DM), 53, 177–83.
    OpenUrl
  57. ↵
    Furnham, A., Moutafi, J., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2005). Personality and intelligence: Gender, the Big Five, self-estimated and psychometric intelligence. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 13(1), 11–24.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  58. ↵
    Gale, C. R., Batty, G. D., Tynelius, P., Deary, I. J., & Rasmussen, F. (2010). Intelligence in early adulthood and subsequent hospitalisation and admission rates for the whole range of mental disorders: longitudinal study of 1,049,663 men. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass.), 21(1), 70–77.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  59. ↵
    Gao, Y., & Raine, A. (2010). Successful and unsuccessful psychopaths: A neurobiological model. Behavioral sciences & the law, 28(2), 194–210.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  60. *.
    Gawda, B. (2008a). A graphical analysis of handwriting of prisoners diagnosed with antisocial personality. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 107, 862–872.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  61. *.
    Gillstrom, B. J. (1995). Abstract thinking in criminal psychopaths. (doctoral dissertation), The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC
  62. *.
    Glass, S. J., & Newman, J. P. (2006). Recognition of facial affect in psychopathic offenders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 815–820.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  63. *.
    Glass, S. J., & Newman, J. P. (2009). Emotion processing in the criminal psychopath: The role of attention in emotion-facilitated memory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 229–234.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. *.
    Goldstein, D. S. (1998). Assessment of frontal lobe functioning in psychopathy. (doctoral dissertation), The Herman M. Finch University of Health Sciences - The Chicago Medical School.
  65. ↵
    Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence, 24(1), 79–132.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  66. ↵
    Gottfredson, L. S. (2002). Where and why g matters: Not a mystery. Human performance, 15(1-2), 25–46.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  67. ↵
    Gottfredson, L. S., & Deary, I. J. (2004). Intelligence predicts health and longevity, but why?. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(1), 1–4.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  68. ↵
    1. F. D. Horowitz
    1. R. F. Subotnik
    1. D. J. Matthews
    Gottfried, A. W., Gottfried, A. E., & Guerin, D. W. (2009). Issues in early prediction and identification of intellectual giftedness. In F. D. Horowitz, R. F. Subotnik, & D. J. Matthews (Eds.), The development of giftedness and talent across the life span (pp. 43–56). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  69. ↵
    Greengross, G., & Miller, G. (2011). Humor ability reveals intelligence, predicts mating success, and is higher in males. Intelligence, 39(4), 188–192.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  70. ↵
    Haier, R. J., Jung, R. E., Yeo, R. A., Head, K., & Alkire, M. T. (2004). Structural brain variation and general intelligence. Neuroimage, 23(1), 425–433.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  71. ↵
    Hare, R. D. (1996). Psychopathy a clinical construct whose time has come. Criminal justice and behavior, 23(1), 25–54.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  72. ↵
    Hare, R. D., Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, A. R., Forth, A. E., Hart, S. D., & Newman, J. P. (1990). The revised Psychopathy Checklist: Reliability and factor structure. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2(3), 338–341.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  73. *.
    Hare, R.D. & McPherson, L.M. (1984). Psychopathy and perceptual asymmetry during verbal dichotic listening. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 141–149.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  74. *.↵
    Hare, R. D., & Jutai, J. W. (1988). Psychopathy and cerebral asymmetry in semantic processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 329–337.
    OpenUrl
  75. ↵
    Hare, R. D. (1999). Psychopathy as a risk factor for violence. Psychiatric Quarterly, 70(3), 181–197.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  76. ↵
    Hare, R. D., & Vertommen, H. (1991). The Hare psychopathy checklist-revised. Multi-Health Systems, Incorporated.
  77. *.
    Hart, S. Forth, A.D., & Hare, R.D. (1990). Performance of criminal psychopaths on selected neuropsychological tests. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 374–379.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  78. ↵
    Hart, C. L., Taylor, M. D., Smith, G. D., Whalley, L. J., Starr, J. M., Hole, D. J., … & Deary, I. J. (2004). Childhood IQ and cardiovascular disease in adulthood: prospective observational study linking the Scottish Mental Survey 1932 and the Midspan studies. Social science & medicine, 59(10), 2131–2138.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. New York: Academic Press.
  80. *.
    Heinzen, H., Köhler, D., Godt, N., Geiger, F., Huchzermeier, C. (2011). Psychopathy, intelligence and conviction history. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 34, 336–340.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  81. ↵
    Hemphill, J. F., Hare, R. D., & Wong, S. (1998). Psychopathy and recidivism: A review. Legal and criminological Psychology, 3(1), 139–170.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  82. ↵
    Hernstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The Bell Curve. New York: The Free Press.
  83. *.
    Herpertz, S. C., Werth, U., Lucas, G., Qunaibi, M., Schuerkens, A., Kunert, H.-J. (2001). Emotion in criminal offenders with psychopathy and borderline personality disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 737–745.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  84. *.
    Hiatt, K. D., Lorenz, A. R., & Newman, J. P. (2002). Assessment of emotion and language processing in psychopathic offenders: Results from a dichotic listening task. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1255–1268.
    OpenUrl
  85. *.
    Hiatt, K. D., Schmitt, W. A., & Newman, J. P. (2004). Stroop tasks reveal abnormal selective attention among psychopathic offenders. Neuropsychology, 18, 50–59.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  86. *.
    Hiatt, K. D. (2005). Interhemispheric integration among psychopathic offenders. (doctoral dissertation), The University of Wisconsin - Madison, Wisconsin, WI.
  87. ↵
    Higgins, D. M., Peterson, J. B., Pihl, R. O., & Lee, A. G. (2007). Prefrontal cognitive ability, intelligence, Big Five personality, and the prediction of advanced academic and workplace performance. Journal of personality and social psychology, 93(2), 298–319.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  88. ↵
    Higgins, J.P.T., Thompson, S.G., Deeks, J.J., & Altman, D.G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Bristish Medical Journal, 327, 557–560.
    OpenUrl
  89. ↵
    Hirschi, T., & Hindelang, M. J. (1977). Intelligence and delinquency: A revisionist review. American Sociological Review, 571–587.
  90. *.
    Howard, R., & McCullagh, P. (2007). Neuroaffective processing in criminal psychopaths: Brain event-related potentials reveal task-specific anomalies. Journal of Personality Disorders, 21, 322–339.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  91. *.
    Ishikawa, S. S., Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., & Lacasse, L. (2001). Autonomic stress reactivity and executive functions in successful and unsuccessful criminal psychopaths from the community. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 423–432.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  92. *.
    Johansson, P., & Kerr, M. (2005). Psychopathy and Intelligence: A Second Look. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 357–369.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  93. ↵
    Kanazawa, S. (2006). IQ and the wealth of states. Intelligence, 34(6), 593–600.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  94. ↵
    Kandel, E., Mednick, S. A., Kirkegaard-Sorenson, L., Hutchings, B., Knop, J., Rosenberg, R., & Schulsinger, F. (1988). IQ as a protective factor for subjects at high risk for antisocial behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 224–226.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  95. ↵
    Kiehl, K.A. (2006). A cognitive neuroscience perspective on psychopathy: Evidence for paralimbic system dysfunction. Psychiatry Research, 142, 107–128.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  96. *.↵
    Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., & Liddle, P. F. (2000). An event-related potential investigation of response inhibition in schizophrenia and psychopathy. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 210–221.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  97. *.
    Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Brink, J. (2001). Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 677–684.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  98. *.
    Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Hare, R. D., & Liddle, P. F. (2004). Temporal lobe abnormalities in semantic processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 130, 297–312.
    OpenUrl
  99. *.
    Kiehl, K. A., Laurens, K. R., Bates, A. T., & Liddle, P. F. (2006). Psychopathy and semantic processing: An examination of the N400. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 293–304.
    OpenUrl
  100. *.
    Kiehl, K. A., Bates, A. T., Laurens, K. R., Hare, R. D., & Liddle, P. F. (2006). Brain potentials implicate temporal lobe abnormalities in criminal psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 443–453.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  101. *.
    Kosson, D. S. (1996). Psychopathy and dual-task performance under focusing conditions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 391–400.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  102. *.
    Kosson, D. S. (1998). Divided visual attention in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 373–391.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  103. *.
    Kosson, D. S., Suchy, Y., Mayer, A. R., & Libby, J. (2002). Facial affect recognition in criminal psychopaths. Emotion, 2, 398–411.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  104. *.
    Kosson, D. S., Miller, S. K., Byrnes, K. A., & Leveroni, C. L. (2007). Testing neuropsychological hypotheses for cognitive deficits in psychopathic criminals: A study of global-local processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 267–276.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  105. ↵
    Kranzler, J. H., & Jensen, A. R. (1991). The nature of psychometric g: Unitary process or a number of independent processes?. Intelligence, 15(4), 397–422.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  106. *.
    Kumari, V., Das, M., Hodgins, S., Zachariah, E., Barkataki, I., Howlett, M. (2005). Association between violent behavior and impaired prepulse inhibition of the startle response in antisocial personality disorder and schizophrenia. Behavioral Brain Research, 158, 159–166.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  107. *.↵
    Kumari, V., Aasen, I., Taylor, P., Fytche, D. H., Das, M., Barkataki, I. (2006). Neural dysfunction and violence in schizophrenia: An fMRI investigation. Schizophrenia Research, 84, 144–164.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  108. *.
    Kumari, V., Das, M., Taylor, P. J., Barkataki, I., Andrew, C., Sumich, A. (2009). Neural and behavioural responses to threat in men with a history of serious violence and schizophrenia or antisocial personality disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 110, 47–58.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  109. ↵
    Langevin, R., & Curnoe, S. (2011). Psychopathy, ADHD, and brain dysfunction as predictors of lifetime recidivism among sex offenders. International journal of offender therapy and comparative criminology, 55(1), 5–26.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  110. ↵
    Lipsey, M.W., & Wilson, D.B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. London: SAGE.
  111. *.
    Llanes, S. J., & Kosson, D. S. (2006). Divided visual attention and left hemisphere activation among psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 28, 9–18.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  112. *.
    López, M., Kosson, D. S., Weissman, D. H., & Banich, M. T. (2007). Interhemispheric integration in psychopathic offenders. Neuropsychology, 21, 82–93.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  113. *.
    Lösel, F., & Schmucker, M. (2004). Psychopathy, risk taking, and attention: a differentiated test of the somatic marker hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 522–529.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  114. *.
    Lorenz, A. R., & Newman, J. P. (2002). Deficient response modulation and emotion processing in low-anxious Caucasian psychopathic offenders: Results from a lexical decision task. Emotion, 2, 91–104.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  115. ↵
    Lynn, R., & Mikk, J. (2009). National IQs predict educational attainment in math, reading and science across 56 nations. Intelligence, 37(3), 305–310.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  116. ↵
    Lynn, R. and Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQ and the wealth of nations. Westport, CT: Praeger.
  117. ↵
    Maes, J.H.R., & Brazil, I.A. (2013). No clear evidence for a positive association between the interpersonal-affective aspects of psychopathy and executive functioning. Psychiatry Research, 210, 1265–1274.
    OpenUrl
  118. *.
    Mayer, A. R., & Kosson, D. S. (2000). Handedness and psychopathy. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, & Behavioral Neurology, 13, 233–238.
    OpenUrl
  119. ↵
    McGurn, B., Starr, J. M., Topfer, J. A., Pattie, A., Whiteman, M. C., Lemmon, H. A., … & Deary, I. J. (2004). Pronunciation of irregular words is preserved in dementia, validating premorbid IQ estimation. Neurology, 62(7), 1184–1186.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  120. *.
    Mercer, K. D., Selby, M. J., & McClung, J. (2005). The effects of psychopathy, violence and drug use on neuropsychological functioning. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 23, 65–86.
    OpenUrl
  121. *.
    Mills, R. M. I. (1995). Cerebral asymmetry in psychopaths: A behavioral and electrocortical investigation. (doctoral dissertation), The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC.
  122. *.
    Mitchell, D., Colledge, E., Leonard, A., & Blair, R. (2002). Risky decisions and response reversal: Is there evidence of orbito-frontal cortex dysfunction in psychopathic individuals? Neuropsychologia, 40, 2013–2022.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  123. *.
    Mitchell, D., Richell, R. A., Leonard, A., & Blair, R. (2006). Emotion at the expense of cognition: Psychopathic individuals outperform controls on an operant response task. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 559–566.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  124. ↵
    Morgan, A. B., & Lilienfeld, S.O. (2000). A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 113–136.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  125. ↵
    Motzkin, J. C., Newman, J. P., Kiehl, K. A., & Koenigs, M. (2011). Reduced prefrontal connectivity in psychopathy. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(48), 17348–17357.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  126. ↵
    Muñoz, L. C., Frick, P. J., Kimonis, E. R., & Aucoin, K. J. (2008). Verbal ability and delinquency: Testing the moderating role of psychopathic traits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 414–421.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  127. ↵
    Murray, J., Irving, B., Farrington, D. P., Colman, I., & Bloxsom, C. A. (2010). Very early predictors of conduct problems and crime: results from a national cohort study. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 51(11), 1198–1207.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  128. *.
    Nestor, P. G., Kimble, M., Berman, I. Haycock, J. (2002). Psychosis, psychopathy, and homicide: A preliminary neuropsychological inquiry. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 138–140.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  129. *.
    Newman, J. P., Patterson, C. M., & Kosson, D. S. (1987). Response perseveration in psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 145–148.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  130. *.
    Newman, J. P., Schmitt, W. A., & Voss, W. D. (1997). The impact of motivationally neutral cues on psychopathic individuals: Assessing the generality of the response modulation hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 563–575.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  131. *.
    Nijman, H., Merckelbach, H., & Cima, M. (2009). Performance intelligence, sexual offending and psychopathy. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 15, 319–330.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  132. ↵
    Ogilvie, J.M., Stewart, A.L., Chan, R.C.K., & Shum, D.H.K. (2011). Neuropsychological measures of executive function and antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis. Criminology, 49, 1063–1107.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  133. ↵
    Patrick, C. J., Edens, J. F., Poythress, N. G., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Benning, S. D. (2006). Construct validity of the psychopathic personality inventory two-factor model with offenders. Psychological assessment, 18(2), 204–208.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  134. *.↵
    Pham, T., Philippot, P., & Rime, B. (2000). Subjective and autonomic responses to emotion induction in psychopaths. L‘Encephale: Revue de psychiatrie clinique biologique et therapeutique, 26, 45–51.
    OpenUrl
  135. *.
    Pham, T., Vanderstukken, O., Philippot, P., & Vanderlinden, M. (2003). Selective attention and executive functions deficits among criminal psychopaths. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 393–405.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  136. ↵
    Piquero, A. R., & White, N. A. (2003). On the relationship between cognitive abilities and life-course-persistent offending among a sample of African Americans: A longitudinal test of Moffitt’s hypothesis. Journal of Criminal Justice, 31(5), 399–409.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  137. ↵
    Plomin, R., & Deary, I. J. (2015). Genetics and intelligence differences: five special findings. Molecular psychiatry, 20(1), 98–108.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  138. ↵
    Porter, S., Brinke, L., & Wilson, K. (2009). Crime profiles and conditional release performance of psychopathic and non-psychopathic sexual offenders. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 14(1), 109–118.
    OpenUrl
  139. *.↵
    Raine, A., & Venables, P. H. (1988). Enhanced P3 evoked potentials and longer P3 recovery times in psychopaths. Psychophysiology, 25, 30–38.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  140. *.
    Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., LaCasse, L., & Colletti, P. (2000). Reduced prefrontal grey matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 119–127.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  141. *.
    Raine, A., Lencz, T., Taylor, K., Hellige, J. B., Bihrle, S., Lacasse, L. (2003). Corpus callosum abnormalities in psychopathic antisocial individuals. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 1134–1142.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  142. *.
    Raine, A., Ishikawa, S. S., Arce, E., Lencz, T., Knuth, K. H., Bihrle, S. (2004). Hippocampal structural asymmetry in unsuccessful psychopaths. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 185–191.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  143. ↵
    Reidy, D.E., Shelley-Tremblay, J.F., & Lilienfeld, S.O. (2011). Psychopathy, reactive aggression, and precarious proclamations: A review of behavioral, cognitive, and biological research. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16, 512–524.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  144. *.
    Richell, R., Mitchell, D., Newman, C., Leonard, A., Baron-Cohen, S., & Blair, R. (2003). Theory of mind and psychopathy: Can psychopathic individuals read the ‘language of the eyes’? Neuropsychologia, 41, 523–526.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  145. *.
    Richell, R., Mitchell, D., Peschardt, K., Winston, J., Leonard, A., Dolan, R. (2005). Trust and distrust: The perception of trustworthiness of faces in psychopathic and non-psychopathic offenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1735–1744.
    OpenUrl
  146. ↵
    Ritchie, S. (2015). Intelligence: All that matters. Hodder & Stoughton.
  147. ↵
    Ross, S. R., Moltó, J., Poy, R., Segarra, P., Pastor, M. C., & Montañés, S. (2007). Gray’s model and psychopathy: BIS but not BAS differentiates primary from secondary psychopathy in noninstitutionalized young adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(7), 1644–1655.
    OpenUrl
  148. ↵
    Salekin, R. T., Lee, Z., Schrum Dillard, C. L., & Kubak, F. A. (2010). Child psychopathy and protective factors: IQ and motivation to change. Psychology, public policy, and law, 16(2), 158–176.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  149. ↵
    Salekin, R. T., Neumann, C. S., Leistico, A. M. R., & Zalot, A. A. (2004). Psychopathy in youth and intelligence: An investigation of Cleckley’s hypothesis. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33(4), 731–742.
    OpenUrl
  150. ↵
    Salekin, R. T., Rogers, R., & Sewell, K. W. (1996). A review and meta-analysis of the Psychopathy Checklist and Psychopathy Checklist-Revised: Predictive validity of dangerousness. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 3(3), 203–215.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  151. ↵
    Salthouse, T. A. (2004). What and when of cognitive aging. Current directions in psychological science, 13(4), 140–144.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  152. ↵
    Schaie, K. W. (2005). What can we learn from longitudinal studies of adult development?. Research in human development, 2(3), 133–158.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  153. *.
    Schalling, D., & Rosen, A.S. (1968). Porteus maze differences between psychopathic and non-psychopathic criminlas. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7, 224–228.
    OpenUrl
  154. *.
    Schiffer, B., Pawliczek, C., Mu Ller, B., Forsting, M., Gizewski, E., Leygraf, N., Hodgins, S. (2014). Neural mechanisms underlying cognitive control of men with lifelong antisocial behavior. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 222, 43–51.
    OpenUrl
  155. *.
    Schmitt, W. A. (2000). Psychopathy and the response modulation hypothesis: Specifying the information processing deficiency. (doctoral dissertation), The University of Wisconsin - Madison, Wisconsin, WI.
  156. *.
    Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Harari, H., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Levkovitz, Y. (2010). The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in affective theory of mind deficits in criminal offenders with psychopathic tendencies. Cortex, 46, 668–677.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  157. *.
    Smith, S. S., Arnett, P. A., & Newman, J. P. (1992). Neuropsychological differentiation of psychopathic and nonpsychopathic criminal offenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 1233–1243.
    OpenUrl
  158. *.
    Smith, A. M. (1999). An fMRI investigation of frontal lobe functioning in psychopathy and schizophrenia during a go/no go task. (doctoral dissertation), The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC.
  159. *.
    Snowden, R. J., Gray, N. S., Smith, J., Morris, M., & MacCulloch, M. J. (2004). Implicit affective associations to violence in psychopathic murderers. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 15, 620–641.
    OpenUrl
  160. ↵
    Spearman, C. (1904). “General Intelligence,” objectively determined and measured. The American Journal ofPsychology, 15(2), 201–292.
    OpenUrl
  161. *.
    Sreenivasen, S., Walker, S.C., Weinberger, L.E., Kirkish, P. & Garrick, T. (2008). Four-facet PCL-R structure and cognitive functioning among high violent criminal offenders, Journal of Personality Assessment, 90, 197–200.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  162. *.
    Stevens, M. C., Kaplan, R. F., & Hesselbrock, V. M. (2003). Executive-cognitive functioning in the development of antisocial personality disorder. Addictive Behaviors, 28, 285–300.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  163. ↵
    Strenze, T. (2007). Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence, 35(5), 401–426.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  164. *.
    Suchy, Y., & Kosson, D. S. (2005). State-dependent executive deficits among psychopathic offenders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 311–321.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  165. *.
    Suchy, Y., & Kosson, D. S. (2006). Forming, switching, and maintaining mental sets among psychopathic offenders during verbal and nonverbal tasks: Another look at the left-hemisphere activation hypothesis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 538–548.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  166. *.
    Sutker, P. B., Moan, C. E., & Allain, A. N., Jr.. (1983). Assessment of cognitive control in psychopathic and normal prisons. Journal of Behavioral Assessment, 5, 275–287.
    OpenUrl
  167. *.
    Swogger, M. T. (2006). Risk taking in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders. (doctoral dissertation), Rosalind Franklin University of Medicine and Science, Chicago, IL.
  168. ↵
    Thompson, P. M., Cannon, T. D., Narr, K. L., Van Erp, T., Poutanen, V. P., Huttunen, M., … & Dail, R. (2001). Genetic influences on brain structure. Nature neuroscience, 4(12), 1253–1258.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  169. ↵
    Toga, A. W., & Thompson, P. M. (2005). Genetics of brain structure and intelligence. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 1–23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  170. ↵
    Ttofi, M. M., Farrington, D. P., Piquero, A. R., & DeLisi, M. (2016). Protective factors against offending and violence: Results from prospective longitudinal studies. Journal of Criminal Justice, 45, 1–3.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  171. ↵
    Vaughn, M. G., Edens, J. F., Howard, M. O., & Smith, S. T. (2009). Investigation of Primary and Secondary Psychopathy in a Statewide Sample of Incarcerated Youth. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 7(3), 172–188.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  172. *.
    Völlm, B., Richardson, P., McKie, S., Reniers, R., Elliott, R., Anderson, I. M. (2010). Neuronal correlates and serotonergic modulation of behavioural inhibition and reward in healthy and antisocial individuals. Journal ofPsychiatric Research, 44, 123–131.
    OpenUrl
  173. ↵
    Vitacco, M. J., Neumann, C. S., & Jackson, R. L. (2005). Testing a four-factor model of psychopathy and its association with ethnicity, gender, intelligence, and violence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 466–476.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  174. ↵
    Vitacco, M. J., Neumann, C. S., & Wodushek, T. (2008). Differential relationships between the dimensions of psychopathy and intelligence: replication with adult jail inmates. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 35, 48–55.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  175. ↵
    Wilson, J. Q., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1985). Crime and human behavior. New York: Simon & Schuster.
  176. *.↵
    Yang, Y., Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., LaCasse, L., & Colletti, P. (2005). Volume reduction in prefrontal grey matter in unsuccessful criminal psychopaths. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1103–1108.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  177. ↵
    Zakzanis, K.K. (2001). Statistics to tell the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth: Formulae, illustrative numerical examples, and heuristic interpretation of effect size analyses for neuropsychological researchers. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 653–667.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  178. *.
    Zeier, J. D., Maxwell, J. S., & Newman, J. P. (2009). Attention moderates the processing of inhibitory information in primary psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 554–563.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted January 17, 2017.
Download PDF
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
On the relationship between psychopathy and general intelligence: A meta-analytic review
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
On the relationship between psychopathy and general intelligence: A meta-analytic review
Olga Sanchez de Ribera, Nicholas Kavish, Brian Boutwell
bioRxiv 100693; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/100693
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
On the relationship between psychopathy and general intelligence: A meta-analytic review
Olga Sanchez de Ribera, Nicholas Kavish, Brian Boutwell
bioRxiv 100693; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/100693

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Neuroscience
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (2543)
  • Biochemistry (4994)
  • Bioengineering (3497)
  • Bioinformatics (15279)
  • Biophysics (6926)
  • Cancer Biology (5427)
  • Cell Biology (7771)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (4558)
  • Ecology (7180)
  • Epidemiology (2059)
  • Evolutionary Biology (10261)
  • Genetics (7532)
  • Genomics (9826)
  • Immunology (4899)
  • Microbiology (13304)
  • Molecular Biology (5165)
  • Neuroscience (29569)
  • Paleontology (203)
  • Pathology (842)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (1470)
  • Physiology (2153)
  • Plant Biology (4780)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (1015)
  • Synthetic Biology (1343)
  • Systems Biology (4022)
  • Zoology (771)