Abstract
The nervous system is notorious for its strong response evoked by a surprising sensory input, but the biophysical and anatomical underpinnings of this phenomenon are only partially understood. Here we utilized in-silico experiments of a biologically-detailed model of a neocortical microcircuit to study stimulus specific adaptation (SSA) in the auditory cortex, whereby the neuronal response adapts significantly for a repeated (“expected”) tone but not for a rare (“surprise”) tone. SSA experiments were mimicked by stimulating tonotopically-mapped thalamo-cortical afferents projecting to the microcircuit; the activity of these afferents was modeled based on our in-vivo recordings from individual thalamic neurons. The modeled microcircuit expressed naturally many experimentally-observed properties of SSA, suggesting that SSA is a general property of neocortical microcircuits. By systematically modulating circuit parameters, we found that key features of SSA depended on synergistic effects of synaptic depression, spike frequency adaptation and recurrent network connectivity. The relative contribution of each of these mechanisms in shaping SSA was explored, additional SSA-related experimental results were explained and new experiments for further studying SSA were suggested.
Introduction
Neurons in the primary auditory cortex, A1, exhibit a phenomenon called stimulus specific adaptation, SSA. When animals are presented with a tone sequence composed of a frequent tone (“standard”) and an occasional rare tone (“deviant”) the response of A1 neurons is markedly reduced to the standard tone, whereas their response to the same tone when deviant remains strong1–5 (see Nelken, 2014 for a definition and introduction to auditory SSA).
SSA is believed to emerge de novo in A1 as it is weak in the main thalamic input station to A1, the ventral medial geniculate body (MGBv)6,9. Therefore, A1 has become a prime focus for a number of experimental and theoretical studies of SSA5,10–13. Modeling studies suggest that SSA can emerge from feed-forward synaptic depression in the thalamocortical synapses13–16 and from local neocortical synaptic depression11,17,18. The models differ in their biological realism, ranging from population-based firing-rate models to spiking point-neuron-based models. Models which incorporate recurrent synaptic depression11 have the greatest success in replicating various properties of SSA, including the dependence of the magnitude of SSA on the frequency difference between the deviant and the standard, on the probability of the deviant, on the intertone interval, and on the input amplitude. These models also replicated the important property of specific deviance sensitivity – the larger response to a deviant tone (when the other tone presentations have a single expected tone frequency) than to the same tone, with the same probability, when the other tones vary in frequency. However, none of these models included the detailed cellular and synaptic diversity of neocortical microcircuits, nor did they replicate or explain the full repertoire of SSA without requiring specific parameter tuning. Indeed, we still lack a complete understanding of the underlying biophysical mechanisms of SSA. Towards this end, the present study uses a biologically-detailed dense computer reconstruction of a neocortical microcircuit, NMC19, to study SSA. This model, which has been extensively validated against anatomical and physiological experiments20–23 was challenged with the question: Does SSA as found in A1 emerge in this microcircuit without parameter adjustment? If it does, then such a model might have strong explanatory power for the underlying biophysical mechanisms of the phenomenon.
The NMC comprises a ∼0.3 mm3 volume of the neocortical tissue and is modeled after the rat somatosensory cortex, consisting of 6 layers, a total of ∼31,000 neurons, 55 morphological cell types, as well as the electrical properties of these neurons and of their synaptic connections (∼36 million synapses). The circuit was reconstructed based on the available experimental data and connectivity rules as described in 19 and 24. The model was not built in order to answer any specific a priori question, but to recreate the detailed anatomy and physiology of the NMC. Nevertheless, the model has been successfully used to perform in silico experiments that explore the anatomy and physiology of local neocortical circuits. For example, the model revealed the strong constraints of neuron morphology on local connectivity25 and how they result in non-random structures26, characterized the role of directed cliques of neurons in shaping neuronal responses27, and suggested mechanisms to suppress response variability caused by synaptic noise28.
We found that, when supplying the NMC with a biologically plausible auditory input via tonotopically-arranged thalamo-cortical axons, the model exhibited SSA with properties that mimicked many experimental observations. By modifying a variety of circuit parameters, we identified multiple mechanisms that may support SSA. In agreement with previous studies, synaptic depression of thalamo-cortical synapses as well as, to a lesser degree, depression of cortico-cortical synapses, shape SSA. Importantly, the model uncovered two additional features, spike frequency adaptation (SFA) and network connectivity that, even in the absence of synaptic depression, synergistically give rise to key features of SSA. This “replicate and explain” paradigm enabled us to explore the relative contribution of each of these mechanisms in determining the strength of SSA in the 6 neocortical layers and to replicated and explain other properties of SSA - its dependence on tone intensity29,30 and on neuron frequency preference31.
Results
Response to auditory surprise emerges naturally in the modeled thalamo-cortical circuit
The simulated dense neocortical microcircuit, NMC, consists of 31,346 neurons and ∼36 million intrinsic synapses (Fig. 1a). This microcircuit is innervated by 574 thalamo-cortical axons (Fig. 1b and see Methods), making a total of ∼ 2 million thalamo-cortical synapses. In this model, neurons in lower layer 3 and lower layer 5 receive the largest number of thalamo-cortical synapses19 in accordance with experimental data32. Each thalamo-cortical axon branches in the 3D volume of the NMC, creating multiple excitatory synapses on nearby cortical cells. An example of the postsynaptic cells receiving excitatory synaptic inputs from two thalamo-cortical axons (red and blue axons and respective colored cortical neurons) is shown in Fig. 1c. The hexagon at the bottom illustrates the tonotopic organization of the simulated thalamic input (see also hexagon at the middle of Fig. 1d).
To mimic auditory input impinging on the NMC from the thalamus, the time-dependent firing rate of the modeled thalamic axons was fitted to the rates measured experimentally in the auditory thalamus, MGBv (Supplementary Fig. 1 and see Methods). For each simulated tone presented, the instantaneous firing rate, f(t, s), of each axon followed an alpha function Eq. (1). f(t, s) depend on the tuning curve of the axon FR(s, b), which has a triangular waveform Eq. (2). In those functions, t is time, b is the axon’s best frequency, and s is the tone frequency (see Methods). The parameters for Eq. (1,2) were fitted such that the simulated response of the thalamic axons to specific tone frequencies mimicked the experimentally measured response at tone intensity of 60 dB (Methods and Supplementary Fig. 1). Two exemplar tuning curves are shown in Fig. 1d, top and bottom frames at left, with their corresponding instantaneous firing rate shown respectively at right (f(t, s) for 2 input frequency; s). The modeled thalamic axons were organized in a tonotopic manner such that their best frequency spans 2 octaves over 560 μm (Fig. 1d hexagon in the middle, see details in Methods), in accordance with experimental findings33,34. Axons with low best frequency are marked by a bluish color and axons with high best frequency with a reddish color (color code scale at right).
Using the model illustrated in Figure 1, we next simulated the oddball experimental paradigm1,13 as follows. We selected two frequencies, f1 = 6,666 Hz and f2 = 9,600 Hz, that are centered (on log scale) around 8,000 Hz and presented them to the NMC with a 300 ms inter-stimulus interval. In one case, f1 served as a standard tone (presented 95% of the time) interspersed randomly 5% of the time with f2, the deviant tone. In the second case, the roles of f1 and f2 were reversed (see Methods and13). We focused our analysis on excitatory neurons located within ± 25 μm from the center of the simulated microcircuit (“center microcolumn”, see green neurons in Fig. 4a). These cortical neurons receive most of their thalamic synapses from axons whose best frequency is ∼8,000 Hz (green axons in the hexagon at Fig. 1d).
We found that the neurons in the center microcolumn responded significantly more to the deviant tone than to the same tone when standard (Fig. 2a1 and a2, red bars vs. blue bars). This behavior is the hallmark of the stimulus-specific adaptation1, showing that the model indeed succeeds to replicate the phenomenon. We validated that the center microcolumn is not selective to tone identity by showing that the cells respond equally to f1 and f2 when they are presented with equal probability (Fig. 2a3; the “equal paradigm”14, see additional quantification in Supplementary Fig. 2).
We next quantified the SSA in single excitatory cortical neurons; two examples are shown, one for Layer 2/3 pyramidal cell and one for Layer 5 pyramidal cell (Fig. 2b1-b4). In both neurons, the lower was the probability of the presented frequency, the higher was the response of the neuron (Supplementary Fig. 2). This behavior matched the results found in vivo1,14,35. To quantify the degree of SSA, we computed the common-contrast SSA index CSI1,13 (see Methods) for each neuron. CSI = 0 indicates that there is no difference in the response to the two frequencies when standard and deviant (Fig. 2c), while positive values indicate a stronger responses to at least one of the two tones when deviant. The CSI of most neurons was positive and so was the mean across layers (thick line). Note that the largest CSI was at middle layers and lower layer 6 which, interestingly, received relatively weak thalamic input (Fig. 1b and see Discussion). The average CSI value across all layers was 0.49 ± 0.29 (mean ± std). We also computed the SSA index (see Methods) for f1 (SI1) and for f2 (SI2) and plotted them in Fig. 2d against each other. Note that most values fall in the first quadrant, implying that the neurons are more responsive to the deviant than to the standard, irrespective of the tone frequency. The mean SSA index value was 0.47 ± 0.33 and 0.53 ± 0.39 for SI1 and SI2, respectively (red circle Fig. 2d). A summary of the SSA in the microcircuit for the present paradigm together with an additional stimulus paradigm is presented in Supplementary Fig. 3 showing that, without any parameter tuning, the NMC model replicates rather successfully diverse aspects of SSA. Specifically, that the response of the neurons is significantly higher to the deviant than to the standard tone1,3–5,13,14,29,31,35, that this difference depends on the probability of the deviant1,14,35, and that the response to the deviant is larger than the response to the same tone when it is presented among many tones with densely-packed frequencies - the ‘diverse narrow’ paradigm4,13,14. Furthermore, in some cases, the response for a rarely presented tone (‘deviant alone’ or ‘rare’) were smaller than the responses to the same tone presented as a deviant accompanied by a standard tone4,13,14. However, Supplementary Fig. 3d-f shows that other experimental feature of SSA was not replicated by our model (see Discussion).
Uncovering the neuronal mechanisms underlying SSA
The contribution of synaptic depression and spike-frequency adaptation to SSA
As our dense replica of a cortical microcircuit together with its biologically constrained set of mechanisms replicated many of the properties of SSA, we next used it to uncover the biophysical mechanisms underlying this phenomenon. Synaptic depression is widely considered as the fundamental mechanism underlying SSA. In the NMC it is modeled as a short-term reduction of the probability of a spike triggering synaptic transmission, as described in the Tsodyks-Markram model36. We therefore first removed this effect by fixing the release probability (both in the cortico-cortical and the thalamo-cortical synapses) of all excitatory depressing synapses (see Methods). Following this manipulation, the SSA was indeed reduced (compare Fig. 3a to 3b). The mean CSI was reduced from 0.49 ± 0.29 (mean ± std) to 0.16 ± 0.2 (a 67% reduction) whereas the mean SI1 and SI2 indices changed from 0.47 ± 0.33 and 0.53 ± 0.39 to 0.16 ± 0.29 and 0.14 ± 0.33, respectively (a 69% reduction, compare red circle in insets in Fig. 3a to that of Fig. 3b). However, surprisingly, blocking synaptic depression did not completely eliminate SSA (Fig. 3b), implying that additional mechanisms support SSA in the model.
We therefore systematically examined the effect of a broad range of model parameters on the emergence of SSA. Including removing from the model a variety of membrane ion channels, simulating only some, out of the six, cortical layers, removing synaptic facilitation, etc. Eventually, we found that, when removing both synaptic depression and spike frequency adaptation (SFA), the SSA was completely abolished (Fig. 3d). SFA was removed by blocking Ca2+-dependent potassium channels from all modeled cells (see Methods, Supplementary Fig. 4 and Discussion). Like synaptic depression, removing SFA alone reduced SSA but did not abolish it (Fig. 3c). Indeed, in this case, the mean CSI value was reduced from 0.49 ± 0.29 to 0.21 ± 0.29 (by 57% reduction). This results clearly shows that SSA emerges due to at least two mechanisms, synaptic depression and spike frequency adaptation, operating synergistically. The interaction between these two mechanisms is supralinear; while the average CSI value was 0.16 without synaptic depression (but with SFA) and 0.21 without SFA (but with synaptic depression), it was 0.49 when both mechanisms are present. For 1,719 out of 2,378 neurons, the summed CSI was smaller than the CSI for both mechanisms simultaneously (Supplementary Fig. 5).
The summary of the above results is depicted in Fig. 3e where the mean CSI value across cortical layers for the different conditions is shown. Note that, synaptic depression alone generates SSA (light green bars), and SFA alone generates SSA (orange bars). When the two mechanisms are active together, the SSA is larger (dark green bars). A detailed exploration of the origin of the differences in SSA among the different cortical layers is elaborated in the Discussion.
We next explored the contribution of cortico-cortical versus thalamo-cortical synaptic depression to SSA. We found that, in our model, thalamo-cortical synaptic depression has a much stronger impact on SSA than cortico-cortical synaptic depression. (Compare Supplementary Fig. 6 first column, lower left frame to the frame in 2nd row and 2nd column). When only cortico-cortical depression is active (i.e., without SFA and thalamo-cortical synaptic depression) cortical neurons have a wide tuning curve. This is because, in this condition, the cortical neurons do not adapt (no SFA) and, on top of it, there is no thalamo-cortical synaptic depression37. Consequently, cortical neurons that are selective to, for example, f1 also respond strongly to f2. When f1 arrives as the deviant, their cortico-cortical synapses are already significantly depressed. In contrast, thalamic axons are narrowly tuned (responding selectively either to f1 or to f2). Consequently, in this condition, thalamic axons that are selective to f1 will respond weakly to f2. When f1 arrives as the deviant their thalamo-cortical synapses are only weakly depressed, resulting in a strong response to the deviant tone and, thus, with a strong SSA. Exploration of additional combinations of different mechanisms underlying SSA is elaborated in Supplementary Fig. 6.
Emergence of SSA due to spike frequency adaptation
Figure 3b shows that, even in the absence of synaptic depression, SSA still persists in the microcircuit due to SFA (Fig. 3d). In the absence of synaptic depression, the cells consisting of the “center microcolumn”, i.e., cells that have no thalamo-cortical preference for either of the two stimuli (green cells in Fig. 4a), receive the same number of active thalamic inputs when f1 or f2 are presented. Therefore, on the face of it, these cells should respond in the same manner (and similarly adapt due to SFA) to f1 and f2. However, these “green neurons” also receive cortico-cortical inputs that affect their responses to f1 and f2. To test whether these cortico-cortical inputs mediate the effects of SFA on SSA, we removed all cortico-cortical synapses from the model. This manipulation, on top of removing synaptic depression (but leaving SFA intact), abolished SSA in these neurons (Supplementary Fig. 6 first row in the 3rd column). Thus, cortico-cortical connectivity was required in order for SFA to contribute to SSA.
To explore the role of cortico-cortical connectivity, we examined the responses of two additional microcolumns (blue and yellow cells in Fig. 4a) in the SSA protocol, in the absence of synaptic depression. The blue microcolumn consists of neurons that are selective for f1 (6,666 Hz); the yellow microcolumn is selective for f2 (9,600 Hz). The green microcolumn whose preferred frequency lies midway between f1 and f2 (8,000 Hz). When f2 was the standard, the f2-selective (“yellow” cells) adapted due to SFA (compare Supplementary Fig. 3c, “deviant alone” green bar, to the “standard”, blue bar). As a result, their feedforward effect on the green cells was small. When f1 was presented as deviant (in the same sequence in which f2 was standard), the f1-selective cells (“blue” cells) respond strongly (because they were not adapted by the many presentations of f2; Fig. 4b1, top). This effect on yellow and blue cells does not require cortico-cortical connectivity (Fig. 4b1 vs. 4b2, top), but in order for them to affect the activity of the green cells, cortico-cortical connections are required (Fig. 4b1 vs. 4b2, middle and 4b3 vs. 4b4). Thus, in this condition, the green cells responded more strongly to the deviant (f1) than to the standard (f2). The reverse also holds for f2 as deviant and f1 as standard (Fig. 4c1, middle row, red trace is higher than the blue traces). This explains how SFA in conjunction with recurrent network connectivity causes SSA in the green cells. This cascade is illustrated with the animation in Supplementary Video 1. Remarkably, this situation recapitulates essential elements of the simplified model for SSA that is based on adaptation of narrowly tuned inputs13,14.
In conclusion, we note that, while, classically, synaptic depression was considered to be the major mechanism responsible for SSA, our dense in silico neocortical microcircuit model, receiving tonotopically-mapped thalamic input, enabled us to demonstrate that at least two additional mechanisms (SFA combined with network connectivity) are involved in shaping cortical SSA. Our study also shows that in our model SSA is primarily a network phenomenon because, when the network is disconnected, the thalamo-cortical input per se (and its respective synaptic depression) gives rise to only a small SSA response and only in neurons that receive thalamo-cortical inputs (Supplementary Fig. 6 first row in 2nd column).
Additional properties of SSA explained via the reconstructed microcircuit
We next attempted to replicate, using the NMC model, and mechanistically explain other experimentally observed properties of SSA. One such property is the dependency of SSA on the amplitude (intensity) of the presented tones. It was found experimentally that stronger tones result in lower CSI29,30. We replicated this experiment by setting the firing rate of the modeled thalamic axons so that it mimics the experimental response to different tone intensities (Sound Pressure Levels, SPL, Fig. 5a, Supplementary Fig. 1, and Methods). Indeed, as found experimentally, the microcircuit showed a decrease in CSI with the increase in tone intensity (Fig. 5b). When examining the frequency-response area (FRA) of thalamic axons (Fig. 5a), we observe that two main features of the tuning curve change with tone intensity. The higher the tone intensity, the higher is the firing rate of the thalamic axons and the wider is their tuning curve (Fig. 5a and Supplementary Fig. 1). To separate the effect of each of these two parameters on CSI we manipulated, in the model, either the tuning width of the thalamic axons (Fig. 5c) or their maximal firing rate (Fig. 5d), leaving all other variables unchanged (see Methods). The range of values used for testing the effects of the width of the tuning curve (Fig. 5c) and of the firing rate (Fig. 5d) was within the experimental range (Supplementary Fig. 1). Both the increase in firing rate and in tuning width resulted in a decrease in the CSI, but the change in the tuning width had a much greater effect (compare Fig. 5c to 5d).
Next, we utilized the circuit to uncover which of the two biophysical mechanisms (synaptic depression or SFA) is responsible for the reduction in CSI with increasing tone intensity. To achieve this, we replicated the protocol of Fig. 5b, once when removing SFA and once when blocking synaptic depression (Fig. 5e). Both manipulations resulted in a comparable reduction of the CSI with an increase in tone intensity (Fig. 5e), implying that both mechanisms are involved in the dependence of CSI on tone intensity. This is to be expected, because when tone intensity is increased, the thalamo-cortical afferents selective to the deviant also start responding to the standard due to the increase in tuning width. As a consequence, both synaptic adaptation and SFA will reduce the strength of the response to the deviant as a consequence of exposure to the standard. Consequently, in cases with high tone-intensity, the difference in response between a standard and a deviant is smaller (smaller CSI).
Finally, we explored the experimental finding that the strength of the SSA depends on the frequency preference of the cortical neurons31. Indeed, the tuning curve of neurons in the auditory cortex is typically not symmetrical and, therefore, the neurons do not respond with the same firing rate to the two tones (f1 and f2) that are centered around their best frequency10,13,29,31. Chen et al. (2015) reported that, when using the oddball paradigm, the SI of the tone that gave rise to the lower firing rate (the “Non-Preferred” frequency SINP; e.g., f2,) was lower than the SI of the tone that gave rise to the higher firing rate (the “Preferred” frequency SIP; e.g., f1; See Fig. 5 in31). We independently verified this result by analyzing the dataset accompanying Nieto-Diego et al., 2016 (see Methods). We found that SIP is significantly larger than SINP (0.221 ± 0.035 versus 0.11 ± 0.037, n = 31; p = 0.032 t-test; mean ± SEM). Fig. 6a, b shows that this also occurs in our simulated microcircuit. We first plotted the firing rate of the excitatory cortical neurons as a function of the tonotopic axis (Fig. 6a). As expected, cortical neurons that are closer to the thalamic axons with f1 as the BF (black circle) have a higher firing rate for f1 (black line in Fig. 6a) and vice versa for f2 (grey line in Fig. 6a). Fig. 6b shows that, as in the experiment, the larger the firing rate of a neuron to f2 relative to f1 the larger the SI2 is (and vice versa for SI1, not shown). Together, Fig. 6a and 6b correspond to the experimental finding that the tone that gives rise to the lower firing rate has lower SI value.
This finding can be understood by considering the involvement of SFA in the emergence of SSA (Fig. 3d). In the absence of cortico-cortical connectivity, SFA may cause a neuron that is selective to f1 to respond more strongly to f2 as standard than to f2 as deviant (low SI2). This may happen when f1 is highly effective in driving the neuron, and therefore, when standard, the firing of this neuron would be strongly adapted. Consequently, when f2 occurs as a deviant, the response of the neuron is small. If f2 is much less effective in driving the neuron, SFA would affect the neuronal responses to a lesser extent and the neuron would respond more strongly to f2 under this condition. Thus, for this cell, SI2 is expected to be low and possibly even negative. We tested that prediction by removing cortico-cortical connections and synaptic depression from the model and found that indeed neurons that are more selective to f1 have lower SI2 (Fig. 6c). Furthermore, we found that, when blocking SFA, while leaving all other mechanisms intact, the dependence of SI2 on the frequency preference of the neuron vanishes (Fig. 6d).
Discussion
This work proposes a general approach, which could be termed “replicate and explain”, for linking experimentally-observed (emergent) circuit responses to their underlying anatomical and biophysical properties. This approach utilizes a generic dense computer replica of the circuit studied as an “experimental” tool that incorporates a broad range of biophysical and anatomical details (membrane ion channels, synaptic and firing dynamics, cell and synapse types, dendritic morphology, cortical layers, etc.). Yet, this circuit model was not built to capture any a priori specific high-level phenomenon. We have used the “replicate and explain” approach to study SSA in the auditory system, utilizing the cortical microcircuit model of young rat developed by Markram et al., (2015), and adding to this circuit tonotopically mapped thalamo-cortical axons33,34 together with modeling the response of these axons to pure tones based on our own unpublished experimental data (Supplementary Fig. 1). This dense circuit replicated a substantial amount (but not all, see below) of the experimental properties of SSA, without tuning of the underlying modeling parameters; it provided mechanistic explanations for the emergence of SSA and related phenomena, together with a set of experimentally-testable predictions. The study suggested that a significant part of SSA is not a special property of the whole brain in vivo, nor is it specific to the auditory cortex and it has provided a set of new mechanisms that support SSA.
Our “replicate and explain” approach differs from the typical computational route taken to study circuit dynamics, whereby simplified circuit models are constructed to study the dependence of an observed phenomenon, e.g., SSA, on unobserved biophysical parameters38,39, for example, synaptic depression. While this classical modeling approach is invaluable for testing specific mechanisms that might explain the observed phenomena, the model is a priori tailored to replicate the phenomenon and, by design, it ignores other putative underlying mechanisms (and the interactions among them) that might impact the studied phenomena40.
In agreement with previous theoretical suggestions, we found that thalamo-cortical synaptic depression (and to a lesser degree also cortico-cortical synaptic depression) plays a key role in generating SSA13,15. Yet, removing synaptic depression from the modeled circuit uncovered additional mechanisms responsible for SSA. Namely, a synergetic “collaborative” interaction between spike frequency adaptation, SFA, at the cellular level and cortico-cortical network connections. SFA was previously proposed as a putative mechanism underlying SSA41, but its impact at the network level for generating SSA was not demonstrated. We have shown that SFA, in the absence of network connectivity, does not generate SSA in neurons that are equally selective to f1 and f2 (Fig. 4), whereas in neurons that are not equally selective to f1 and f2, SFA results with positive SI for the frequency (say f1) that the neuron is selective for and negative or zero for f2 (Fig. 6). This is in contrast to the experimental observation, whereby both SI1 and SI2 were positive13. One key experimentally testable prediction from this study is that SSA will be significantly weakened by blocking ion channels responsible for SFA (i.e., the Ca2+-dependent K channels, which could be blocked by albumin42). This study thus shows how a computation (in our case of a “surprise response”) can be generated in networks receiving spatially mapped sensory input (e.g., tonotopic input, retinotopic input) via SFA mechanism.
In accordance with experimental results3,43 we found that SSA was weaker in the thalamo-recipient layers (lower layers 3 and 5 in our model, see Fig. 1b and 2c). In the thalamo-recipient layers the standard tone generated a strong response and a weaker response in the other layers. When the deviant arrives, neurons in all layers increase their firing rate by approximately the same amount. This implies that the CSI (and thus SSA) will be lower in the thalamo-cortical recipient layers (see Methods for CSI definition). Note also that neurons in the non-recipient layers (that respond weakly to a standard tone) are less adapted and this allows them to increase their response more significantly to the deviant tone. Our “replicate and explain” methodology explained two additional SSA-related experimental phenomena. First, by modeling in vivo spike time recordings of MGBv neurons responding to pure tones at various sound pressure levels (SPL), we tested the oddball paradigm at different SPL. We found that, as in the experiments, the larger the tone intensity the lower the SSA29,30. The main reason for this effect is the widening of the thalamic axon tuning curve with tone intensity which, in turn, affects the capability of both SFA and synaptic depression to generate SSA. Second, we found that, in agreement with the experiments, SSA depended on the frequency preference of the neurons31 and showed that SFA is the main mechanism responsible for this dependency (Fig. 6).
Our model did not succeed in replicating the experimental distribution of SSA values in different cortical layers (compare Fig. 2c to Fig, 7 in 3) nor did it replicated an important property of SSA in primary auditory cortex: its specific deviance sensitivity. In auditory cortex, responses to a tone when deviant (accompanied by a standard) are comparable in size and sometimes larger than the responses to the same tone, occurring at the same probability, when accompanied by many widely-spaced, tones. This is the ‘control’ condition of the mismatch negativity in EEG literature8 and the ‘diverse broad’ condition in13,14. In our model, the responses to a deviant frequency were almost always smaller than the response to this frequency in the ‘diverse broad’ condition (see Supplementary Fig. 3d-f). Thus, the deviant responses, while larger than standard responses, are still too small to fit the experimentally measured ones. This is actually also the major failure of previous models that were based on adaptation of narrowly-tuned inputs6,13,14. This might be the result of our ability to model only six tones in the diverse broad condition, compared to the twelve tones that are used experimentally. We also note that this response depends on the location of the neurons, where neurons with BS close to the deviant frequency show a deviant response that is larger than the response to the ‘diverse broad’ (Supplementary Fig. 3e), although only for one frequency. Further modeling efforts are required to solve this puzzling experimental result.
With the increase in computer power we are able, for the first time, to connect via a dense computer replica of neuronal circuits, the subcellular (ion channels and synapses), the cellular (cell types) and the network (connectivity) levels, and obtain a new and lucid picture of the working of the brain at multitude levels of operation44. This will enable to resolve other long-standing open questions such as the emergence of “salt and pepper” organization in sensory systems of rodents45–48 and the impact of different excitatory and/or inhibitory cell types on network dynamics5,49–51. Furthermore, this approach could be scaled-up for asking as broader questions as we will soon have detailed computer replicas of the rodent hippocampus, cerebellum, thalamus, as well as a detailed model of the whole rodent neocortex with full thalamo-cortical connections52–55. Then we might explore, and provide mechanistic explanations, for a wide range of in vivo multiscale phenomena such as EEG signals (and its modulation, e.g., due to a “surprise”) which, after more than 100 years since they were discovered, are only partially understood
Methods
Dense model of neocortical microcircuit (NMC)
We performed simulations of electrical activity on a previously published model of a neocortical microcircuit of a two-week-old rat. Reconstruction of the circuits and its simulation methods were described in Markram et al.19. Briefly, this microcircuit (Fig. 1a) consisted of 31,346 biophysical Hodgkin-Huxley NEURON models and around 7.8 million synaptic connections forming around 36.4 million synapses. Synaptic connectivity between 55 distinct morphological types of neurons (m-types) was predicted algorithmically and constrained by experimental data 24. The densities of ion-channels on morphologically-detailed neuron models were optimized to reproduce the behavior of different electrical neuron types (e-types) and synaptic dynamics recorded in vitro56. Simulations were run on HPE SGI 8600 supercomputer (BlueBrain V) using NEURON57 and CoreNEURON58. NEURON models and the connectome are available online at bbp.epfl.ch/nmc-portal59. In Supplementary Fig. 3d-f a larger circuit was simulated (see below).
Simulation of baseline spontaneous activity
To account for the missing long-range connections and missing neuromodulators, neurons were depolarized with a somatic current injection of 85% of first spike threshold. In addition, synapses spontaneous release probability was set to mimic an in vivo like state. As described by Markram et al.19, the use parameter of synaptic transmission and the spontaneous release probability were modulated according to extracellular calcium concentration ([Ca2+]o). In all our simulations [Ca2+]o was set to 1.23mM (see also 28). Synaptic conductances and kinetics are as in Markram et al.19. For the excitatory, synapses, the NMDA component was revised such that the “steepness” factor, γ, of the NMDA conductance was set to 0.0860–62.
Simulation of realistic auditory input delivered to the NMC
Auditory input was simulated using 574 reconstructed thalamic axons19 that were activated tonotopically as follows: Each thalamic axon had x, y coordinates which correspond to the center of its projection to the NMC. The x-coordinate spans from 0 to 560 µm and was denoted as the tonotopic axis (Fig. 1d). The best frequency, BF, of each axon corresponds linearly to its x-coordinate, such that BF of 4,000 Hz corresponded to x = 0 µm, and BF of 16,000 Hz to x = 560 µm. This process resulted in 2 octaves difference along the x-axis33,34,63,64.
To set the firing-rate dynamics of the thalamic axons so that it replicates the firing-rate dynamics of the rodent auditory thalamic nucleus, we first analyzed in vivo recordings of rat ventral division of the medial geniculate body (MGBv), (see below). We examined the response of MGBv neurons to pure tones at frequencies ranging from 1 kHz to 64 kHz (steps of 0.166 octaves) and at amplitudes ranging from 40 to 90 dB Sound Pressure Levels (SPL). Out of 26 MGBv neurons recorded experimentally, 17 had a V-shaped frequency response area (FRA) when measured in the first 50 ms after tone presentation (see example in Supplementary Fig. 1a left). We then used the instantaneous firing rate of each of these 17 MGBv neurons as the target for the fit using Eq. (1), which is a standard alpha function (see example in Supplementary Fig. 1b and see also 65). The other 9 of the axons were either non-responsive to tone presentation or had a highly irregular tuning curve. Their mean firing-rate was 1.5 Hz.
Where s is the presented frequency and t is time. The parameters for this alpha function are: b - the neuron best frequency; ∑ - maximum firing rate; µ - width of tuning curve; τ - response time constant. The frequency-dependent delay (delay(s,b)) had a Gaussian waveform (e.g., the delay is shortest when s = b) with the following parameters: baseline - the maximal delay; norm - the max delay difference; σ - the Gaussian standard deviation. These parameters were fitted with SciPy66 least-square function (using the default parameters) with the target of minimizing ∑s f(t, s) − MGBv(t, s). This process resulted in a distribution for each of parameter, for each tone amplitude (Supplementary Fig. 1c-h). We used these parameter distributions to create the spike times of the thalamic axons.
Because 9 out of 26 MGBv axons did not have a characteristic V-shape response, we started by randomly selecting 40% of the modeled thalamic axons and activated them spontaneously at the rate to 1.5 Hz (see above). For each of the remaining (60%) thalamic axons we randomly sampled the parameters ∑ and τ from a Gaussian distribution that fitted the corresponding parameter distribution in Supplementary Fig. 1c-h and µ from a 2-parameter exponential distributed that fitted its distribution (Supplementary Fig. 1c-h). The baseline, norm, σ that were selected for the delay function were that of the mean of the corresponding parametric distribution (Supplementary Fig. 1c-h). Using this sampling process, we satisfy all parameters for f(t, s). Then, for each tone presentation, the spike times of the axon were generated from an inhomogeneous Poisson process with time-varying rate f(t, s) + 1.5 Hz.
In Figure 5c,d all the parameters for each axon were sampled from the parameter distribution for an input intensity of 60 dB (Supplementary Fig. 1e) except for the location parameter of the exponential distribution for the tuning width (Supplementary Fig. 5c) and for the mean of the Gaussian distribution for the max firing rate (Supplementary Fig. 5d).
The FRA in Figure 5a was constructed by creating a thalamic axon with the average values of ∑, τ and µ for each tone amplitude, and averaging the first 50 ms of its response to different tone presentations.
SSA protocols
We attempted to replicate as closely as possible the in vivo experimental protocols used to explore SSA in rodent auditory cortex4,5,13,14,29,35,67. To achieve this, two tones f1 = 6,666 Hz and f2 = 9,600 Hz were selected as the base frequencies for our simulations; these frequencies are separated by 0.52 octaves around the center frequency of the simulated cortical circuit (8,000 Hz); see Fig. 1d and Fig. 4a. Each simulated experiment started with 2 seconds without an auditory tone followed by 500 tone presentations, with an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 300 ms. We presented tones in five paradigms, described here. The oddball paradigm, where the standard tone was presented at p = 95% (unless specified otherwise) and the deviant tone was presented with p = 5%. The order of the tone presentations was random. In the equal paradigm, each tone was presented 250 times in a random order. In the deviant alone paradigm, the deviant timing was identical to the deviant timing in oddball paradigm, but the standard tone was not presented at all. In the diverse narrow paradigm, 20 tones equally distributed between log2(f1) − 0.19 to log2(f2) + 0.19 were each presented 25 times in a random order. In the diverse broad paradigm – 6 tones equally distributed between log2(f1) − 1.05 to log2(f2) + 1.05. For simulating this condition (Supplementary Fig. 3d), we simulated a 3D-slice of cortical circuit containing ∼103,207 neurons consisting of 1000 µm along the tonotopic (x-axis) by 465 µm width (y-axis) and with same depth (z-axis) as in as in Fig. 1a. This 3D-slice was taken from the full 7-column circuit in Markram et al.19.
Data analysis
The analysis of SSA response in the circuit was conducted on neurons at ± 25 µm about the center of the simulated circuit. These neurons have a best frequency of around 8,000 Hz and respond similarly to f1 and f2 when each is presented alone (see Supplementary Fig. 3b1). Unless stated otherwise, common-contrast SSA Index (CSI) measurements (and SI1, SI2; see below) are shown only for neurons with a firing rate larger than 0.15 Hz. For the oddball paradigm, we compared the neuron response (50 ms from tone presentation) for 24 randomly selected standards (f1s or f2s) to their response for 24 deviants (f1d or f2d). We discarded the first response to a standard and a deviant tone
Manipulation of synaptic depression
Synapses in the microcircuit are separated into three types of short-term synaptic plasticity: facilitating, depressing, and pseudo-linear. The dynamics of these synapses is governed by the respective set of time constants19. To remove short time synaptic plasticity from a given synaptic connection, we set the facilitation and depression time constants to 0 while keeping the release probability constant.
Manipulation of spike frequency adaptation
Excitatory neuron in the microcircuit contains calcium-activated potassium channels (SK_E2, see also 68,69). This hyperpolarizing current was shown to be involved in generating spike frequency adaptation, SFA70. To remove the SFA from the microcircuit we abolished this current by removing all SK_E2 channels from all modeled compartments.
Analysis of previously published data
The experimental plots in Supplementary Fig. 4c (black and gray lines) were digitally reconstructed from Fig. 3d in Abolafia et al., 2011, and overlaid on the axes of the simulated results.
The experimental data from Taaseh, 2010, in Fig. 5b (purple line) was digitally extracted (5 median points) from Fig. 5d of Yarden and Nelken11
The experimental data from Nieto-Diego et al.29, in Fig. 5b (blue line) was created by analyzing the dataset accompanying the article, S1 Data Oddball (MUA). We extracted all SSA results of A1 units and calculated the median CSI as a function of SPL.
We used the same dataset to calculate SIP and SINP. For each neuron that was recorded in SPL>50, we compared its firing rate response to f1 and to f2 as standards. The SI of the frequency that gave rise to the larger response was defined as SIP whereas the SI of the frequency that gave rise to the smaller response was defined as SINP.
Visualization
Network visualizations were created in Fig. 1b using RTNeuron71 and in Fig. 1c using Mayavi72. Single neuron morphology visualizations (Fig. 2b) were created using NeuroMorphoVis73. Figures were created using Matplotlib74.
Experimental procedures
The thalamic inputs were modeled based on unpublished data recorded in rat MGB, courtesy of Leila Khouri and Maciej Jankowski. All experiments were performed according to protocols approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Hebrew University. Hebrew University is an AAALAC approved institution. Animal preparation was as in Taaseh et al., 2011. Following the craniotomy, an electrode array (16 or 32 tungsten electrodes, Alpha-Omega) was introduced to the MGB using stereotaxic coordinates. The data analyzed here consisted of responses to pure tones of 37 different frequencies (1,000 Hz to 32,000 Hz, 6 tones/octave, 50 ms long with 10 ms linear onset and offset ramps, 2/s). Each tone was repeated 10 times at each sound level. Sound levels covered the range between 20-30 dB SPL to 90 dB SPL at 10 dB steps. All data were stored as disk files and analyzed offline (Matlab, Mathworks). Following the experiment, the rats were perfused transcardially, the brains removed and used for histological verification of the recording locations. Units with large spikes were isolated using a unit-specific threshold. Only units with clear significant responses to tones were used.
Funding
This study received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Framework Program for Research and Innovation under the Specific Grant Agreement No. 785907 (Human Brain Project SGA2), the ETH domain for the Blue Brain Project, the Gatsby Charitable Foundation, the NIH Grant Agreement U01MH114812 and support by a personal ISF grant (1126/18).
Competing financial interests
Authors declare no competing financial interests.
Data and materials availability
All data will be published upon publication
Authors Contribution
O.A. and I.S. conceived the study. O.A., I.S, I.N, M.R and H.M wrote the manuscript. O.A. carried out the simulations and the analysis. I.N contributed the experimental data. H.M., S.R., J.K., M.R, E.M, O.A. and I.S developed the in silico microcircuit and provided the respective simulation data.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgments
We thank Pramod Kumbhar for the dedicated technical support. We thank all members of the Segev labs for many fruitful discussions and valuable feedback regarding this work. We thank Leila Khouri and Maciej Jankowski for the experimental data.