Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

No Evidence for Long-range Male Sex Pheromones in Two Malaria Mosquitoes

Serge Bèwadéyir Poda, View ORCID ProfileBruno Buatois, Benoit Lapeyre, Laurent Dormont, Abdoulaye Diabaté, Olivier Gnankiné, Roch K. Dabiré, View ORCID ProfileOlivier Roux
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.05.187542
Serge Bèwadéyir Poda
1Institut de Recherche en Sciences de la Santé (IRSS), Bobo-Dioulasso, Burkina Faso
2Laboratoire d’Entomologie Fondamentale et Appliquée (LEFA), Unité de Formation et de Recherche en Sciences de la Vie et de la Terre (UFR-SVT), Université Joseph KI-ZERBO, Ouagadougou, Burkina Faso
3MIVEGEC, Univ. Montpellier, IRD, CNRS, Montpellier, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Bruno Buatois
4CEFE, Univ. Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, Univ. Paul Valéry Montpellier 3, Montpellier, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Bruno Buatois
Benoit Lapeyre
4CEFE, Univ. Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, Univ. Paul Valéry Montpellier 3, Montpellier, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Laurent Dormont
4CEFE, Univ. Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, Univ. Paul Valéry Montpellier 3, Montpellier, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Abdoulaye Diabaté
1Institut de Recherche en Sciences de la Santé (IRSS), Bobo-Dioulasso, Burkina Faso
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Olivier Gnankiné
2Laboratoire d’Entomologie Fondamentale et Appliquée (LEFA), Unité de Formation et de Recherche en Sciences de la Vie et de la Terre (UFR-SVT), Université Joseph KI-ZERBO, Ouagadougou, Burkina Faso
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Roch K. Dabiré
1Institut de Recherche en Sciences de la Santé (IRSS), Bobo-Dioulasso, Burkina Faso
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Olivier Roux
1Institut de Recherche en Sciences de la Santé (IRSS), Bobo-Dioulasso, Burkina Faso
3MIVEGEC, Univ. Montpellier, IRD, CNRS, Montpellier, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Olivier Roux
  • For correspondence: olivier.roux@ird.fr
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Data/Code
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

Cues involved in mate seeking and recognition prevent hybridization and can be involved in speciation processes. In malaria mosquitoes, females of the two sibling species Anopheles gambiae s.s. and An. coluzzii mate in monospecific male swarms and hybrids are rare. Long-range sex pheromones driving this behavior have been debated in literature but to date, no study has proven their existence or their absence. Here, we attempted to bring to light their existence. To put all the odds in our favor, we used different chemical ecology methods such as behavioral and electrophysiological assays as well chemical analyses, and we worked with mosquitoes at their optimal physiological mating state i.e. with swarming males during their natural swarming windows. Despite all our efforts, our results support the absence of long-range sex pheromones involved in swarm detection and recognition by females. We briefly discuss the implications of this finding in ecology, evolution and for control strategies.

INTRODUCTION

Cues involved in mate seeking and mate recognition prevent hybridization and can be involved in speciation processes. In insects, these cues can be visual, acoustical or chemical with a short or a long-range action and are highly species specific (Alexander et al. 1997; Clements 1999).

In mosquitoes, many species mate in flight, within individual aggregations called “mating swarms” (Gibson 1945; Downes 1969; Savolainen 1978; Shelly and Whittier 1997; Sivinski and Petersson 1997). In malaria mosquitoes of the Anopheles gambiae complex, mating takes place at sunset in monospecific swarms containing a few males to thousands of them in which virgin conspecific females come to find a mate (Charlwood and Jones 1980; Marchand 1984; Charlwood et al. 2002; Diabaté et al. 2003, 2009; Howell and Knols 2009; Sawadogo et al. 2014). In West Africa, An. coluzzii and An. gambiaes.s. are often found in sympatry but they form distinct swarms spatially segregated and hybrids are rare (≈1%) (della Torre et al. 2001; Tripet et al. 2001; della Torre et al. 2005; Diabaté et al. 2006; Costantini et al. 2009; Diabaté et al. 2009; Sawadogo et al. 2013, 2014). No evidence for selection against hybrids was found and spermatheca analyses showed that these species mostly mate assortatively (Persiani et al. 1986; Tripet et al. 2001; Diabaté et al. 2005, 2007; Hahn et al. 2012; Pombi et al. 2017). This suggests that reproductive isolation between these two sibling species is achieved by strong pre-mating reproductive barriers (della Torre et al. 2001; Diabaté et al. 2007; Lehmann and Diabaté 2008). In addition, since females usually only mate once in their lives (Clements 1992), errors in the choice of mate should be costly and fall under negative selection. Consequently, one would expect to find specific cues that lead females to conspecific male swarms. However, the way females are attracted to swarms is unknown.

In species of the An. gambiae complex, several cues have been identified to play a role in bringing sexes together. Acoustic cues were shown to be involved in close-range recognition, the male and the female adjusting their respective wing-beat frequencies to converge on a shared harmonic frequency (Gibson et al. 2010; Pennetier et al. 2010). However, it was recently demonstrated that females of some Anopheles species are able to detect swarm sounds only in a very close vicinity. Thus, they are unable to use swarm sound to locate or identify swarms at long range (Feugère et al. 2021). Visual cues also play an important role in species segregation with An. coluzzii males swarming over contrasted visual ground markers (marker, hereafter) and An. gambiae males swarming over bare ground (Diabaté et al. 2009). However, a recent work showed that, like An. coluzzii, An. gambiae males also use visual markers but rather to locate their swarm at a distance from the marker (Poda et al. 2019). Moreover, females also use these markers to form swarms in the absence of males suggesting that females may use these markers to join the swarm location (Poda et al. 2019). Nevertheless, the distance between two heterospecific swarms using the same visual ground marker can be about 2 m in semi-field conditions (Poda et al. 2019) and it is still unknown from which distance mosquitoes can see such markers, suggesting that females could cross the location of heterospecific swarms by accident. Chemical cues have also been under investigation in Anopheles species. Heptacosane, a cuticular hydrocarbon, enhances the interaction between males and females (Wang et al. 2021). This compound, however, can only be perceived by contact and can consequently be involved only in mate recognition during courtship and to stimulate acceptance by females. Moreover, the absence of assortative mating in confined heterospecific males and females in laboratory cages or indoor swarms (Dao et al. 2008), suggests that close-range mating cues, if they exist, cannot ensure total reproductive isolation by themselves. Species isolation is thus likely to occur through long-range and specific swarm recognition cues acting as a first barrier in pre-mating processes which prevent hybridization by limiting contact between sexes of the different species.

To our knowledge, volatile sex pheromones in the An. gambiae complex have never been brought to light without ambiguity. Charlwood et al. (2002) reported an absence of response of An. gambiae males in natural swarms to squashed females on filter paper or to living females in a net cage. However, in natural conditions, females are the ones attracted to male aggregation sites. Consequently, males should be the ones that emit long-range pheromones and should be the attractive sex. Nevertheless, a laboratory study failed to demonstrate any attractiveness of dead males to virgin females in a Y-tube olfactometer (Gomulski 1988). However, the difficulties in highlighting the existence of such pheromones might be due to an emission in very low quantities and/or exclusively during swarming. Indeed, in sandfly species, which also mate in large aggregations of males, it was shown that the concentration of male-produced sex pheromones is greater in larger swarms, and hence, the chances for individual males of mating with a conspecific female are also greater (Kelly and Dye 1997; Bray et al. 2010). Similarly, Diabaté et al. (2011) showed that An. coluzzii females were more frequently attracted to large swarms, suggesting a potential additive effect of cues released by males. Males gathering in large swarms may increase their “detectability” in the female’s olfactory landscape (Shelly and Whittier 1997). In addition, in many insects, pheromone release and receptivity were shown to respond to diel rhythms or even strict time windows of only a few minutes or hours (Bjostad et al. 1980; Merlin et al. 2007; Rund et al. 2013; Levi-Zada et al. 2014) or even to be dependent of the presence of conspecifics (Andersson et al. 2007; Robledo and Arzuffi 2012). Nevertheless, a recent work by Mozūraitis et al. (2020) and concomitant to a first version of the present work (Poda et al. 2021), highlighted the existence of five volatile compounds potentially emitted by Anopheles males. According to the authors, these might be involved in aggregation behavior, attracting both males and females, and increasing the insemination rate. However, though an interesting biological activity was described by the authors, the claim that these compounds are male swarming aggregation pheromones must be considered with caution and it requires further investigations. Indeed, these five compounds (acetoin, sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal) are very frequently found in nature and, as reported by the authors, have been shown to have a biological activity in Anopheles mosquitoes in very different contexts from the reproductive behavior. They were repeatedly found in human and animal body odor (Verhulst et al. 2010; Pandey and Kim 2011; Dormont et al. 2013b, a; Tchouassi et al. 2013; McBride et al. 2014), breath (Poli et al. 2010; Calenic and Amann 2014; Filipiak et al. 2014; Cainap et al. 2020), and in host-plants (Dekel et al. 2019). These represent the main sources of food (blood and sugar meals, respectively) for Anopheles mosquitoes. As a matter of fact, octanal, nonanal and decanal are part of a blend used in some traps to mimic mammalian host odor for several species of mosquito vectors (Tchouassi et al. 2013; Nyasembe et al. 2014). Some of these compounds were also found associated to oviposition sites (Suh et al. 2016; Wondwosen et al. 2016, 2018) and to ambient air (Kostiainen 1995; Kruza et al. 2017). In addition, sulcatone is thought to be responsible for discrimination between humans and animals in human-seeking mosquitoes (McBride et al. 2014). Thus, it is not surprising that they trigger a strong biological/flight activity in mosquitoes.

Nevertheless, the discovery of sex pheromones or a highly attractive blend in malaria mosquitoes would be a precious step toward the development of new control and monitoring strategies. Furthermore, sex or aggregation pheromones could help in designing sexually competitive mosquitoes in sterile insect and gene drive techniques. Here, we investigated the existence of long-range sex pheromones in An. gambiae and An. coluzzii mosquitoes, that may allow the females to detect, recognize and track conspecific male swarms. On the premise that such pheromones could be produced only by males during swarming windows and to put all the odds in our favor, we used different chemical ecology methods, always with living swarming mosquitoes during their natural swarming windows. First, we investigated the long-range behavioral response of females exposed to the volatile blend from male swarms in an olfactometer. Second, we collected and analyzed volatile organic compounds (VOCs) with different methods on both laboratory-induced swarms and natural swarms. And third, we tested for an antenna-electrophysiological response of females to male swarm VOCs. As much as possible, we used both recently colonized mosquitoes and large experimental set-ups to ensure males produced a free swarming behavior. In addition, we replicated the main experiment by Mozūraitis et al. (2020) to search for the five compounds. We specifically added two new controls, one to check if these compounds were male specific, and the other to discard a potential environmental/laboratory pollution.

Methods and Materials

Mosquitoes

Mosquitoes used in behavioral experiments and for both VOC collection and electrophysiological analyses were from colonies raised from wild gravid females collected in inhabited human dwellings in Burkina Faso (West Africa). Anopheles coluzzii were collected in 2017 in Bama and An. gambiae in 2015 in Soumousso. Bama is a village located in a rice-growing area located 30 km North of Bobo-Dioulasso (11°24’14”N; 04°24’42”W) where previous studies showed that the Anopheles population is almost exclusively composed of An. coluzzii (Mosqueira et al. 2015; Poda et al. 2018). Soumousso is a typical Guinean savannah village located 30 km North-East of Bobo-Dioulasso (11°00’46”N, 4°02’45”W) where An. coluzzii and An. gambiae coexist, with a predominance of the latter (Diabaté et al. 2004, 2006). Gravid females were placed individually in oviposition cups containing tap water. After oviposition, females were identified to species by routine PCR-RFLP (Santolamazza et al. 2008). The larvae were gathered according to their species and reared in tap water, fed with Tetramin® Baby Fish Food (Tetrawerke, Melle, Germany) ad libitum. Adult mosquitoes were held in 30 × 30 × 30 cm mesh-covered cages and provided with a 5% glucose solution ad libitum. Insectarium conditions were maintained at 27±2 °C, 70±10% RH and 12L:12D. The colonies were refreshed twice a year with F1 from mosquito females caught in the wild.

Mosquitoes used in the electrophysiological study were transferred as eggs from Burkina Faso to France. On their arrival, eggs were allowed to hatch in osmosed water and the larvae were fed with Tetramin® Baby Fish Food ad libitum. Adult mosquitoes were held in 20 × 20 × 20 cm mesh-covered cages and provided with honey diluted at 5% ad libitum. Females were fed with rabbit blood on a PS6 Power Unit (Hemotek, Blackburn, UK) for egg production. Mosquitoes were reared in a laboratory climate chamber KBF-S720 (BINDER Gmbh, Tuttlingen, Germany) at 27±2 °C, 70±10% RH and 12L:12D with a sunset time programmed at 3:00pm (synchronization of swarming/mating time and electrophysiological test time to ensure an optimal receptivity of females). Mosquitoes used for the experiments were sexed early after emergence and sexes were kept in separate rearing cages to prevent mating.

Mosquitoes used to replicate the experiment by Mozūraitis et al. (2020) were from a 15-year-old colony of An. gambiae (Kisumu strain) reared in the IRD laboratory of Montpellier. They were maintained at 27 ±2 °C, 80 ± 10% RH, with a photoperiod cycle of 12h light: 12h dark and reared as described above.

Long-range behavioral response of virgin females to swarm volatile organic compounds

Olfactometer setup

Bioassays were conducted in a dual-port olfactometer originally designed to study host preference in the An. gambiae complex (Lefèvre et al. 2009, 2010; Vantaux et al. 2015; Nguyen et al. 2017). The odor source container was modified to enclose male swarms in boxes made of transparent plexiglass (L × W × H: 60 × 60 × 120 cm; “swarming boxes” hereafter). Each swarming box was connected by a PVC air vent hoses (L × Ø: 600 × 10 cm, W3-65014-HQ4, HQ, USA) to a collecting glass box (L × W × H: 32 × 33.5 × 44 cm) which was linked to a mesh-covered releasing cage (L × W × H: 50 × 40 × 40 cm) by a glass tube (L × Ø: 60 × 10 cm) (Fig. 1). A custom-made electric fan was located at the mid-length of each air vent hose and drew air from the swarming boxes (odor-sources) to the releasing cage, providing an odor-laden air current against which mosquitoes in the releasing cage were induced to fly. The air flow was controlled thanks to two mechanisms; first, a power regulator (HQ-Power PS1502A, Velleman, Gavère, Belgium) connected to fans; and second, iris dampers (10 cm Ø; CIR D100, France air, France) connecting the air vent hoses to the collecting boxes. The openings of the air vent hoses on both the swarming and collecting boxes sides were covered with nets to prevent mosquitoes from flying into the air vent hoses. The swarming boxes (odor-sources) were located side-by-side outdoors and the olfactometer inside a room (Fig. 1). The air speed in the releasing cage was regulated at 18±2 cm.s-1 using an anemometer (Model 425, Testo, Forbach, France) and the room temperature and relative humidity were set at 27±2 °C and 80±10% RH thanks to an air conditioner and a humidifier (Defensor 505, Condair, Croissy-Beaubourg, France), respectively.

Fig. 1:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Fig. 1:

Schematic of the olfactometer set up. S: swarm; M: marker. Not at scale.

Bioassays

Test preparation

In the morning, about 8 hours before the time of the test, 100 2-to 3-day-old virgin females of An. coluzzii and An. gambiae were colored with two different colored powders (Luminous Powder Kit, BioQuip, Rancho Dominguez, California, USA) according to the species. In addition, about 500 4-to 5-day-old An. coluzzii or An. gambiae males were released into a swarming box to allow them to acclimate. Both males and females were provided with a 10% glucose solution and were kept under insectary conditions until the time of the test. To avoid biases during the test due to both humidity and odor as a result of manipulation or bacterial proliferation, the empty swarming box (control box) was also provided with glucose.

Test execution

Bioassays were carried out at sunset, when males naturally swarm. About 30 min before sunset time, the swarming boxes were moved outside, the cups containing the glucose pads were removed and visual markers made of a 20 × 20 cm black cloth were placed either under or next to the boxes for An. coluzzii or An. gambiae, respectively (see Poda et al. 2019). These markers allowed to trigger swarming behavior and to stabilize swarms in the middle of the box. For the same purpose, we provided an artificial twilight horizon made of a 40 W incandescent bulb (2,500 K) located on the floor between a white wall and a 50 cm high black horizon (Facchinelli et al. 2015; Niang et al. 2019; Poda et al. 2019). Inside, about 10 min before introducing females, the fans were switched on in order to purge the air vent hoses and the air flow was set at 18±2 cm.s-1. The glass tube openings on the releasing cage side were covered with nets to prevent female mosquitoes from flying into the collecting boxes before the test began. The complete olfactometer, except the end of the releasing cage, was covered with a white cloth to eliminate visual bias during the test and to provide a diffused light. Sunset light was allowed to enter the olfactometer room until dark and an additional artificial twilight made of a 40 W incandescent bulb (2,500K) projected on the wall of the room on the side of the collecting boxes was also provided. Thirty minutes before sunset, about 100 females of each species (N=200, 1 replicate) were released simultaneously into the releasing cage of the olfactometer allowing them to acclimate.

When the males started to swarm (i.e. flying in loops at a constant location within the swarming boxes, see Downes 1969; Poda et al. 2019), the nets covering the glass tube openings and preventing access of females to the collecting boxes were removed thanks to threads that were attached to the nets. The females were allowed to respond for 20 min (swarm duration), then the glass tube openings were covered again with nets and females that reached the upwind collecting boxes were removed with a mouth aspirator, killed by freezing and identified according to their coloration (species) under a binocular (LEICA S6E) and counted. The remaining mosquitoes inside the releasing cage were also removed. After each test (1 per day), the olfactometer was cleaned with 95% ethanol to remove odor contaminants. All materials were handled with gloves to avoid contamination with skin compounds.

Test combinations

Four combinations of choice tests were performed: i) An. coluzzii male swarm vs. empty box (hereafter, An. coluzzii test); ii) An. gambiae male swarm vs. empty box (hereafter, An. gambiae test), iii) An. coluzzii vs. An. gambiae male swarms (hereafter, An. coluzzii vs. An. coluzzii test) and iv) empty vs. empty box (hereafter, control test). We ran 12 replicates per combination and females were used only once. Males were used twice in two consecutive days. In that case, a 10% glucose solution was introduced into the two swarming boxes after the test and boxes were kept under insectarium conditions until the next day. The swarming boxes were cleaned with ethanol between each batch of males. To avoid biases, the matching of species and colors was switched between each test, and we alternated the treatments (mosquitoes or control) between the swarming boxes and the right and left arm of the olfactometer. We assessed the instrumental and arm bias through an empty vs. empty box choice test (control test). The olfactometer did not present any symmetrical biases (Fig. S1).

Collection of swarm volatile organic compounds (VOCs)

Because compounds can be emitted at a given time and only produced in tiny quantities at individual scale, we chose to work directly on swarms. Three different sampling methods were used; two methods were run in a laboratory setup (Niang et al. 2019) and one in the field.

Dynamic headspace collection in the laboratory

VOC collection setup

The collection of swarm VOCs was performed in a room specially designed to stimulate Anopheles swarming behavior thanks to a set of visual stimuli (see Niang et al. (2019) for details). The headspace volume consisted of a 50 × 50 × 50 cm transparent plexiglass box (extraction box). Such a volume was a trade-off between a volume large enough to allow males to display a swarming behavior without too much constraints and a volume small enough to be sucked up in a reasonable time. Entrance and exit flow rates were maintained by two pumps and regulated by flowmeters. The entrance flow was higher than the exit flow ensuring that the system was continuously purged to compensate for the inevitable leaks, and that no contaminated outside air would enter the system. At the entrance side, ambient air was purified in a glass charcoal filter and then humidified passing through deionized water. Exit flow passed through an odor trap to adsorb VOCs. Three different odor traps were used for the chemical analyses and electrophysiological analyses. Tenax-TA/Carbotrap sorbent stainless steel 6mm diameter tubes (Gerstell, Mülheim, Germany) and Porapak-Q sorbent VCT glass tubes (ARS, Gainesville, Florida, USA) were used for chemical analyses. Porapak-Q sorbent VCT tubes and home-made sorbent Micro-traps were used for electrophysiological analysis. Micro-traps were constituted by ChromatoProbe® quartz microvials of Varian Inc. (length: 15 mm; inner diameter: 2 mm), previously cut closed-end and filled with 3 mg of a 1:1 mix of Tenax-TA and Carbotrap® (60–80 and 20–40 mesh, respectively; Sigma Aldrich, Munich, Germany)(See Dormont et al. 2013b). Flow rates and VOC collection durations were dependent on the odor trap (See Table 1).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1:

Air inlet and outlet rates, and VOC collection duration according to the type of trap used.

VOC collection

About 500 4- to 6-day-old males of An. gambiae or An. coluzzii were transferred into the extraction box about 30 min before swarming time. Clean air was pushed into the box at a rate of 10 L.min-1 to purge the box from both human and mosquito container odors before connecting it to the odor trap. Five minutes after males started to swarm (i.e. flying in loops at a constant location within the box), the air flow was adjusted at the required rate and the odor trap connected. Forty minutes later, swarming behavior was stopped by turning off the light and the VOC collection continued until the required collection time was reached (see Table 1). After VOC collection, samples were stored at 4 °C until analysis. After males were removed, the box was cleaned with 95% ethanol and then flushed with clean air for at least 5 hours to remove the odor contaminants left from the previous extraction. A total of 30 swarm extracts were collected; 17 An. coluzzii swarms (5, 8 and 4 with Tenax/Carbotrap tubes, Porapak-Q VCT tubes and Micro-traps, respectively) and 13 An. gambiae swarms (6 and 7 with Tenax/Carbotrap tubes, and Porapak-Q VCT tubes, respectively). Control consisted of a clean empty box. Before they were sent and used in Burkina Faso, adsorbents were cleaned as follows. Tenax-TA/Carbotrap tubes were heated at 250 °C for 30 min under a 30 mL.min-1 flow then sealed. Porapak-Q VCT tubes were eluted with 3 mL of hexane then packed individually in a nalophan bag. Chromatoprobes were heated for 2 h at 270 °C by 100 chromatoprobes under a 100 mL.min-1 nitrogen flow, then packed in a glass vial with a Teflon cap.

VOC collection from natural swarms

VOCs were collected from a natural swarm of An. coluzzii in the village of Bama. Static sorptive collections of volatiles from a non-enclosed swarm were performed with Twisters® (100 μm PDMS stir bar; Gerstel, Mülheim, Germany; Bicchi et al. 2000; Tienpont et al. 2000). The swarm flew about 2.5 to 3.5 m above the ground. Thus, we used a 1.5 m long glass stick (Glaswarenfabrik Karl Hecht GmbH & Co, Germany) with a metallic push pin head glued at the tip and covered with nalophan to introduce the magnetic twisters directly into the swarm. Control twisters were placed about 3 m away upwind from the swarm. VOC collection lasted for the whole swarm duration (about 20-25 min). We chose a swarm far from habitations and livestock to limit odor pollutions, containing a large number of males (more than 1,000) and attractive for females (observation of a large number of couples). After VOC collections, twisters were individually placed into glass vials and stored at 4 °C until analysis. Five replicates were performed on the same swarm on different days.

Solvent extraction

About 1,000 4- to 6-day-old males of An. gambiae or An. coluzzii were introduced into a 30 × 30 × 30 cm cage and kept under insectarium conditions. The cage was made of unpainted metallic frames covered by white nets and the bottom was covered with a white paper. Thirty minutes before sunset time, the cage was placed outside and mosquitoes were observed. A 5 × 5 cm black cloth was placed in or out of the cage according to the species as described above to stimulate swarming behavior. About 5 min after mosquitoes started to swarm, the entire cage was quickly placed into a freezer at −20° C for about 10 min. Then, mosquitoes were transferred into a 20 ml glass tube and covered with a 1:1 mix of Hexane and Dichloromethane. The tube was sealed and mosquitoes were kept in the solution for 24h at 27±2 °C. Then, the solution was filtered on glass wool to remove mosquito scales and stored at 4° C until analysis. Three different controls were made: i) with 1- to 2-day-old virgin males (young male control), ii) with 2- to 3-day-old virgin females (female control) and iii) with solvent mix alone (blank control). A total of 8 swarm extracts were done; 4 of An. coluzzii and 4 of An. gambiae and 1 replicate of each type of control per species.

VOC collection with SPME fiber

We replicated the experiment from the study by Mozūraitis et al. (2020) in which they collected mosquito headspace with a SPME fiber in a 1L bottle. However, because most methods used to introduce mosquitoes into a bottle may potentially result in entering pollutants as well, we added a supplementary control. One way to easily introduce the mosquitoes is to blow them from a mouth aspirator into the bottle. This classic method may bring breath volatiles within the recipient, which can contain the five compounds described in Mozūraitis et al. (2020) (Poli et al. 2010; Calenic and Amann 2014; Filipiak et al. 2014; Cainap et al. 2020). Consequently, we added a “breath control”. In addition, we also replicated the experiment with females to check for sex specificity. In the morning, the two glass bottles were cleaned with acetone and dried under a clean air flow, then closed with nalophan. Three hours before swarming time (5pm), we collected the headspace of empty bottles with polydimethylsiloxane/divinylbenzene-coated SPME fibers for 1h (step 1: empty bottle). Then, we simulated the introduction of mosquitoes by blowing 3 times for 3 seconds with a mouth aspirator into the bottles. These were then closed with Nalophan (6pm) and the head space of the breath of the experimenter was collected for 1h with SPME fibers (step 2: breath control). We then introduced 60-70, 5- to 7-day-old unmated males in each bottle with the mouth aspirator, closed the bottles with Nalophan (7pm) and let the mosquitoes acclimate for 1h. During the acclimation period, we simulated a light transition from photophase to scotophase to trigger crepuscular flight. Then, when males started to fly (8pm), SPME fibers were introduced for 1h (step 3: “swarm” extract). Six different fibers were used and cleaned at 250 °C during 5 min in the injector of a gas-chromatograph (HP 6890 Series PLUS, Agilent, Santa Clara, USA) just before use. Each fiber was always used for the same type of extract and in the same bottle. Before the experiment, we made sure that the fibers had similar sensitivity by exposing them to a mix of the five compounds. Only minor quantitative differences were detected, and this was considered during subsequent statistics. Secondly, we performed the same 3 steps but with 60-70, 3- to 4-day-old virgin females instead of males. And finally, as during the introduction of mosquitoes, we blew a second time into the bottles and repeated the three steps of headspace collection but this time, instead of introducing mosquitoes in the 3rd step, we blew again into the bottles (breath ×2) to check for potential accumulation of breath compounds. We kept track of the day of the experiment, the bottles and fibers identity for statistical analyses.

Chemical analyzes

Samples obtained on Porapak-Q VCT tubes were eluted with 150 μl of Hexane and injected (1 μl) into a Gas Chromatograph coupled with Mass Spectrometer (GC-MS). Those obtained on Tenax/Carbotrap tubes, twisters or SPME fibers were directly thermo-desorbed into GC-MS. Solvent extracts were injected as is or concentrated under a gentle stream of nitrogen. All the analyses were run at the “Platform for Chemical Analyses in Ecology” (PACE), the technical facilities of the LabEx CeMEB (Centre Méditerranéen pour l’Environnement et la Biodiversité, Montpellier, France).

Liquid analysis (Porapak elutions and solvent extractions)

Liquids were analysed with a GC-MS QP2010 Plus (Shimadzu, Kyoto, Japan). Mass spectra were recorded in electronic impact mode (EI) at 70eV over a m/z mass range from 38 to 350. The temperature of the transfer line and the ion source were programmed to 250 °C and 200 °C, respectively. The injections were made with an injector temperature of 250 °C, and a 1:4 split mode ratio. Analyses were performed using a 30 m × 0.25 mm × 0.25 μm Optima 5-MS (Macherey-Nagel, Düren, Germany) fused silica capillary column with a constant helium flow set close to 1 ml.min-1. The oven temperature was programmed as follows: 40 °C (held 5 min) to 250 °C at 6 °C.min-1, and finally to 300 °C at 14 °C.min-1 and held 2 min. GC-MS Solution software (Shimadzu, Kyoto, Japan) was used for data processing of these analyses, with the NIST 2011 as spectrum database.

Tenax/Carbotrap tubes and twister analysis

These sorbents were analysed as described by Souto-Vilarós et al. (2018). Samples were analysed using a gas chromatograph (GC, Trace™ 1310, Thermo Scientific™ Milan, Italy) coupled with a mass spectrometer (MS, ISQ™ QD Single Quadrupole, Thermo Scientific™ Milan, Italy). The column used was an Optima 5-MS, the same as for liquid analysis. Absorbent traps were handled with a Multi Purpose Sampler (Gerstell, Mülheim, Germany) and desorbed with a double stage desorption system, composed of a Thermal Desorption Unit (TDU) and a Cold Injection System (CIS) (Gerstell, Mülheim, Germany). The sorbents were desorbed in splitless mode at 250 °C on a trap cooled at −80 °C by liquid nitrogen. Then, the trap was heated to 250 °C with a 1:4 split ratio to inject the compounds in the column. The carrier gas used was helium at 1 ml.min-1. Oven temperature program was as follows: held at 40 °C for 3 min., then increased to 220 °C at 5 °C.min-1 and finally to 250 °C at 10 °C.min-1, and held for 2 min. Mass spectra were recorded in electronic impact mode (EI) at 70eV over a m/z mass range from 38 to 350. The temperature of the transfer line and the ion source were programmed to 250 °C and 200 °C, respectively. Xcalibur™ software (Thermo Scientific™, Milan, Italy) was used for data processing and the NIST 2011 as spectrum database.

SPME fiber analysis

We analyzed SPME fibers as described in Mozūraitis et al. (2020) with a GC-MS QP2010-SE (Shimadzu, Kyoto, Japan). Volatiles were desorbed into the injector at 225 °C for 1 min in split mode (1:4 ratio). Analyses were performed using a 30 m × 0.25 mm × 0.25 μm J&W DB-Wax silica capillary column (Agilent, Santa Clara, USA) with a constant helium flow at 1 ml.min-1. The oven temperature was programmed as follows: 40 °C (held 1 min.) to 150 °C at 5 °C.min-1, and finally to 220 °C at 20 °C.min-1 and held 9 min. Mass spectra were recorded in electronic impact mode (EI) at 70eV over a m/z mass range from 38 to 350. The temperature of the transfer line and the ion source were 250 °C and 200 °C, respectively. GC-MS Solution software (Shimadzu, Kyoto, Japan) was used for data processing of these analyses.

Electrophysiological analysis

Electrophysiological activity of female olfactive detection, on the male swarm extracts, was tested with an Electro-Antennografic Detection system, coupled with Gas Chromatography (GC-EAD) at the PACE. Thirty minutes before the mosquito scotophase, both 4- to 6-day-old males and 2- to 3-day-old virgin females were placed separately in cardboard cups (Ø = 75 mm, h = 100 mm), provided with a 5% honey solution and transferred from the insectarium to the electrophysiology laboratory at PACE. At mating time, mosquitoes were placed in the dark. Males were checked for erected antennae as a proxy to ensure that change in the light-dark cycle, transport and electrophysiology laboratory conditions did not alter mosquito motivation for mating. Two minutes after male antennae were erected, a female was removed from the cardboard for the antennal preparation.

The female’s head was gently excised with a pair of surgical scissors and mounted between two glass capillary tubes of 76 mm in length and 1.12 mm in diameter (World Precision Instrument, Sarasota, USA), pulled and cut using a vertical micropipette-puller (P-30 model, World Precision Instruments, Hertfordshire, United Kingdom) filled with insect Ringer solution (6.0 g.l-1 NaCl, 0.4 g.l-1 KCl, 0.27 g.l-1 CaCl2 and 3.20 g.l-1 NaC3H5O3) and connected to silver wires. The indifferent electrode was inserted into the back (the foramen) of the isolated head and the intact tip of one antenna was inserted into the recording electrode which was connected to an Electro-Antennography Detector setup (EAD, Syntech IDAC-2, Kirchzarten, Germany). One end of the glass electrodes was pulled to a fine point which was then broken to ensure a close fit with the female’s head and antenna.

The above EAD setup was linked to a Gas Chromatograph-Flame Ionization Detector (GC-FID, CP-3800, Varian, Palo Alto, USA) equipped with an Optima 5-MS capillary column, the same as for the GC-MS analyses. Liquid injections after Porapack solvent elution were made in a 1079 PTV injector (Programmed Temperature Vaporizator) at 250 °C with a 1:4 split ratio. Microtraps were desorbed in the same injector using a ChromatoProbe sample introduction device (Varian, Palo Alto, USA) with the same split ratio and temperature programmed as follows: increased from 40 °C to 250 °C at 200 °C.min-1 and held for 5 min. Oven temperature was held at 50 °C for 0.40 min, then increased to 180 °C at 10 °C.min-1, and finally to 220 °C at 12 °C.min-1, for a run duration less than 17 min. The carrier gas used was helium at 1 ml.min-1. The effluent was split equally into two deactivated fused silica capillary columns (100 cm x 0.25 mm), one leading to the FID (270 °C) and the other into a heated EAD port (200 °C) (transfer line: Syntech, Kirchzarten, Germany) and leaded to antenna. The GC effluent for antenna was mixed with charcoal-filtered, humidified airflow (300 ml.min-1). StarWorkstation 6.41 software (Varian, Palo Alto, USA) was used for data processing.

Different types of male swarm extracts were tested on the antennae of several conspecific females. Antennae of 19 An. coluzziifemales were tested with solvent extracts, Porapak-Q VCT tube extracts and Micro-trap extracts (8, 9 and 2 females, respectively) and 20 An. gambiae females were tested with solvent and Porapak-Q VCT tube extracts (10 females each). Before GC-EAD analysis of the VOC extracts, we checked the living antenna for good activity using a negative (solvent) and a positive (mix of many VOCs) stimulus solutions. We proceeded as follows: 5μl of each solution was applied to a 1 cm × 2 cm filter paper contained in a Pasteur pipette. The pipette was then placed in an air pump system and the volatiles were directly sent to the antennal preparation with a pulse duration of 0.5 s and a flow rate of 890 ml.min-1 regulated by a CS-55 Stimulus Controller (Syntech, Hilversum, Netherlands).

Identification of specific and active VOCs

For all the chemical analysis, we compared the chromatograms of swarming mosquito extracts with those of their corresponding controls to search for any qualitative or quantitative differences. When such a difference was pinpointed, we matched the mass spectra of the compounds of interest with the NIST 2011 MS library. A solution of n-alkanes (Alkanes standard solution, 04070, Sigma Aldrich, Darmstadt, Germany) was also injected to calculate the linear retention index (LRI) of these compounds of interest (Zellner et al. 2008) and their LRIs were compared with those reported in the literature.

In the electrophysiological analysis, we studied the chromatograms in a range of LRI between 800 and 1700, including compounds present at very low and trace amounts. When a depolarization was observed in the electrophysiological analysis, a section of 20 points of LRI was studied around the compound’s LRI on the GC-MS trace. Comparison of the samples with their respective controls collected on the same day, allowed to subtract potential contaminants from the samples. Non-natural compounds, such as industrial chemicals and/or compounds not naturally produced by living organisms (Charpentier et al. 2012), were considered as contaminants.

We also particularly searched for the presence of acetoin, sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal reported by Mozūraitis et al. (2020). With that aim, we defined a LRI window of 20 points on both sides of the LRI of these compounds on the bases of injected standards (for SPME fibers; acetoin, sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal, ≥98%, 99%, 99%, ≥98% and ≥98% GC, respectively, Sigma-Aldrich, Darmstadt, Germany) or literature (for other extraction methods). Then, we searched for their specific ions at those LRI as follows: (LRI/ion) 711/88, 981/108, 998/84, 1100/98, and 1201/112 for acetoin, sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal, respectively. Then, when a peak corresponding to both LRI and specific ion was found, we compared its spectra with the NIST 2011 MS library before quantification with integration units.

Statistical analysis

Both the activation rate and choice were analyzed using binomial Generalized Linear-Mixed Models (GLMM, “glmer” function in “lme4” package). Tested combinations (four levels: An. coluzzii, An. gambiae, An. coluzzii vs. An. gambiae and control tests), female species (two levels: An. coluzzii and An. gambiae) and their interaction were considered as fixed effects, and replicates as random effects. All analyses were performed using R (version 3.4.0).

We analyzed separately the quantities using integration units of acetoin, sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal. When pairing between the controls and the mosquito extracts was possible (with SPME and twister) and to account for repeated measurements, we used Gaussian GLMMs. In those cases, the extract (3 levels for SPME fibers: empty bottles, breath and flying mosquitoes (either male or females or breath ×2) and 2 levels for twisters: control and swarm) was considered as fixed effect. The replicates (i.e. the day for twisters and the SPME) and fiber nested within the bottle for SPME fibers were considered random effects. Considering the SPME fibers as a random effect made it possible to account for their minor differences of sensitivity in the model. When pairing between the controls and the mosquito extracts was not possible (Tenax-TA/Carbotrap and Porapak-Q VCT tubes), Gaussian GLMs were used. The extract (empty box, An. coluzzii swarm or An. gambiae swarm) was considered as fixed effect. In some occasions, some compounds were not found in the extracts or only in two samples. Consequently, it was not possible to perform statistical analyses and we considered the extracts as not significantly different. No statistical analysis was performed with liquid extracts due to a small number of replicates. For model selection, we used the stepwise removal of terms, followed by likelihood ratio tests. Term removals that significantly reduced explanatory power (P <0.05) were retained in the minimal adequate model. All percentages are provided with their 95% confident interval.

RESULTS

Behavioral assays

Over 12 replicates of c.a. 200 females for each of the four combinations, a total of 8,918 females were tested (4,423 An. coluzzii and 4,495 An. gambiae) among which 3,591 flew upwind into the collecting boxes (activation rate: 40.2±1%). There was a significant interaction between female species and the four combinations of swarm boxes tested (Embedded Image, P <0.001). Species subset analyses showed that the different test combinations had no effect on the An. coluzzii activation rate (Embedded Image, P=0.12; Fig. 2). However, in An. gambiae, there was a significant effect of the test combinations on the female activation rate (Embedded Image, P = 0.001; Fig. 2) with a higher activation rate when females were exposed simultaneously to both An. coluzzii and An. gambiae male swarms than when exposed to an An. coluzzii swarm alone (z = 4.01, P <0.001) or to the control (two empty boxes: z = 2.57, P = 0.04). Nonetheless, no difference was found when females were exposed to an An. gambiae male swarm alone (z = −1.49, P = 0.44). Neither the An. coluzzii swarms, nor the An. gambiae swarms were attractive for activated females (female species: Embedded Image, P = 0.11; choice test combinations: Embedded Image, P = 0.56 and female species × choice test combination interaction: Embedded Image, P = 0.84; Fig. 3).

Fig. 2:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Fig. 2:

Mosquito activation rate, expressed as the proportion of females caught in both collecting boxes out of the total number released females for each of the four tested combinations. Different letters indicate difference at P<0.05.

Fig. 3:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Fig. 3:

Mosquito choice, expressed as the proportion of females caught in one or the other collecting box out of the total number of “activated” females for each test.

Chemical analyses

No qualitative or quantitative differences were detected between samples of swarming males and their relative controls for both species obtained from the Porapak-Q, Twisters® and the liquid extracts (Figs S2 & S3). Minor quantitative differences were detected with the Tenax/Carbotrap tubes. However, mass spectra showed that these peaks were pollutants, such as silicones, BTX (mono-aromatic compounds like benzene, toluene and xylene), alkanes which were all emitted by the box containing the mosquitoes. Quantitative differences including linear organic acids were also detected, but they were not reproducible.

During the experiment replicating the protocol by Mozūraitis et al. (2020), both males and females started to fly at the expected time (8pm) but due to the small volume it was not possible to determine if the group was a swarm. Analyses of SPME fibers in search for acetoin, sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal showed that acetoin was absent from almost all the male samples (Fig. 4A), but was present on three occurrences in female extracts (over six) (Fig. 4B). Sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal were found in almost all types of extracts including controls. However, we did not find significant increased quantities of the five compounds in “swarming” mosquito extracts compared to the breath controls (Fig. 4). The control to test the effect of blowing twice in the bottle showed that the headspaces of “breath” and “breath ×2” were very similar (Fig. S4), demonstrating an absence of accumulation of breath odor between the breath control and the introduction of mosquitoes.

Fig. 4:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Fig. 4:

Quantities (integration units) of acetoin, sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal contained in the headspace of the empty bottles, the breath of the manipulator and flying males (A) and females (B) of Anopheles gambiae Kisumu and collected with SPME fibers. The intensity values correspond to the counts related to the abundance of the specific ions representative of each molecule formed in the mass spectrometer and correspond to the amount of compound analyzed. The box plots indicate the median (wide horizontal bars), the 25th and 75th percentiles (squares), and the minimum and maximum values (whiskers). The black dots represent outliers and grey dots raw data. NS=not-significant; * =P < 0.05; ** =P < 0.01; *** =P < 0.001.

A posteriori, we also quantified acetoin, sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal in our previous GC-MS analyses in search for similar variations as described in Mozūraitis et al. (2020). We did not find acetoin in our chromatograms. Indeed, as we used a low polarity Optima 5-MS column, acetoin (if present) was eluted before the LRI 800. In the samples obtained from natural swarms of An. coluzzii with the twister method, sulcatone and octanal were absent. The quantities of both nonanal and decanal were not significantly higher in the swarm than in the control (Fig. 5A). In laboratory swarm samples, sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal were found inconsistently. Nevertheless, swarms did not show higher quantities of these compounds compared to the empty box (Fig. 5B) excepted in samples obtained with the Porapack tubes (Fig. 5C). However, this difference was not statistically significant. Whatever, the method used, we can note a high quantitative variability between the samples. This was true for both the control and the mosquito samples.

Fig. 5:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Fig. 5:

Quantities (integration units) of sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal contained in a natural swarm sampled with twisters (A); in the headspace of the empty box, swarming Anopheles coluzzii males and An. gambiae males sampled with Tenax/Carbotrap tubes (B); or collected with Porapack tubes (C). The intensity values correspond to the counts related to the abundance of the specific ions representative of each molecule formed in the mass spectrometer and correspond to the amount of compound analyzed. The box plots indicate the median (wide horizontal bars), the 25th and 75th percentiles (squares), and the minimum and maximum values (whiskers). The black dots represent outliers and grey dots raw data. NS=not-significant; * =P < 0.05; ** =P < 0.01; *** =P < 0.001. NA: The analytical procedure for these analyses did not allow detection of acetoin.

DISCUSSION

For the past decades, several authors have suggested the existence of long-range sex pheromones in the attraction of An. gambiae s.l. females to male swarms (Charlwood et al. 2002; Tripet et al. 2004; Diabaté et al. 2011; Poda et al. 2019) but to date, no reliable experimental evidence is available. Here, we used behavioral, physiological and detailed chemical approaches to attempt to highlight the existence of such compounds. However, despite a large set of methodologies and working hypotheses to maximize the chances of highlighting the presence of pheromones (i.e. timing, physiological state, natural behavior, recently colonized mosquitoes), our study failed to provide evidence of the presence of long-range male sex pheromones in both An. coluzzii and An. gambiae. We also replicated an experimental setup that recently showed an increase in quantity of five compounds in “swarming” males compared to controls. However, unlike in Mozūraitis et al. (2020), we did not find any significant difference with our controls.

During our behavioral tests in the olfactometer, females were not attracted to air currents passed over male swarms regardless of the species. During the experiment, the males released in the large plexiglass boxes and exposed to natural light started to fly randomly at sunset. Then, about 150-200 males out of the 500 released (<50%) gathered in the upper half of the box flying in loops without touching the box walls. They also reacted to the movements of the visual marker, meaning they were indeed swarms. The other males flew randomly in the box, bouncing against the walls. Females showed a good activation rate but they did not show a particular choice for any arm, containing a swarm or not. Nevertheless, An. gambiae females showed an intriguing higher activation rate when exposed simultaneously to both a conspecific and a heterospecific swarm compared to a single An. coluzzii swarm. This was, however, probably without biological significance as no difference was detected in the test providing An. gambiae females with a choice between the control and An. gambiae swarm, vs. a test offering two controls.

Our behavioral result was consistent with our physiological study. Indeed, despite the fact that we checked for both behavioral receptivity of mosquitoes and for receptivity of mounted antennae, no consistent antennal depolarization was observed in females across assays when exposed to swarm extracts. This suggests a lack of response by females to any volatile chemicals present in our swarm extracts.

Chemical analyses were also negative and no compounds specific for male swarms could be detected whatever the method used. Moreover, unlike in Mozūraitis et al. (2020), the quantities of acetoin, sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal were found inconsistently across swarming mosquito samples and not in larger quantities compared to controls, making it difficult to support the assumption according to which they could actually be emitted by males. These divergent results could be explained by the fact that, unlike in the experiments of Mozūraitis et al. (2020), we used a control which considered the potential introduction of pollutions at the same time as mosquitoes. This showed that breath was responsible for most of the variability. The most convincing result showing that sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal are probably not pheromones emitted by males is the one obtained with the twister method. Indeed, twisters are coated with the same adsorbent phases as SPME fibers used both in this study and in the one by Mozūraitis et al. (2020). According to the manufacturer, twisters are up to a thousand times more sensitive than SPME fibers partly due to a larger sorbent volume. Moreover, we exposed the twisters directly in a natural swarm formed by more than 1 000 males (probably up to 6 000 males according to the estimation of trained technical staffs).

Despite this sensitivity, the number of mosquitoes and the natural biological context, sulcatone and octanal were not found and both nonanal and decanal had similar quantities in the swarm and outside the swarm (control twister placed 3 m upwind from the swarm). In addition, the high quantitative variability found in the laboratory and the fact that these compounds are frequently found in controls suggest that they could be laboratory and/or human pollutions that are difficult to control for.

Our results contrast with those of Mozūraitis et al. (2020) probably for several reasons. First, in their laboratory experiments, they reported a simple flight activity instead of a swarming activity in which males should fly in regular loops with erected antennae (Downes 1969; Poda et al. 2019). This can be explained by the absence of adequate visual stimuli such as ground markers which are mandatory to trigger swarming behavior (Charlwood and Jones 1980; Marchand 1984; Gibson 1985; Facchinelli et al. 2015; Niang et al. 2019; Poda et al. 2019). Second, this flight activity was extraordinary long for a swarming flight, exceeding 200 min in the presence of the five compounds. Indeed, swarming behavior is known to last for only 20-30 min in nature (Charlwood and Jones 1980; Marchand 1984; Sawadogo et al. 2013, 2014; Bimbilé Somda et al. 2018) and only up to 60 min in artificial conditions (Charlwood and Jones 1980; Poda et al. 2019). In the latter, the number of males in swarms decreased over time while the others flew randomly, bouncing on the flight arena walls (Poda et al. 2019). Indeed, swarming is an activity with a high energy demand, consuming 50% of sugar and glycogen reserves in 25 min (Maïga et al. 2012, 2014). Mosquitoes probably switched to a more random flight to try to escape and search for a sugar meal to refuel their reserves. This behavior can be stimulated by acetoin, sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal, as they are frequently emitted from nectar sources and fermented sugar (Goodrich et al. 2006; Schiestl 2010; Dekel et al. 2019). Finally, they also reported that these five compounds increased insemination rates in five different species in semi-field cages. However, instead of stopping the experiment after the swarming time, they collected the females in the morning. As they left both a dark box in the arena as resting site and a sugar source, it is likely that males and females also mated in or around these two resources during the night. Indeed, it was shown that mating can occur indoors, outside of swarming times and locations (Dao et al. 2008). The boxes and sugar sources with these five compounds were probably more attractive than the control ones gathering more mosquitoes and thus inducing more inseminations.

Independently, our methods can also have weaknesses. Even if the number of swarming males was biologically relevant compared to natural swarms, a higher number of males could be needed in experimental setups, or the sensitivity thresholds of our chemical and electrophysiological apparatus are below the ability of insects to detect low amounts of compounds. In addition, although the air flow used in our VOC collection in laboratory was not unconventional, it might have been too high inducing a leak of compounds of interest. Likewise, the air quality could have masked some compounds emitted in minute quantities even though our behavioral experiment and VOC collection in natural swarms led to negative results too.

However, altogether and keeping in mind that an absence of evidence is not an evidence of absence, our results support the absence of long-range sex pheromones emitted by male swarms. However, further investigations are needed and complementary methodologies such as electro-physiology on palps (Lu et al. 2007; Iatrou and Biessmann 2008; Pitts et al. 2011; Guidobaldi et al. 2014), molecular analyses of olfactory protein expression on adequate physiological status, or real-time chemical analyses of swarm volatiles with a sensitive apparatus such as a proton-transfer-reaction mass spectrometry could be options. The eventuality that organic compounds with low volatility might be involved in mating process other than long-range attraction cannot be discarded.

The question of how Anopheles females seek male swarms is still open. The lack of post-mating barrier (Persiani et al. 1986; Diabaté et al. 2005, 2007; Hahn et al. 2012; Pombi et al. 2017) and the low rate of hybrids in this geographical area (della Torre et al. 2001, 2005; Tripet et al. 2001; Diabaté et al. 2006) necessarily involve a reasonably distant process which prevents females from entering heterospecific swarms. As acoustic cues have shown some limitations at long range (Feugère et al. 2021), and because long-range chemical cues are still disputable, visual cues such as ground markers (Diabaté et al. 2009; Sawadogo et al. 2014; Poda et al. 2019) could be good candidates, but their effective range and level of specificity is poorly known.

DECLARATIONS

Funding

This work was funded by a grant from the Agence Nationale de la Recherche (ANR-15-CE35-0001-01) awarded to O.R. S.B.P. received financial support through a doctoral fellowship from the Institut de Recherche pour le Développement (IRD).

Ethical approval

The use of rabbit blood was approved by the Ministry of Higher Education, Research and Innovation under the registration No. APAFIS#13817-2018022712203932 v4.

Availability of data and material

The raw datasets are available online: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4719568.

Code availability

Script and codes are available online: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4719568.

Authors’ contributions

O.R. conceived the study. O.R., S.B.P., B.B., B.L. and L.D. designed the chemical and electrophysiological experiments. O.R. and S.B.P. performed chemical extractions and SBP and BB performed the chemical analysis. S.B.P. and B.L. performed the electrophysiological experiments. O.R. and S.B.P. designed olfactometric experiments and S.B.P. performed data collection. S.B.P. and O.R. performed statistical analyses. S.B.P., O.R., B.B., B.L. drafted the manuscript and L.D., O.G., A.D, and R.K.D critically revised the manuscript. All authors revised the manuscript, gave final approval for publication and are accountable for the work performed therein.

Conflict of interest disclosure

The authors of this preprint declare that they have no financial conflict of interest with the content of this article.

SUPPLEMENTARY FILES

Fig. S1:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Fig. S1:

Test for olfactometer symmetry. Female choice, expressed as the proportion of female mosquitoes caught in one or the other collecting box out of the total number retrieved from both collecting boxes for the test with empty boxes (control vs. control combination).

Fig. S2 A:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Fig. S2 A:

Chromatograms obtained with eluted Porapak extracts from headspaces of the control, an An. coluzzii swarm and an An. gambiae swarm (linear retention index (LRI) 800 to 1700). B. Enlarged view of the retention time (RT) interval containing the LRI at which An. coluzzii antennal responses were detected in GC-EAD. Doted white lines correspond to the theoretical RT of Sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal. No difference was detected between the control and the swarm extracts. All the peaks correspond to emissions born from the material.

Fig. S3 A:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Fig. S3 A:

Chromatograms obtained with the desorption of twisters obtained with the static sorptive collection methods under natural conditions in the field (linear retention index (LRI) 800 to 1700). The twister was either located within a non-enclosed swarm of An. coluzzii or 3 m away upwind (control). B. Enlarged view of the retention time (RT) interval containing the LRI at which An. coluzzii antennal responses were detected in GC-EAD. Doted white lines correspond to the theoretical RT of sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal. When in italic, the compound was absent. No difference was detected between the control and the swarm extracts. All the peaks correspond to environmental emissions.

Fig. S4:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Fig. S4:

Quantities (integration units) of acetoin, sulcatone, octanal, nonanal and decanal contained in the headspace of the empty bottles, the breath of the manipulator and the breath of the manipulator after blowing for the second time (breath ×2) sampled with SPME fibers. The intensity values correspond to the counts related to the abundance of the specific ions representative of each molecule formed in the mass spectrometer and correspond to the amount of compound analyzed. The box plots indicate the median (wide horizontal bars), the 25th and 75th percentiles (squares), and the minimum and maximum values (whiskers). The black dots represent outliers and grey dots raw data. NS=not-significant; * =P < 0.05; ** =P < 0.01.

Acknowledgements

We thank David Sanou for his assistance in the field, Stephane Somda, Sofan Somé, Bethsabee Scheid, Marie Rossignol and Carole Ginibre for their help in mosquito rearing and Heidi Lançon for proofreading the paper. Version 6 of this preprint has been peer-reviewed and recommended by Peer Community In Ecology (https://doi.org/10.24072/pci.ecology.100091).

Footnotes

  • Version 6 of this preprint has been peer-reviewed and recommended by Peer Community In Ecology (https://doi.org/10.24072/pci.ecology.100091)

  • https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4719568

REFERENCES

  1. ↵
    1. Choe JC,
    2. Crespi BJ
    Alexander RD, Marshall DC, Cooley JR (1997) Evolutionary perspectives on insect mating. In: Choe JC, Crespi BJ (eds) The evolution of mating systems in insects and Arachnids. Cambridge University Press, pp 4–31
  2. ↵
    Andersson J, Vongvanich N, Wiklund C, et al. (2007) Male sex pheromone release and female mate choice in a butterfly. J Exp Biol 210:964–970. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.02726
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    Bicchi C, Cordero C, Iori C, et al. (2000) Headspace Sorptive Extraction (HSSE) in the headspace analysis of aromatic and medicinal plants. J High Resolut Chromatogr 23:539–546. https://doi.org/10.1002/1521-4168(20000901)23:9<539::AID-JHRC539>3.0.CO;2-3
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    Bimbilé Somda NS, Poda BS, Sawadogo PS, et al. (2018) Ecology of reproduction of Anopheles arabiensis in an urban area of Bobo-Dioulasso, Burkina Faso (West Africa): monthly swarming and mating frequency and their relation to environmental factors. PLoS One 13:e0205966. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0205966
    OpenUrl
  5. ↵
    Bjostad LB, Gaston LK, Shorey HH (1980) Temporal pattern of sex pheromone release by female Trichoplusia ni. J Insect Physiol 26:493–498. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1910(80)90122-5
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  6. ↵
    Bray DP, Alves GB, Dorval ME, et al. (2010) Synthetic sex pheromone attracts the leishmaniasis vector Lutzomyia longipalpis to experimental chicken sheds treated with insecticide. Parasit Vectors 3:1–11. https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-3305-3-16
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Cainap C, Pop LA, Balacescu O, Cainap SS (2020) Early diagnosis and screening in lung cancer. Am J Cancer Res 10:1993–2009
    OpenUrl
  8. ↵
    Calenic B, Amann A (2014) Detection of volatile malodorous compounds in breath: current analytical techniques and implications in human disease. Bioanalysis 6:357–376. https://doi.org/10.4155/bio.13.306
    OpenUrl
  9. ↵
    Charlwood JD, Jones MDR (1980) Mating in the mosquito, Anopheles gambiae s.l. II. Swarming behaviour. Physiol Entomol 5:315–320. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-3032.1980.tb00241.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  10. ↵
    Charlwood JD, Pinto J, Sousa CA, et al. (2002) The swarming and mating behaviour of Anopheles gambiae s.s. (Diptera: Culicidae) from São Tomé Island. J Vector Ecol 27:178–183
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  11. ↵
    Charpentier MJE, Barthes N, Proffit M, et al. (2012) Critical thinking in the chemical ecology of mammalian communication: roadmap for future studies. Funct Ecol 26:769–774. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2435.2012.01998.x
    OpenUrl
  12. ↵
    Clements AN (1999) The biology of mosquitoes. Volume 2: sensory, reception and behaviour. CABI Publishing, Wallingford, UK
  13. ↵
    Clements AN (1992) The biology of mosquitoes: development, nutrition and reproduction. Chapman & Hall, London
  14. ↵
    Costantini C, Ayala D, Guelbeogo MW, et al. (2009) Living at the edge: biogeographic patterns of habitat segregation conform to speciation by niche expansion in Anopheles gambiae. BMC Ecol 9:16. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6785-9-16
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Dao A, Adamou A, Yaro AS, et al. (2008) Assessment of alternative mating strategies in Anopheles gambiae: does mating occur indoors? J Med Entomol 45:643–52
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  16. ↵
    Dekel A, Yakir E, Bohbot JD (2019) The sulcatone receptor of the strict nectar-feeding mosquito Toxorhynchites amboinensis. Insect Biochem Mol Biol 111:103174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ibmb.2019.05.009
    OpenUrl
  17. ↵
    della Torre A, Fanello C, Akogbeto M, et al. (2001) Molecular evidence of incipient speciation within Anopheles gambiae s.s. in West Africa. Insect Mol Biol 10:9–18. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2583.2001.00235.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  18. ↵
    della Torre A, Tu Z, Petrarca V (2005) On the distribution and genetic differentiation of Anopheles gambiae s.s. molecular forms. Insect Biochem Mol Biol 35:755–769. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ibmb.2005.02.006
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  19. ↵
    Diabaté A, Baldet T, Brengues C, et al. (2003) Natural swarming behaviour of the molecular M form of Anopheles gambiae. Trans R Soc Trop Med Hyg 97:713–716. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004549
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Diabaté A, Brengues C, Baldet T, et al. (2004) The spread of the Leu-Phe kdr mutation through Anopheles gambiae complex in Burkina Faso: genetic introgression and de novo phenomena. Trop Med Int Heal 9:1267–1273. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-3156.2004.01336.x
    OpenUrl
  21. ↵
    Diabaté A, Dabiré KR, Kim HE, et al. (2005) Larval development of the molecular forms of Anopheles gambiae (Diptera: Culicidae) in different habitats: a transplantation experiment. J Med Entomol 42:548–553
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  22. ↵
    Diabaté A, Dabiré KR, Millogo N, Lehmann T (2007) Evaluating the effect of postmating isolation between molecular forms of Anopheles gambiae (Diptera: Culicidae). J Med Entomol 44:60–64
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  23. ↵
    Diabaté A, Dabiré RK, Kengné P, et al. (2006) Mixed swarms of the molecular M and S forms of Anopheles gambiae (Diptera: Culicidae) in sympatric area from Burkina Faso. J Med Entomol 43:480–483. https://doi.org/10.1603/0022-2585(2006)43[480:msotmm]2.0.co;2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  24. ↵
    Diabaté A, Dao A, Yaro AS, et al. (2009) Spatial swarm segregation and reproductive isolation between the molecular forms of Anopheles gambiae. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 276:4215–4222. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1167
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Diabaté A, Yaro AS, Dao A, et al. (2011) Spatial distribution and male mating success of Anopheles gambiae swarms. BMC Evol Biol 11:184. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-11-184
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    Dormont L, Bessière J-M, Cohuet A (2013a) Human skin volatiles: a review. J Chem Ecol 39:569–578. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10886-013-0286-z
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Dormont L, Bessiere JM, McKey D, Cohuet A (2013b) New methods for field collection of human skin volatiles and perspectives for their application in the chemical ecology of human-pathogen-vector interactions. J Exp Biol 216:2783–2788. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.085936
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    Downes JA (1969) The swarming and mating flight of Diptera. Annu Rev Entomol 14:271–298. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.en.14.010169.001415
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  29. ↵
    Facchinelli L, Valerio L, Lees RS, et al. (2015) Stimulating Anopheles gambiae swarms in the laboratory: application for behavioural and fitness studies. Malar J 14:271. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12936-015-0792-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    Feugère L, Gibson G, Manoukis NC, Roux O (2021) Mosquito sound communication: are male swarms loud enough to attract females? J R Soc Interface 18:20210121. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2021.0121
    OpenUrl
  31. ↵
    Filipiak W, Filipiak A, Sponring A, et al. (2014) Comparative analyses of volatile organic compounds (VOCs) from patients, tumors and transformed cell lines for the validation of lung cancer-derived breath markers. J Breath Res 8:027111. https://doi.org/10.1088/1752-7155/8/2/027111
    OpenUrl
  32. ↵
    Gibson G (1985) Swarming behaviour of the mosquito Culex pipiens quinquefasciatus: a quantitative analysis. Physiol Entomol 10:283–296. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-3032.1985.tb00049.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  33. ↵
    Gibson G, Warren B, Russell IJ (2010) Humming in tune: sex and species recognition by mosquitoes on the wing. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 11:527–540. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10162-010-0243-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    Gibson NHE (1945) On the mating swarms of certain Chironomidae (Diptera). Trans R Entomol Soc London 95:263–294. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2311.1945.tb00263.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  35. ↵
    Gomulski L (1988) Aspects of mosquito mating behaviour. University of London
  36. ↵
    Goodrich KR, Zjhra ML, Ley CA, Raguso RA (2006) When flowers smell fermented: the chemistry and ontogeny of yeasty floral scent in Pawpaw (Asimina triloba: Annonaceae). Int J Plant Sci 167:33–46. https://doi.org/10.1086/498351
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  37. ↵
    Guidobaldi F, May-Concha IJ, Guerenstein PG (2014) Morphology and physiology of the olfactory system of blood-feeding insects. J. Physiol. Paris 108:96–111
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Hahn MW, White BJ, Muir CD, Besansky NJ (2012) No evidence for biased co-transmission of speciation Islands in Anopheles gambiae. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 367:374–384. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0188
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    Howell PI, Knols BGJJ (2009) Male mating biology. Malar J 8:S8. https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-2875-8-S2-S8
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  40. ↵
    Iatrou K, Biessmann H (2008) Sex-biased expression of odorant receptors in antennae and palps of the African malaria vector Anopheles gambiae. Insect Biochem Mol Biol 38:268–274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ibmb.2007.11.008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  41. ↵
    Kelly DW, Dye C (1997) Pheromones, kairomones and the aggregation dynamics of the sandfly Lutzomyia longipalpis. Anim Behav 53:721–731. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0309
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  42. ↵
    Kostiainen R (1995) Volatile organic compounds in the indoor air of normal and sick houses. Atmos Environ 29:693–702
    OpenUrl
  43. ↵
    Kruza M, Lewis AC, Morrison CG, Carslaw N (2017) Impact of surface ozone interactions on indoor air chemistry: a modeling study. Indoor Air 27:1001–1011. https://doi.org/10.1111/ina.12381
    OpenUrl
  44. ↵
    Lefèvre T, Gouagna LC, Dabire KR, et al. (2009) Evolutionary lability of odour-mediated host preference by the malaria vector Anopheles gambiae. Trop Med Int Heal 14:228–236. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-3156.2009.02206.x
    OpenUrl
  45. ↵
    Lefèvre T, Gouagna LC, Dabiré KR, et al. (2010) Beer consumption increases human attractiveness to malaria mosquitoes. PLoS One 5:e9546. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009546
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    Lehmann T, Diabaté A (2008) The molecular forms of Anopheles gambiae: a phenotypic perspective. Infect Genet Evol 8:737–746. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.meegid.2008.06.003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  47. ↵
    Levi-Zada A, Fefer D, David M, et al. (2014) Diel periodicity of pheromone release by females of Planococcus citri and Planococcus ficus and the temporal flight activity of their conspecific males. Naturwissenschaften 101:671–678. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-014-1206-y
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    Lu T, Qiu YT, Wang G, et al. (2007) Odor coding in the maxillary palp of the malaria vector mosquito Anopheles gambiae. Curr Biol 17:1533–1544. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.062
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  49. ↵
    Maïga H, Dabiré RK, Lehmann T, et al. (2012) Variation in energy reserves and role of body size in the mating system of Anopheles gambiae. J Vector Ecol 37:289–297
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Maïga H, Niang A, Sawadogo SP, et al. (2014) Role of nutritional reserves and body size in Anopheles gambiae males mating success. Acta Trop 132S:S102–S107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actatropica.2013.08.018
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Marchand RP (1984) Field observations on swarming and mating in Anopheles gambiae mosquitoes in Tanzania. Netherlands J Zool 34:367–387
    OpenUrlWeb of Science
  52. ↵
    McBride CS, Baier F, Omondi AB, et al. (2014) Evolution of mosquito preference for humans linked to an odorant receptor. Nature 515:222–227. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13964.Evolution
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  53. ↵
    Merlin C, Lucas P, Rochat D, et al. (2007) An antennal circadian clock and circadian rhythms in peripheral pheromone reception in the moth Spodoptera littoralis. J Biol Rhythms 22:502–514. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730407307737
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  54. ↵
    Mosqueira B, Soma DD, Namountougou M, et al. (2015) Pilot study on the combination of an organophosphate-based insecticide paint and pyrethroid-treated long lasting nets against pyrethroid resistant malaria vectors in Burkina Faso. Acta Trop 148:162–169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actatropica.2015.04.010
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  55. ↵
    Mozūraitis R, Hajkazemian M, Zawada JW, et al. (2020) Male swarming aggregation pheromones increase female attraction and mating success among multiple African malaria vector mosquito species. Nat Ecol Evol 4:1395–1401. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-020-1264-9
    OpenUrl
  56. ↵
    Nguyen PL, Vantaux A, Hien DFS, et al. (2017) No evidence for manipulation of Anopheles gambiae, An. coluzzii and An. arabiensis host preference by Plasmodium falciparum. Sci Rep 7:9415. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-09821-x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  57. ↵
    Niang A, Nignan C, Poda BS, et al. (2019) Semi-field and indoor setups to study malaria mosquito swarming behavior. Parasit Vectors 12:446. https://doi.org/doi.org/10.1186/s13071-019-3688-0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    Nyasembe VO, Tchouassi DP, Kirwa HK, et al. (2014) Development and assessment of plant-based synthetic odor baits for surveillance and control of malaria vectors. PLoS Negl Trop Dis 9:e89818. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089818
    OpenUrl
  59. ↵
    Pandey SK, Kim K (2011) Human body-odor components and their determination. Trends Anal Chem 30:784–796. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trac.2010.12.005
    OpenUrl
  60. ↵
    Pennetier C, Warren B, Dabiré KR, et al. (2010) “Singing on the wing” as a mechanism for species recognition in the malarial mosquito Anopheles gambiae. Curr Biol 20:131–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.040
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  61. ↵
    Persiani A, Dideco MA, Petrangeli G (1986) Osservzioni di laboratorio su polimorfismi da inversione originati da incroci tra popolazioni diverse di Anopheles gambiae s.s. Ann Dell’Istituto Super Di Sanita 22:221–224
    OpenUrl
  62. ↵
    Pitts RJ, Rinker DC, Jones PL, et al. (2011) Transcriptome profiling of chemosensory appendages in the malaria vector Anopheles gambiae reveals tissue- and sex-specific signatures of odor coding. BMC Genomics 12:271. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2164-12-271
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    Poda SB, Buatois B, Lapeyre B, et al. (2021) No evidence for long-range male sex pheromones in two malaria mosquitoes. bioRxiv Prepr. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.05.187542
  64. ↵
    Poda SB, Nignan C, Gnankiné O, et al. (2019) Sex aggregation and species segregation cues in swarming mosquitoes: role of ground visual markers. Parasites and Vectors 12:589. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13071-019-3845-5
    OpenUrl
  65. ↵
    Poda SB, Soma DD, Hien A, et al. (2018) Targeted application of an organophosphate-based paint applied on windows and doors against Anopheles coluzzii resistant to pyrethroids under real life conditions in Vallée du Kou, Burkina Faso (West Africa). Malar J 17:136. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12936-018-2273-x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  66. ↵
    Poli D, Goldoni M, Corradi M, et al. (2010) Determination of aldehydes in exhaled breath of patients with lung cancer by means of on-fiber-derivatisation SPME-GC/MS. J Chromatogr B 878:2643–2651. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jchromb.2010.01.022
    OpenUrl
  67. ↵
    Pombi M, Kengne P, Gimonneau G, et al. (2017) Dissecting functional components of reproductive isolation among closely related sympatric species of the Anopheles gambiae complex. Evol Appl 00:1–19. https://doi.org/10.1111/eva.12517
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  68. ↵
    Robledo N, Arzuffi R (2012) Influence of host fruit and conspecifics on the release of the sex pheromone by Toxotrypana curvicauda males (Diptera: Tephritidae). Environ Entomol 41:387–391.https://doi.org/10.1603/en11037
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    Rund SSC, Bonar NA, Champion MM, et al. (2013) Daily rhythms in antennal protein and olfactory sensitivity in the malaria mosquito Anopheles gambiae. Sci Rep 3:2494. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep02494
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    Santolamazza F, Mancini E, Simard F, et al. (2008) Insertion polymorphisms of SINE200 retrotransposons within speciation islands of Anopheles gambiae molecular forms. Malar J 7:163. https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-2875-7-163
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    Savolainen E (1978) Swarming in Ephemeroptera: the mechanism of swarming and the effects of illumination and weather. Ann Zool Fennici 15:17–52
    OpenUrl
  72. ↵
    Sawadogo PS, Costantini C, Pennetier C, et al. (2013) Differences in timing of mating swarms in sympatric populations of Anopheles coluzzii and Anopheles gambiae s.s. (formerly An. gambiae M and S molecular forms) in Burkina Faso, West Africa. Parasit Vectors 6:275. https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-3305-6-275
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    Sawadogo PS, Namountougou M, Toé KH, et al. (2014) Swarming behaviour in natural populations of Anopheles gambiaeand An. coluzzii: review of 4 years survey in rural areas of sympatry, Burkina Faso (West Africa). Acta Trop 130:24–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actatropica.2013.10.015
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  74. ↵
    Schiestl FP (2010) The evolution of floral scent and insect chemical communication. Ecol Lett 13:643–656. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01451.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  75. ↵
    1. Choe JA,
    2. Crespi JB
    Shelly TE, Whittier TS (1997) Lek behavior of insects. In: Choe JA, Crespi JB (eds) The evolution of mating systems in insects and Arachnids. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 294–309
  76. ↵
    1. Choe JA,
    2. Crespi JB
    Sivinski JM, Petersson E (1997) Mate choice and species isolation in swarming insects. In: Choe JA, Crespi JB (eds) The evolution of mating systems in insects and Arachnids. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 294–309
  77. ↵
    Souto-Vilarós D, Proffit M, Buatois B, et al. (2018) Pollination along an elevational gradient mediated both by floral scent and pollinator compatibility in the fig and fig-wasp mutualism. J Ecol 106:2256–2273. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2745.12995
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  78. ↵
    Suh E, Choe D, Saveer AM, Zwiebel LJ (2016) Suboptimal larval habitats modulate oviposition of the malaria vector mosquito Anopheles coluzzii. PLoS One 11:e0149800. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149800
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    Tchouassi DP, Sang R, Sole CL, et al. (2013) Common host-derived chemicals increase catches of disease-transmitting mosquitoes and can improve early warning systems for rift valley fever virus. PLoS Negl Trop Dis 7:e2007. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pntd.0002007
    OpenUrl
  80. ↵
    Tienpont B, David F, Bicchi C, Sandra P (2000) High capacity headspace sorptive extraction. J Microcolumn Sep 12:577–584. https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-667X(2000)12:11<577::AID-MCS30>3.0.CO;2-Q
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  81. ↵
    Tripet F, Dolo G, Traoré S, Lanzaro GC (2004) The “wingbeat hypothesis” of reproductive isolation between members of the Anopheles gambiae complex (Diptera: Culicidae) does not fly. J Med Entomol 41:375–384. https://doi.org/10.1603/0022-2585-41.3.375
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    Tripet F, Touré YT, Taylor CE, et al. (2001) DNA analysis of transferred sperm reveals significant levels of gene flow between molecular forms of Anopheles gambiae. Mol Ecol 10:1725–1732. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0962-1083.2001.01301.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  83. ↵
    Vantaux A, Hien DFS, Yameogo B, et al. (2015) Host-seeking behaviors of mosquitoes experimentally infected with sympatric field isolates of the human malaria parasite Plasmodium falciparum: no evidence for host manipulation. Front Ecol Evol 3:86. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2015.00086
    OpenUrl
  84. ↵
    Verhulst NO, Andriessen R, Groenhagen U, et al. (2010) Differential attraction of malaria mosquitoes to volatile blends produced by human skin bacteria. PLoS One 5:e15829. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015829
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    Wang G, Diabaté A, Liu J, et al. (2021) Clock genes and environmental cues coordinate Anopheles pheromone synthesis, swarming, and mating. Science (80-) 371:411–415. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abd4359
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  86. ↵
    Wondwosen B, Birgersson G, Seyoum E, et al. (2016) Rice volatiles lure gravid malaria mosquitoes, Anopheles arabiensis. Sci Rep 6:37930. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep37930
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  87. ↵
    Wondwosen B, Birgersson G, Tekie H, et al. (2018) Sweet attraction: sugarcane pollen-associated volatiles attract gravid Anopheles arabiensis. Malar J 17:90. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12936-018-2245-1
    OpenUrl
  88. ↵
    Zellner B d’Acampora, Bicchi C, Dugo P, et al. (2008) Linear retention indices in gas chromatographic analysis: a review. Flavour Fragr J 23:297–314. https://doi.org/10.1002/ffj
    OpenUrlCrossRef
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted January 11, 2022.
Download PDF
Data/Code
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
No Evidence for Long-range Male Sex Pheromones in Two Malaria Mosquitoes
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
No Evidence for Long-range Male Sex Pheromones in Two Malaria Mosquitoes
Serge Bèwadéyir Poda, Bruno Buatois, Benoit Lapeyre, Laurent Dormont, Abdoulaye Diabaté, Olivier Gnankiné, Roch K. Dabiré, Olivier Roux
bioRxiv 2020.07.05.187542; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.05.187542
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo LinkedIn logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
No Evidence for Long-range Male Sex Pheromones in Two Malaria Mosquitoes
Serge Bèwadéyir Poda, Bruno Buatois, Benoit Lapeyre, Laurent Dormont, Abdoulaye Diabaté, Olivier Gnankiné, Roch K. Dabiré, Olivier Roux
bioRxiv 2020.07.05.187542; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.05.187542

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Ecology
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (4239)
  • Biochemistry (9172)
  • Bioengineering (6804)
  • Bioinformatics (24063)
  • Biophysics (12155)
  • Cancer Biology (9564)
  • Cell Biology (13825)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (7658)
  • Ecology (11737)
  • Epidemiology (2066)
  • Evolutionary Biology (15540)
  • Genetics (10672)
  • Genomics (14359)
  • Immunology (9511)
  • Microbiology (22901)
  • Molecular Biology (9129)
  • Neuroscience (49113)
  • Paleontology (357)
  • Pathology (1487)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (2583)
  • Physiology (3851)
  • Plant Biology (8351)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (1473)
  • Synthetic Biology (2301)
  • Systems Biology (6205)
  • Zoology (1302)