Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

The Aging Brain and Executive Functions Revisited: Implications from Meta-Analytic and Functional-Connectivity Evidence

View ORCID ProfileMarisa K. Heckner, View ORCID ProfileEdna C. Cieslik, View ORCID ProfileSimon B. Eickhoff, View ORCID ProfileJulia A. Camilleri, View ORCID ProfileFelix Hoffstaedter, View ORCID ProfileRobert Langner
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.15.204941
Marisa K. Heckner
1Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-7: Brain and Behaviour), Research Centre Jülich, Jülich, Germany
2Institute of Systems Neuroscience, Medical Faculty, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Marisa K. Heckner
  • For correspondence: m.heckner@fz-juelich.de r.langner@fz-juelich.de
Edna C. Cieslik
1Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-7: Brain and Behaviour), Research Centre Jülich, Jülich, Germany
2Institute of Systems Neuroscience, Medical Faculty, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Edna C. Cieslik
Simon B. Eickhoff
1Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-7: Brain and Behaviour), Research Centre Jülich, Jülich, Germany
2Institute of Systems Neuroscience, Medical Faculty, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Simon B. Eickhoff
Julia A. Camilleri
1Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-7: Brain and Behaviour), Research Centre Jülich, Jülich, Germany
2Institute of Systems Neuroscience, Medical Faculty, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Julia A. Camilleri
Felix Hoffstaedter
1Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-7: Brain and Behaviour), Research Centre Jülich, Jülich, Germany
2Institute of Systems Neuroscience, Medical Faculty, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Felix Hoffstaedter
Robert Langner
1Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-7: Brain and Behaviour), Research Centre Jülich, Jülich, Germany
2Institute of Systems Neuroscience, Medical Faculty, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Robert Langner
  • For correspondence: m.heckner@fz-juelich.de r.langner@fz-juelich.de
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

Healthy aging is associated with changes in cognitive performance including executive functions (EFs) and their associated brain activation patterns. However, it has remained unclear which EF-related brain regions are affected consistently, because the results of pertinent neuroimaging studies and earlier meta-analyses vary considerably. We, therefore, conducted new rigorous meta-analyses of published age differences in EF-related brain activity. Out of a larger set of regions associated with EFs, only left inferior frontal junction (IFJ) and left anterior cuneus/precuneus (aC/PrC) were found to show consistent age differences. To further characterize these two age-sensitive regions, we performed seed-based resting-state functional connectivity (RS-FC) analyses using fMRI data from a large adult sample with a wide age range. We also assessed associations of the two regions’ whole-brain RS-FC patterns with age and EF performance. Although functional profiling and RS-FC analyses point towards a domain-general role of left IFJ in EFs, the pattern of individual study contributions to the meta-analytic results suggests process-specific modulations by age. Our analyses further indicate that left aC/PrC is recruited differently by older (compared to younger) adults during EF tasks, potentially reflecting inefficiencies in switching the attentional focus. Overall, our findings question earlier meta-analytic results and suggest a larger heterogeneity of age-related differences in brain activity associated with EFs. Hence, they encourage future research that pays greater attention to replicability, investigates age-related differences in deactivation, and focuses on more narrowly defined EF subprocesses, combining multiple behavioral assessments with multi-modal imaging.

Highlights - Healthy aging is linked to deterioration in executive functions (EFs)

- ALE meta-analyses examined consistent age differences in brain activity linked to EFs

- In a larger set of EF regions, only left IFJ and (pre)cuneus were sensitive to age

- Advanced age was linked to weaker functional coupling within EF-related networks

- Our findings question earlier meta-analytic findings

1. Introduction

1.1 Executive Functions

Executive functions (EFs) are a loosely defined set of cognitive control processes that are taken to be critical for goal-directed thought and behavior in complex environments. Despite the lack of a clear formal definition of EFs as well as their ambiguous mapping on typical EF-tasks, there is relative agreement on their importance for regulating human behavior through modulating cognition in a top-down fashion (Diamond, 2013; Jurado & Rosselli, 2007). Different lines of research on how EFs might be best fractionated into subcomponents suggest models that argue for the existence of three core EFs: inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive set shifting (e.g., Lehto, 1996; Miyake et al., 2000; for reviews see: Alvarez & Emory, 2006; Diamond, 2013). We acknowledge, however, that this differentiation is not undisputed (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Engle & Kane, 2004; Norman & Shallice, 1986; Stuss, 2006).

For a long time, it was thought that EFs were exclusively based on frontal lobe functioning as patients with frontal lesions often showed deficits in EFs leading to the interchangeable use of the terms “executive dysfunction” and “frontal lobe dysfunction” (e.g., Duncan, 1986; Owen et al., 1990; Shallice et al., 1982). However, patients with frontal lesions can perform within a normal range on EF tasks (e.g., Eslinger & Damasio, 1985; Shallice & Burgess, 1991) and patients with non-frontal lesions can show similar deficits like patients with frontal lesions (e.g., Anderson et al., 1991; Mountain & Snow, 1993). Years of research led Don Stuss and his collaborators (1995; 2006; 2011) to the assumption that there is a substantial fractionation of frontal lobe functions and that EFs represent only one functional category within the frontal lobes. Previous neuroimaging studies have revealed notable differences in the brain regions involved in EFs, which may be partly due to the elusive conceptualization of EFs (Collette et al., 2006) as well as the wealth of different perspectives, operationalizations, and traditions in this research, which have resulted in a co-existence of rather diverse labels for the brain networks and regions associated with EFs (Camilleri et al., 2018). Although there are differences between different tasks probing EFs, there also seem to be core regions consistently involved, like left inferior frontal junction (IFJ; e.g., Emery et al., 2008; Milham et al., 2002; Zysset et al., 2007). Duncan and collaborators (Duncan, 2010; Duncan & Owen, 2000; Fedorenko et al., 2013) investigated and defined these core regions and proposed that a “multiple-demand” (MD) brain system was consistently recruited during all kinds of cognitively demanding tasks.

Müller et al. (2015) used a similar approach: They integrated results from three neuroimaging meta-analyses investigating working memory (Rottschy et al., 2012), vigilant attention (Langner & Eickhoff, 2013), and inhibitory control (Cieslik et al., 2015), highly discussed subcomponents of EFs (Alvarez & Emory, 2006; Miyake et al., 2000), to define a common core network for EFs. This network was similar to Duncan’s MD system and comprised seven regions: mid-cingulate cortex/supplementary motor area (MCC/SMA), bilateral IFJ/inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), right middle frontal gyrus (MFG), bilateral anterior insula (aIns), right inferior parietal cortex (IPC), and intraparietal sulcus (IPS). Camilleri et al. (2018) went on to propose an extended MD network (eMDN) based on task-dependent and task-independent functional connectivity (FC) analyses seeded from the regions of the meta-analytically defined MD network by Müller and colleagues, to consider the perspective of a more widely distributed network. Camilleri et al. reported 17 regions as part of the eMDN (bilateral IFJ, aIns, SMA/pre-SMA, IPS, putamen, thalamus, MFG extending into the inferior frontal sulcus, dorsal premotor cortex [dPMC], and left inferior temporal gyrus). While the current paper focuses on EF-related activations, for the sake of completeness, we consider it necessary to briefly mention the functional relevance of the default-mode network (DMN) as it is assumed to be activated during stimulusindependent or spontaneous cognition and deactivated during externally focused cognition. The DMN comprises a network of brain regions that includes the precuneus (PrC)/posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), anterior medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and lateral inferior parietal cortex (Shulman et al., 1997; for reviews see: Anticevic et al., 2012; Raichle, 2015). In the interest of space, we did not go into more detail and referred to reviews on this topic.

Taken together, EFs seem to be a macro-construct rather than a single process, which involves distributed networks instead of any particular region, with a core network and more specific regions that are recruited depending on certain task demands (Camilleri et al., 2018; Miyake & Friedman, 2012; Teuber, 1972).

1.2 Healthy Aging

Healthy aging is associated with altered cognitive performance and brain activation patterns in several cognitive domains, especially non-routine tasks that tax executive control processes (Drag & Bieliauskas, 2010; Park et al., 2002; Stuss & Craik, 2019). Although the aging brain faces unfavorable changes, such as the decline of dopaminergic receptors (Li et al., 2001; Yang et al., 2003), volumetric shrinkage of many grey-matter structures (Raz et al., 2005; Resnick et al., 2003; Salat et al., 2004), and reduced white-matter density (Head et al., 2004; Wen & Sachdev, 2004), it also seems to aim for an allostatic maintenance of cognitive functions through functional reorganization. This indicates that the neurobiological substrates of our cognitive system are highly dynamic and adaptive across the lifespan (Greenwood, 2007; Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2008).

A common finding is the reduced lateralization of brain activation in older adults, which is thought to be compensatory as it is correlated with better performance in older adults (“hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults” [HAROLD]; Cabeza, 2002). Furthermore, brain activation shifts from posterior to more anterior brain regions have been observed (“posterior– anterior shift in aging” [PASA]; Davis et al., 2007), which might be caused by older adults’ need for exerting executive control for previously automated operations. Additionally, it has been hypothesized that age-related cognitive and behavioral changes are associated with less specialized brain responses and a decrease in FC with age, in the context of structural and neurobiological changes as well as external experiences (Baltes & Lindenberger, 1997; Goh, 2011; Li & Sikström, 2002; Park et al., 2001; 2004). Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell (2008) postulated that the oft-reported increase in task-related lateral PFC activity with age compensates for less efficient neural circuits (“compensation-related utilization of neural circuits hypothesis” [CRUNCH]). Finally, Park and Reuter-Lorenz (2008) attempted to unite previous theories in their “scaffolding theory of aging and cognition” (STAC). In this context, “scaffolds” describe a supportive framework that helps maintain cognitive and behavioral performance at a relatively high level through advanced age via the strengthening of existing connections, development of new connections, and disuse of connections that have become fragile or deficient. These changes, in turn, are assumed to lead to increased bilateral and/or frontal activation in older adults.

Results from neuroimaging studies on age-related changes in the EF subcomponents working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility are rather ambiguous. While some studies reported an increase in bilateral prefrontal activity (e.g., Emery et al., 2008; Madden et al., 1999) and a decrease in occipital activity (e.g., Ansado et al., 2012; Madden et al., 2002, 2010), other studies reported occipital activity increase (e.g., Bloemendaal et al., 2016; Van Impe et al., 2011) and frontal activity decrease in older adults (e.g., Bloemendaal et al., 2016; Schulte et al., 2011). Moreover, the age-related reduction in hemispheric asymmetry of brain activity is not found consistently (e.g., Carp et al., 2010; Toepper et al., 2014). This large amount of heterogeneous, partly contradictory findings illustrates the need for a quantitative data synthesis by means of meta-analysis in combination with taking a systems-level perspective, which includes identifying the connectional profiles of the identified regions with respect to the rest of the brain.

So far, three quantitative neuroimaging meta-analyses investigating cognitive aging in EFs have been published, each with their own limitations as discussed below: Spreng et al. (2010) performed an analysis across all then available experiments probing EFs in age, such as working memory, task switching, and inhibitory control. The authors found consistent EF-related increases in activity with age in bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), right posterior MFG/frontal eye field (FEF), left SMA, and left rostrolateral PFC as well as consistent decreases in activity with age in right ventrolateral PFC. Next, Turner and Spreng (2012) conducted separate meta-analyses for the EF subcomponents working memory and inhibition and found domain-specific patterns of across-experiment convergence. For working memory, consistent increases in activity with age were found in bilateral SMA, right MFG, left IFG, and left IPS; consistently lower activity in older adults was found in right IPS, left aIns/frontal operculum, and left FEF. For inhibition-related brain activity, consistent increases in activity with age was found in right MFG/IFG and left superior frontal gyrus, whereas consistent decreases in activity with age was found in right inferior occipital gyrus. Finally, a third meta-analysis by Di et al. (2014) found consistent increases in EF-related activation with age in bilateral IFG, left anterior cerebellum, left fusiform gyrus (FG), right MFG, and right parahippocampal gyrus. Consistently lower EF-related activation with age was found in bilateral Ins, left MFG, left medial frontal gyrus, and right MCC.

Taken together, the picture produced by these meta-analyses is largely inconclusive. This inconsistency across meta-analyses might result from methodological differences, such as the in- or exclusion of region-of-interest (ROI) contrasts, the particular selection of tasks included, or the approach to testing for age-related differences. Furthermore, all previous meta-analyses corrected for multiple comparisons by controlling the voxel-level false discovery rate (FDR), which has recently been shown to feature low sensitivity and a high susceptibility for false-positive findings in Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) meta-analysis (Eickhoff et al., 2016). In light of these inconsistencies and limitations of earlier efforts as well as the continued growth of the pertinent literature since 2014, a fresh meta-analysis on age-related differences in brain activity associated with EFs appeared much warranted.

1.3 Current Study

In a first step, coordinate-based ALE meta-analysis (Eickhoff et al., 2009, 2012; Turkeltaub et al., 2002, 2012) was used to synthesize results from neuroimaging studies investigating EFs in young and old participants. We started with a meta-analysis of within-group findings, pooling across experimental results obtained in young or old participants, respectively. This approach should test for consistent general EF-related brain activity in our sample of experiments, without regard to age-related differences. It was aimed at replicating previous findings of brain regions involved in EFs. Subsequently, we conducted further meta-analyses of published between-group contrasts, investigating consistent age differences in EF-related brain activity. As a methodological improvement over previous ALE meta-analyses on this topic, we used cluster-level family-wise error (FWE) correction for multiple comparisons (rather than FDR-based correction) and a minimum number of n = 17 experiments per analysis, following the recommendations by Eickhoff et al. (2016).

In a second step, task-independent whole-brain FC patterns of resulting age-sensitive regions were analyzed using resting-state (RS) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data of healthy adults. Finally, we assessed the associations of the regions’ whole-brain FC with age and performance scores representing EF and its subcomponents in order to gain further insights into the mechanisms underlying cognitive aging.

In summary, this study aimed to investigate (i) which brain regions show consistent age differences in EF-related activity at the meta-analytic level, (ii) the connectional profiles of these age-sensitive regions, and (iii) how the connectivity profiles of these regions are affected by aging and EF-capacity.

2. Methods

2.1 ALE Meta-Analysis

2.1.1. Sample

2.1.1.1. Search for Studies

Pertinent studies were searched for in the databases Web of Science, PubMed (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/), PsycINFO (http://ovidsp.tx.ovid.com), and Google Scholar (http://scholar.google.de) using the following search strings: (1) title: “age” or “aging” or “ageing” or “age-related” or “older adults” or “old adults” or “life-span” or “elderly adults”; and (2) title: “executive functions” or “working memory” or “inhibition” or “cognitive flexibility”; and (3) abstract: “fMRI” or “functional magnetic resonance imaging” or “PET” or “positron emission tomography” or “neuroimaging” or “cerebral blood flow.” Subsequently, specific EFrelated task labels were included in the search string as follows: for working memory, “n-back” or “Sternberg” or “delayed match* to sample” or “delayed simple matching”; for inhibitory control, “Stroop” or “flanker” or “Simon” or “stimulus-response compatibility” or “stop signal” or “go/no-go” or “stimulus detection” or “stimulus discrimination” or “selective attention”; and for cognitive flexibility, “task switching” or “dual task” or “set shifting.” The search criteria were partially motivated by previous meta-analyses regarding aging and EFs (Di et al., 2014; Spreng et al., 2010; Turner & Spreng, 2012). The decision on which tasks to include in the extended search string was made based on Diamond’s (2013) definition of typical tasks for each of the subcategories. Finally, earlier meta-analyses on this topic, reviews, and the reference lists of identified studies were inspected for additional studies to be included.

2.1.1.2. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

We included only peer-reviewed publications of fMRI or positron emission tomography (PET) experiments performed in healthy young and old participants without any pharmacological manipulations or other extraneous interventions. Results of group analyses needed to be reported as coordinates of a standard reference space, that is, MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) or Talairach (Talairach & Tournoux, 1988) space. Studies were only included if the whole brain was covered (i.e. coverage of at least 8 cm in the z-dimension). Consequently, no ROI-based results were included. However, some of the experiments we included reported masking of the between-group contrast with the task-positive main effect to restrict group differences to task-related regions (these studies are marked in Tables A1 and A2). We included results from contrasts between task and sensorimotor control or resting-baseline conditions, contrasts between different levels of task difficulty, as well as correlations between age and task-related activity.

Thus, deactivation data, results from connectivity analyses, or correlations and interactions with other variables (e.g., group × performance interactions, correlations with reaction time, etc.) were not considered. In case of uncertainty as to any of these criteria, the corresponding author of the given study was contacted for clarification (these studies are marked in Tables A1 and A2).

To minimize the risk that meta-analytic results were unduly biased by a particular publication, the contribution from any given study was limited to one experiment. If a study reported several experiments eligible for inclusion, their findings (i.e., reported coordinates) were pooled to constitute a single experiment, as suggested by Turkeltaub et al. (2012). Further, if contrasts for both transient and sustained brain activity were available, the contrast reflecting transient activity was chosen as it typically allows for a more process-specific interpretation. For the current approach, coordinates of within-group (i.e., main task effect per group) and between-group (i.e., contrast of task effects between groups: [young > old, old > young]) contrasts as well as correlations between task performance and age were included.

2.1.1.3. Studies Included

After an initial screening of publication abstracts for topicality, 147 studies were retrieved in total. Applying the above criteria left us with 31 eligible studies reporting within-group task effects: 11 for working memory, 12 for inhibition, and 9 for cognitive flexibility. Of note, the study by Townsend et al. (2006) contributed results to two subdomains (inhibition and cognitive flexibility).

In the meta-analyses of age-related differences, 46 eligible studies were included in total: 15 for working memory, 19 for inhibition, 14 for cognitive flexibility, and 1 not clearly assignable to any subdomain. For clarification, not all studies included both within- and between-group contrasts, leading to somewhat different numbers of studies included in the within- and between-group meta-analyses, respectively. Of note, Eich et al. (2016) and Townsend et al. (2006) were included for inhibition and cognitive flexibility, and Lamar et al. (2004) for inhibition and working memory. Studies reporting different tasks (i.e., experiments that contribute to different subdomains) were pooled by the respective subdomain (vs. by subject group) and may thus contribute with two data points. To make sure that this pooling by subcomponent would not have an effect on the results, we additionally computed the meta-analyses with solely one point of data per study, i.e., pooling by subject group. The results were the same. For the sake of interpretability, we therefore decided to pool the aforementioned studies by subcomponent. See Figure 1 for an overview of the different analysis steps conducted.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 1.

Flowchart of the meta-analysis steps conducted.

For further information about the studies included, please see Tables A1 and A2. A checklist for neuroimaging meta-analyses as recommended by Müller et al. (2018) can be found in Table A3.

2.1.2. Activation Likelihood Estimation

2.1.2.1. ALE Algorithm

All meta-analyses were conducted using the revised version of the ALE algorithm for coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging results (Eickhoff et al., 2009, 2012; Turkeltaub et al., 2002, 2012) implemented as in-house MATLAB tools. This algorithm aims to identify areas with across-experiment convergence of activity foci that is higher than expected from random spatial association. Before analysis, any coordinates reported in Talairach space were transformed into MNI space (Lancaster et al., 2007). Because the standard brain templates used in SPM (statistical parametric mapping) since version SPM96 and in FSL (FMRIB Software Library) are given in MNI space, reported results from analyses using SPM or FSL were treated as MNI coordinates unless the authors explicitly mentioned a transformation from MNI to Talairach space or the use of an alternative brain template.

In a first step, important content of the included studies was coded and recorded. In a second step, the reported coordinates of each experiment’s peak activations (“foci”) were projected on a brain template, acknowledging the spatial uncertainty associated with each coordinate by modeling Gaussian probability distributions around each focus. Third, the probability distributions of all activation foci were combined for each voxel, resulting in a modeled activation map. The union of these modeled activation maps then yielded voxel-wise ALE scores, which were compared to a null distribution reflecting a random spatial association between experiments. The p-value of a “true” ALE score was then given by the proportion of equal or higher values obtained under the null distribution. The resulting non-parametric p values for each meta-analysis were cut off at a threshold of p < .05 (family-wise error corrected at cluster level; cluster inclusion threshold at voxel level: p < .001).

2.1.2.2. Meta-Analyses Conducted

First, a meta-analysis pooling across within-group task effects (i.e., main task effects for both age groups) was conducted on all experiments to examine the main effect of performing EF tasks on brain activity independent of age. Second, for examining age-related effects, we performed three different meta-analyses of between-group contrasts: (1) pooled, (2) old > young, and (3) young > old. We also aimed to conduct separate meta-analyses for each EF subcomponent, but only for inhibition more than 17 experiments were found to be eligible. Hence, a comparison between EF subcomponents was not possible. The results of the inhibition-specific meta-analyses can be found in Tables A5 and A6.

Pooled analyses search for consistent group differences in EF-related brain activity, independent of the direction of the between-group effect. For neuroimaging findings, pooled meta-analyses may provide the best summary because the directions of group differences in individual experiments depend on how exactly these differences were calculated, which varies widely between studies: Some authors compute task versus control contrasts at the individual-subject level, which are then compared between old and young adults at group level, whereas others compute group (old versus young) by task (task versus control or baseline) interactions at the second level. As control conditions strongly vary between experiments (from resting baseline to high-level control tasks), effects of between-group activation differences in these control conditions may influence the overall direction of group differences unpredictably (Müller et al., 2017).

2.2 Resting-State Functional Connectivity

To further characterize EF-related brain regions consistently affected by aging (i.e., regions with significant convergence in the pooled age-related meta-analysis), we investigated their RS-FC patterns. Therefore, whole-brain RS-FC analyses were conducted. RS-fMRI images of 413 healthy adults were obtained from the publicly available enhanced Nathan Kline Institute - Rockland Sample (eNKI-RS; Nooner et al., 2012; age range = 18 – 80; mean age = 44.85; SD = 18.51; 272 females). The re-analysis of the data was approved by the local ethics committee of the Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf. Images were obtained with a Siemens TimTrio 3T scanner using BOLD (blood-oxygen-level-dependent) contrast [gradient-echo EPI (echo planar imaging) pulse sequence, TR = 1.4 s, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 65°, voxel size = 2.0 × 2.0 × 2.0 mm3, 64 slices]. 404 volumes were acquired. Participants were instructed to keep their eyes open and maintain fixation on a central dot. Physiological and movement artifacts were removed from RS data by using FIX (FMRIB’s ICA-based Xnoiseifier, version 1.061 as implemented in FSL 5.0.9; Griffanti et al., 2014; Salimi-Khorshidi et al., 2014), which decomposes the data into independent components and identifies noise components using a large number of distinct spatial and temporal features via pattern classification. Unique variance related to the identified artifactual components is then regressed from the data. Data were further preprocessed using SPM12 (Wellcome Trust Centre Neuroimaging, London) and in-house Matlab scripts. After removing the first four dummy scans of each time series, the remaining EPI volumes were then corrected for head movement by a two-pass affine registration procedure: first, images were aligned to the initial volume and, subsequently, to the mean of all volumes. The mean EPI image was then coregistered to the gray-matter probability map provided by SPM12 using normalized mutual information and keeping all EPI volumes aligned. Next, the mean EPI image of each subject was spatially normalized to MNI-152 space using the “unified segmentation” approach (Ashburner & Friston, 2000). The resulting deformation parameters were then applied to all other EPI volumes. Finally, data were spatially smoothed with a 5-mm FWHM (full width at half maximum) Gaussian kernel to improve the signal-to-noise ratio and to compensate for residual anatomic variations.

The BOLD signal time-courses of all voxels within each seed region, expressed as the first eigenvariate, were extracted for each subject. To reduce spurious correlations, variance explained by the mean white-matter and cerebrospinal-fluid signal were removed from the time series together with 24 movement parameters (including derivatives and 2nd-order effects; cf. Satterthwaite et al., 2013), which was subsequently band-pass filtered with the cut-off frequencies of .01 and .08 Hz. Linear (Pearson) correlations between the time series of the seed regions and all other grey-matter voxels in the brain were computed to quantify RS-FC. The resulting voxel-wise correlation coefficients were then transformed into Fisher’s Z-scores and entered in a group-level analysis of variance. The results of this random-effects analysis were masked with the subjects’ mean Z-scores >= .1 and thresholded at a voxel-level FWE-corrected threshold of one-sided p < .05. Here, we chose one-sided testing, as our hypotheses were directed (i.e., we were only interested in the positive coupling between our seed regions and the rest of the brain). An additional extent threshold of 10 contiguous voxels was applied to exclude smaller, potentially spurious clusters.

2.3 Association of RS-FC with Age and EF Abilities

In the same sample of 413 adults, we also examined the association of the seed region’s whole-brain RS-FC with age as well as EF abilities using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). For assessing EF abilities, we computed four compound scores: a total score and three subscores, each representing a particular EF subcomponent (i.e., working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility). The cognitive tasks used were also obtained from the eNKI-RS. Performance raw scores were z-transformed - outliers > |3| standard deviations were removed - and added up to calculate EF subcomponent scores as follows: The working memory compound score consisted of reaction time (RT) and error rate (ER) of the 2-back and 1-back conditions of the Short Letter-N-Back Test, which is part of Penn’s Computerized Neurocognitive Battery (CNB; Gur et al., 2010). The inhibition compound score consisted of (i) the conflict effect of the Attention Network Task (ANT; Fan et al., 2002), (ii) RT and ER of the Color-Word Interference Test, which is part of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS; Delis et al., 2004), and (iii) RT and ER of the Short Penn Continuous Performance Test (Number and Letter Versions), which is also part of Penn’s CNB. The cognitive flexibility compound score consisted of RT of the Trail Making Test, which is part of the D-KEFS, as well as of RT and ER of the Penn Conditional Exclusion Task, part of Penn’s CNB. Finally, for the total EF score, all single scores were added up and divided by the absolute number of scores. All compound scores were multiplied by -1. Hence, higher scores represent higher performance. The results of the ANCOVA were masked with the RS-FC map of the respective seed region (as described above, see section 2.2.) and thresholded at a voxel-level FWE-corrected threshold of two-sided p < .00625 (additional extent threshold of 10 contiguous voxels). The p-value was adjusted for multiple comparisons as we tested four models, and adjusted for two-sided testing as recommended by Chen et al. (2018). All results were anatomically labeled by reference to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps of the human brain using the SPM Anatomy Toolbox version 3 (Eickhoff et al., 2005, 2007) and visualized with the BrainNet Viewer (Xia et al., 2013).

3. Results

3.1 Meta-Analyses

3.1.1. Analysis of EF-related Effects Across Age

A meta-analysis across both age groups and all experiments (reflecting all three EF subcomponents) was conducted to examine the general main effect of taxing EFs on regional brain activity. Significant convergence across experiments was found in left IFJ, left pre-SMA, left IPS/SPL, left mid-FG, left central Ins, and right frontal pole/MFG (see Table 1 and Figure 2).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1

Brain Regions Showing Significant Convergence of Activity in Executive Functions

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 2.

Foci of brain activity showing significant convergence of activity for EFs across age (cluster-level p < .05, family-wise error-corrected for multiple comparisons, cluster-forming threshold at voxel level: p < .001). The scale bar reflects the maximum z-score of the local maxima.

3.1.2. Analyses of Age-related Differences

We performed three different meta-analyses of contrasts between age groups: (1) pooled, (2) old > young, and (3) young > old. The pooled meta-analysis, which included all experiments (n = 49) that probed age differences in EF-related brain activity irrespective of the contrast’s direction, revealed only two regions with significant convergence of such age differences: left IFJ and left anterior cuneus/precuneus (aC/PrC; see Table 1 and Figure 3A). Convergence in left IFJ was almost equally driven by experiments probing working memory (32.95%), inhibition (28.54%), and cognitive flexibility (38.41%). Furthermore, it was more strongly driven by experiments contrasting old > young (60.24%) than by experiments contrasting young > old (39.7%). Convergent activity in left aC/PrC was also driven by experiments on working memory (24.9%), inhibition (41.91%), and cognitive flexibility (32.45%). In contrast to left IFJ, however, it was almost exclusively driven by old > young contrasts (91.68%). Please see Table A4 for a full overview of the study contributions.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 3.

Foci of brain activity showing significant convergence of activity for (A) EFs pooled, (B) EFs old > young (cluster-level p < .05, family-wise error-corrected for multiple comparisons, cluster-forming threshold at voxel level: p < .001). The scale bar reflects the maximum z-score of the local maxima.

The meta-analysis testing for consistently lower brain activity across EF experiments in older (vs. younger) adults (n = 31) did not yield any significant convergence. Conversely, the meta-analysis testing for consistently higher activity across EFs experiments in older (vs. younger) adults (n = 42) revealed significant convergence in left aC/PrC (see Table 1 and Figure 3B).

We also aimed to conduct separate meta-analyses for each EF subcomponent, but only for inhibition more than 17 experiments were found to be eligible. The results of the inhibition-specific meta-analyses can be found in Tables A5 and A6.

3.2 Connectional Characterization

The two age-sensitive regions resulting from the pooled meta-analysis (i.e., left IFJ and left aC/PrC) were connectionally characterized by conducting whole-brain RS-FC analyses. The RS-FC map obtained for left IFJ comprised 14 clusters of significant coupling: the seed region extending into DLPFC, MFG, FEF, dPMC, SMA/pre-SMA, frontal pole, and aIns; left caudate nucleus; left IPS extending into FG and SPL; two clusters in left cerebellum VII; right IFJ extending into DLPFC, FEF, dPMC, and frontal pole; right cerebellum VI and VII; right IPS/angular gyrus; right FG extending into Wernicke’s region; right SMA/pre-SMA; right aIns, right primary somatosensory cortex (S1); and bilateral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; see Table 2 and Figure 4A).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 4.

Whole-brain RS-FC analyses of (A) left IFJ and (B) left aC/PrC (voxel-level family-wise error corrected threshold of one-sided p < .05, extent threshold = 20, masked with the subjects’ mean Z-scores >=.I). The scale bar reflects t-scores.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2

RS-FC Analyses

The RS-FC analysis of left aC/PrC yielded 13 clusters: the seed region extending into bilateral PCC, FG, subiculum, calcarine gyrus, and left IPL; left frontal pole extending into subgenual area, FEF, and bilateral frontopolar cortex; left posterior Insula (pIns) extending into parietal operculum; left cerebellum VII; two clusters in right cerebellum IX and VII; bilateral temporoparietal junction (TPJ); and four clusters in bilateral IFG pars orbitalis (see Table 2 and Figure 4B).

3.3 Association of RS-FC with Age and EF Abilities

3.3.1. Age

An ANCOVA was performed to examine the association between the seed regions’ RS-FC patterns and age. We observed significant negative associations with age for RS-FC between left IFJ and 10 clusters: left aIns, left FEF, left TPJ, bilateral IFJ/DLPFC, bilateral FG, and bilateral aCC (see Table 3 and Figure 5A).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 5.

Significant negative association between whole-brain RS-FC of (A) left IFJ and age and (B) aC/PrC and age, (voxel-level family-wise error-corrected threshold of two-sided p <.00625, extent threshold = 10, masked with RS-FC map of left IFJ and aC/PrC, respectively). The scale bar reflects t-scores.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3

Association of RS-FC and Age

Age was also significantly negatively associated with RS-FC between left aC/PrC and 5 regions: the seed region extending into bilateral visual cortex, left Heschl’s gyrus extending into planum temporale, left SPL, left S1, and left thalamus (see Table 3 and Figure 5B). Finally, age was significantly positively associated with RS-FC between left aC/PrC and 3 regions: left IPL, left PrC, and left TPJ (see Table 3 and Figure 6).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 6.

Significant positive association between whole-brain RS-FC of left aC/PrC and age, (voxel-level family-wise error-corrected threshold of two-sided p < .00625, extent threshold = 20, masked with RS-FC map of left aC/PrC). The scale bar reflects t-scores.

3.3.2. EF Abilities

Finally, we performed an ANCOVA to assess the association between the seed regions’ RS-FC patterns and EF abilities. RS-FC between left aC/PrC and the seed region extending into bilateral visual cortices was significantly positively associated with the total EF score (see Table 4 and Figure 7).

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 7.

Significant positive association between whole-brain RS-FC of left aC/PrC and executive functions, (voxel-level family-wise error-corrected threshold at two-sided p < .00625, extent threshold = 10, masked with RS-FC map of left aC/PrC). The scale bar reflects t-scores.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4

Association of RS-FC and Combined Total Executive Functions and Cognitive Flexibility Compound Scores

We found a significant negative association of the cognitive flexibility score with RS-FC between left aC/PrC and 3 regions: bilateral IPL and right middle temporal gyrus (MTG; see Table 4 and Figure 8A), whereas RS-FC between left aC/PrC and the seed region extending into bilateral visual cortices was significantly positively associated with the cognitive flexibility score (see Table 4 and Figure 8B).

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 8.

(A) Significant negative and (B) positive association between whole-brain RS-FC of left aC/PrC and cognitive flexibility, (voxel-level family-wise error-corrected threshold at two sided p < .00625, extent threshold = 10, masked with RS-FC map of left aC/PrC). The scale bar reflects t-scores.

Neither for working memory nor for inhibitory control was there any significant association between performance (compound scores) and RS-FC of either seed region with the rest of the brain.

4. Discussion

Coordinate-based ALE meta-analyses were used to synthesize the neural correlates of age-related changes in EFs. In particular, we first ran a meta-analysis across all age groups and all three EF subcomponents followed by a pooled and two directed meta-analyses examining age differences in EF-related brain activity. The initial global analysis corroborated a set of regions well-known for being involved in EFs. Consistent activation differences between young and old adults, however, were restricted to left IFJ and left aC/PrC. Subsequently, we assessed the connectional profiles of these two age-sensitive regions and how their RS-FC profiles are modulated by age and EF abilities. Left IFJ was found to be linked to regions involved in executive functioning, whereas left aC/PrC was connectionally linked to regions involved in attentional processes and the DMN. Furthermore, RS-FC between left IFJ and EF-related regions decreased with increasing age. Similarly, RS-FC between left aC/PrC and regions involved in perceptual processes decreased with increasing age, while RS-FC between left aC/PrC and DMN-related regions increased with age. Finally, only very few associations of seed-based RS-FC with EF abilities were observed: RS-FC between left aC/PrC and bilateral visual cortex was positively associated with the total EF score and cognitive flexibility, whereas RS-FC between left aC/PrC and DMN-related regions was inversely associated with cognitive flexibility.

4.1 Comparison to Previous Meta-Analyses

The current results of between-group contrasts deviate quite noticeably from previous meta-analyses of age differences in EF-related brain activity (Di et al., 2014; Spreng et al., 2010; Turner & Spreng, 2012). The only two regions consistently found across these earlier meta-analyses are left IFJ and pre-SMA. Thus, there is substantial disagreement between all meta-analyses devoted to this topic.

These discrepancies might be explained by several methodological differences: First, all previous meta-analyses included several reports of ROI analyses, which biases ALE whole-brain tests for significance towards convergence in the given ROIs (Müller et al., 2017). The null distribution in ALE reflects a random spatial association between findings across the entire brain assuming that each voxel has the same a priori chance of being activated (Eickhoff et al., 2012). The inclusion of ROI analyses would obviously violate this assumption, leading to inflated significance estimations for regions supported by ROI analyses (Müller et al., 2017). Second, all previous meta-analyses attempted to correct for multiple comparisons by controlling the voxel-level FDR, which is considered invalid for topographic inference on smoothed data (Chumbley & Friston, 2009), features low sensitivity, and leads to inflated positive findings (Eickhoff et al., 2016). FWE correction for ALE meta-analyses on the other hand provides good sensitivity and low susceptibility to false positives. Third, previous meta-analyses were partly based on rather small samples, rendering them prone to yielding clusters of “convergence” driven by very few or even single experiments (Eickhoff et al., 2016). Fourth, earlier analyses included some tasks that, according to our definition, would not constitute clear-cut operationalizations of EFs (e.g., sentence comprehension or word generation tasks). Taken together, the inclusion of ROI studies, heterogeneity in the tasks included, limited sample sizes, and FDR-corrected thresholding may have rendered previous meta-analyses very liberal, leading to more widespread but potentially spurious convergence across published results.

4.2 Left IFJ

The pooled meta-analysis of age differences in EF-related brain activity yielded convergence in left IFJ. Our data indicate that left IFJ is recruited to a different degree by younger versus older adults. The sign of this difference, however, appears to depend on the type of task: For tasks taxing working memory, many studies report an age-related decrease in IFJ activation (e.g., Bäckman et al., 2011; Podell et al., 2012; Prakash et al., 2012). Podell et al. (2012) argued that deficits in working memory updating in older adults are accompanied by a reduced utilization of efficient neurocognitive strategies, relative to younger adults. This is in line with the dedifferentiation hypothesis of cognitive aging, stating that brain regions showing specialized responses to specific cognitive tasks become less specialized with increasing age (Baltes & Lindenberger, 1997; Goh, 2011; Li & Sikström, 2002; Park et al., 2001; 2004). In the context of inhibitory control and attention shifting, however, studies report an age-related increase in left IFJ activity (e.g., Korsch et al., 2014; Townsend et al., 2006; Zysset et al., 2007). According to Townsend et al. (2006), the more extensive activation patterns observed in older adults may be due to (i) the failure of within-channel inhibition of irrelevant visual information, or (ii) compensatory neural recruitment caused by the attempt to increase relevant and decrease irrelevant information processing. This is in line with Korsch et al.’s (2014) conclusion that increased age-related IFJ activation is caused by the use of different strategies when irrelevant information interferes with correct response selection. Looking at the individual study contributions to our cluster, our results support these findings. For experiments on cognitive flexibility or inhibition that contributed to the cluster, convergence in left IFJ was mainly driven by the contrast old > young (rather than young > old). In contradistinction, for experiments on working memory, convergence was mainly driven by the contrast young > old (rather than old > young; see Table A4). These findings, although purely descriptive, point to a shared cognitive mechanism in the context of inhibition and cognitive flexibility, possibly leading to the observed similar aging effects on IFJ activity.

In the literature, there also is a well-established link between left IFJ and task switching, set shifting, or updating task representations (Brass & Cramon, 2004; Derrfuss et al., 2005; Worringer et al., 2019), that is, processes that allow adjusting behavior to new external demands in a top-down fashion (i.e., cognitive flexibility). This notion is also supported by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation studies (Higo et al., 2011; Zanto et al., 2011), pointing to IFJ’s causal participation in updating task representations and regulating neural excitability in visual areas according to the task goal. Supporting the broad involvement of left IFJ across EF domains, Derrfuss et al. (2004) mapped the activity from experiments investigating working memory, task switching, and inhibitory control and found a significant overlap in IFJ for all task types. The almost equal contribution of working memory, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility experiments to the IFJ cluster in the pooled EF meta-analysis also points to its importance for all EF subcomponents. Further indirect evidence is provided by IFJ’s location at the junction of the inferior frontal and inferior precentral sulci, and thus at the intersection of three functional neuroanatomical domains: premotor, language, and working memory. Although our study cannot clarify the precise functional role of left IFJ, this region may integrate information from these three domains (Brass et al., 2005). In particular, it is thought to (re)activate and implement relevant stimulus–response mappings, connecting stimulus information with motor output according to behavioral goals (Hartstra et al., 2012; Worringer et al., 2019).

Our RS-FC results further stress left IFJ’s important role in EFs, as its RS-FC map is highly overlapping with Camilleri et al.’s (2018) eMDN, the proposed neural correlate of EFs and with the frontoparietal control network (FPCN; Cole & Schneider, 2007), that is, bilateral ACC/pre-SMA, DLPFC, IFJ, aIns, dPMC, PPC. The negative association between RS-FC of left IFJ and age (see Figure 5A) indicates that age-related connectivity changes are not regionally specific (e.g. prefrontal) but rather wide-spread, including the dorsal attention network (DAN), the FPCN as well as the eMDN. An age-related RS-FC decline in these networks has been reported previously (Campbell et al., 2012; He et al., 2014). The frequently reported age-related decline in EF performance might thus be associated with decreased FC between regions and networks important for executive functioning. Through its functional role, that is, stimulus–response mapping and its importance for all EF subcomponents, left IFJ seems to be operating as a key node for executive functioning and thus showing domain-general recruitment as well as intrinsic correlations to multiple task positive networks.

Summing up, our meta-analytic and connectional findings suggest a pivotal role of left IFJ in EFs. While its involvement in EFs may mostly be domain-general, its recruitment appears to change with age depending on the type of task. As older adults seem to rely more on left IFJ in the context of cognitive flexibility and inhibition, younger adults recruit it more strongly in the context of working memory. Decreased RS-FC with age of left IFJ and regions associated with different task positive networks points to (i) generalized age-related changes across the brain rather than degradation in a particular region, as well as (ii) a possible underlying neural correlate for EF performance decline with age.

4.3 Left anterior Cuneus/Precuneus

Convergence in left aC/PrC was found in the meta-analyses EF pooled and EF old > young. To account for the difficulties in accurately comparing anatomical locations across individuals and studies due to individual differences as well as differences in spatial processing and brain templates (Brett et al., 2002) we chose to label the region of convergence aC/PrC instead of deciding on just one region and thus neglecting important functional implications. Taking the contribution of our region into account, convergence in the pooled meta-analysis was mainly driven by the contrast old > young. Consequently, consistent increased activation in aC/PrC was specific to older compared to younger adults. Furthermore, it has been associated with initiating shifts of attentional focus (Bzdok et al., 2015; Langner & Eickhoff, 2013; Worringer et al., 2019). This is in accordance with our finding of activity convergence in left aC/PrC being driven by the subcomponents inhibition and cognitive flexibility, where shifting the attentional focus and thus inhibiting irrelevant input plays a key role (see Table A4). Previous studies (DiGirolamo et al., 2001; Kuptsova et al., 2016; Townsend et al., 2006) testing age-related differences in attention shifting suggest that younger and older adults relied on the same regions during shift conditions, that is, frontoparietal regions including PrC. Older adults, however, also recruited these regions during the control condition, (i.e. attentional focusing). The authors suggested that older adults relied more on executive networks, even in the non-shift task condition, to compensate for reduced efficiency of sensory and cognitive processing. Another explanation might be that older adults had difficulties inhibiting the alternate task even during the non-shift condition. By inspecting the study contributions to the left aC/PrC cluster in the pooled EF meta-analysis, one can see that 92% of the studies leading to a convergence in left aC/PrC result from the contrast old > young. 83% of these studies did not report any inclusive masking with a task-positive effect, and 68% tested against an active control condition, rather than rest. While we did not directly investigate deactivations – due to the lack of studies available that matched our inclusion criteria – one could argue, based on these numbers, that convergence in left aC/PrC might be mainly driven by consistently greater aC/PrC deactivation in older adults during the control (vs. task) condition and/or consistently greater deactivation in younger adults during the experimental (vs. control) task, rather than a higher task-induced aC/PrC activation in older adults. A greater age-related deactivation during control (vs. task) and deactivation difficulties (compared to younger adults) in task (vs. control) could lead to inefficiencies in attentional switching in older adults. Together with PCC, PrC is assumed to be one of the central and specialized hubs of the DMN, being intrinsically connected to the DMN as well as to attentional networks, in line with our RS-FC findings (see Figure 4B). Its role might be controlling the dynamic interaction between these networks for an efficient distribution of attention (Leech et al., 2011). Furthermore, PrC appears to be in a special position within the DMN as it is coupled with the DMN at rest, and with task positive networks during task performance (Leech et al., 2011; Utevsky et al., 2014). Its wide-spread FC pattern, involving higher association regions, corroborates an important role in integrating internally and externally driven stimulus processing (Cavanna & Trimble, 2006).

While PrC’s RS-FC with sensorimotor regions decreased in older adults, its RS-FC with regions associated with the DMN and DAN increased with age. Previous studies found that older adults failed to deactivate the DMN during a range of cognitive tasks (e.g., Grady et al., 2006; Lustig et al., 2003; Park et al., 2010; Persson et al., 2007). Spreng and Schacter (2011) assumed that this is due to a reduction of large-scale network flexibility in the context of changing task demands. These differences might also be due to differences during fixation, as older adults have a reduced susceptibility to mind wandering (Giambra, 1989; Jackson & Balota, 2012). Furthermore, it might be more difficult for older adults to fixate the cross, possibly explaining an age-related RS-FC increase of left PrC with the DAN. Additionally, it has been proposed that functional networks become less specific with age (Geerligs, Maurits, et al., 2014; Geerligs, Renken, et al., 2014). Thus, there might be a dedifferentiation in activation patterns – in accordance with the aforementioned dedifferentiation hypothesis of neural aging – and a compensatory recruitment of further brain regions. The latter has also been proposed by the cognitive aging theories CRUNCH (Reuter-Lorenz & Cappell, 2008) and STAC (Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2008), which state that in older adults, to maintain cognitive and behavioral performance, connections that have become fragile or deficient are weakened, existing connections are strengthened, and new connections are developed.

RS-FC between left aC/PrC and bilateral visual cortices showed a positive association with the total EF and cognitive flexibility score, whereas RS-FC between left aC/PrC and both bilateral IPL and right MTG revealed negative associations with the latter score. While larger RS-FC of PrC and visual areas seems to support cognitive flexibility, RS-FC of PrC and regions associated with the DMN and DAN is linked to worse performance in cognitive flexibility tasks. Taking our previous findings into account, a similar RS-FC map was positively associated with age, which could be because of a dedifferentiation in activation patterns as proposed in the dedifferentiation theory of neural aging (Baltes & Lindenberger, 1997; Goh, 2011; Li & Sikström, 2002; Park et al., 2001; 2004) or compensatory activations as postulated in CRUNCH (Reuter-Lorenz & Cappell, 2008), and STAC (Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2008). However, given the nature of the available data and the methods applied, we cannot draw firmer and more theory-specific conclusions.

Summing up, our findings suggest that left aC/PrC is specifically recruited by older (vs. younger) adults, possibly to compensate for difficulties in shifting their attentional focus. Conversely, our results indicate an age-related increase in relative aC/PrC deactivation during the control task and/or an age-related decrease in relative aC/PrC deactivation during the experimental task, rising an alternative hypothesis for the higher task-induced aC/PrC activation in older adults. Left aC/PrC’s intrinsic coupling with the DMN and DAN supports its proposed role as a specialized hub, involved in internally as well as externally oriented information processing. The age-related decrease in RS-FC between aC/PrC and sensorimotor networks suggests some decoupling with age that is detrimental to action-related, externally oriented processing; the concurrent increase in RS-FC between DMN and DAN, in turn, suggests age-related difficulties in decoupling aC/PrC from the DMN during task states and from DAN-related regions during rest. Taking left aC/PrC’s often reported covariation with left IFJ during rest into account which was not found in the current study, our findings might reflect (and possibly contribute to) a dediffer-entiation in functional network patterns in older adults, potentially undermining the special role this region plays in shifting between internally and externally directed attention.

4.4 Limitations and Outlook

Although ALE is a well validated and widely used coordinate-based meta-analytic approach, it stands to reason that image-based meta-analyses may have provided greater sensitivity (Salimi-Khorshidi et al., 2009). However, as imaging data have previously been rarely shared, it would have been difficult to impossible to find a sufficient number of experiments with wholebrain images of effect estimates and standard errors.

Further, we were not able to conduct domain-specific meta-analyses for working memory and cognitive flexibility, since too few experiments were eligible for inclusion. More individual fMRI studies would be necessary to separately investigate the three EF subcomponents. The inclusion of more experiments would furthermore allow for testing a domain-specific account of EFs by directly contrasting the subcomponents with each other and testing additional or different EF subdivisions including even more fine-grained EF subprocesses. As previously discussed in the context of left IFJ and left aC/PrC, it seems that there is a process-specific sensitivity to aging. This process specificity may strongly contribute to the observed small to nonexistent across-experiment convergence of age differences in regional EF-related brain activity. In the context of inhibitory control, Korsch et al. (2014) found different age effects for different conflict tasks. In particular, there was overlap in brain activation during a flanker task between the two age groups and additional age-related activity in parietal and frontal regions. In contrast, during a stimulus– response compatibility task, no overlap in brain activation between the two groups was observed. Hence, age differences in EF-related brain activity appear to be task-specific to a substantial degree. This, in turn, would then lead to a heterogeneous distribution of age-related effects across studies, even within EF subdomains, which severely limits the chances for meta-analytic convergence and would argue for changing the focus of future research away from attempting to localize common, (sub)domain-general activation differences between age groups toward identifying process-specific mechanisms of age-related activity modulations. As discussed earlier, another explanation could be age-related regional changes in grey-matter volume (i.e. atrophy). Thus, we recommend that future studies on this topic investigate (i) domains and even subdomains, (ii) compare age-related differences in EFs across different modalities, and (iii) incorporate computational cognitive modeling (Kriegeskorte & Douglas, 2018).

Additionally, due to the small number of studies that reported deactivations, we were only able to investigate activation effects. As our results indicate age-related difficulties in deactivating left aC/PrC in the context of EF-tasks, we call for future studies investigating both directions of task-induced brain activity changes.

Somewhat surprisingly, no significant correlations between the two seeds’ whole-brain RS-FC patterns and the EF subcomponents working memory and inhibition were found. As there is ample evidence for RS-FC correlations with EF abilities in the literature (e.g., Hampson et al., 2006; Markett et al., 2013), a possible explanation could be that the tests used to assess EF domain-related abilities (via compound scores) were not sufficiently representative of the rather broad EF subdomains to yield a valid assessment of individual abilities or, the breadth of the subdomains prevented the scores from sufficiently reflecting particular subprocesses and age modulations thereof. The latter notion is supported by the fact that age correlated only moderately with the combined EF score (r = -.44, p < .001), the cognitive flexibility score (r = -.41, p < .001), and the inhibitory control score (r = -.31, p < .001). It did only weakly correlate with the working memory score (r = -.15, p < .05). For future studies on these questions it may be beneficial to incorporate various psychometric assessments of a particular cognitive function, which would allow isolating function- and test-specific variance in order to elucidate brain–behavior relationships (and their changes across the lifespan) at a more commensurate level of “granularity”.

Comparing our results to those of earlier neuroimaging meta-analyses of age-related differences in EFs underlines the importance of (i) transparently reporting the analysis choices made, (ii) providing a detailed description of inclusion and exclusion criteria and their motivation, and (iii) precisely reporting the papers and contrasts included as well as whether further information was received from the authors of the original study (for guidelines see Müller et al., 2018). Otherwise, even meta-analyses lack comparability and reproducibility.

4.5 Conclusion

The current study suggests that left IFJ and left aC/PrC play an important role in age-related differences in EFs as they were found the only two brain regions that showed consistent age differences in their recruitment during EF tasks across three major domains (working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility). Although RS-FC analyses point towards a domain-general role of left IFJ in EFs, the pattern of contributions to the meta-analytic results also suggests process-specific modulations by age. In particular, older adults appear to rely more on left IFJ in the context of cognitive flexibility and inhibition, whereas younger adults recruited it more strongly in the context of working memory. Our findings further indicate that left aC/PrC is specifically recruited by older adults during EF tasks, potentially reflecting inefficiencies in switching the attentional focus. Overall, our results question earlier meta-analytic findings that suggested different and more comprehensive sets of brain regions as showing consistent age modulations of their EF-related activity. Rather, our findings attest to the substantial heterogeneity of such age-related differences and call for research that pays more attention to replicability and focuses on more narrowly and precisely defined EF subprocesses by combining multiple behavioral assessments, computational cognitive modelling, and multi-modal imaging.

5. Acknowledgements

We thank all contacted authors who contributed results of relevant contrasts not explicitly reported in the original publications, and we apologize to all authors whose eligible papers we might have missed.

6. Financial Disclosures

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

7. Funding

This study was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, EI 816/11-1), the National Institute of Mental Health (R01-MH074457), the Helmholtz Portfolio Theme “Supercomputing and Modeling for the Human Brain”, the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme under Grant Agreement No. 720270 (HBP SGA1) and 785907 (HBP SGA2).

9. Appendix

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table A1

Overview of All Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis of Within-Group Contrasts Comprising Information About the Mean Age, Number of Activation Foci for Each Age Group, Masking with Task-Positive Effect, and Correction

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table A2

Overview of All Studies Included in the Meta-Analyses of Between-Group Contrasts Comprising Information About the Mean Age, Number of Activation Foci for Each Age Group, Masking with Task-Positive Effect, and Correction

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table A3

Checklist for Neuroimaging Meta-Analyses by Müller et al. (2018)

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table A4

Single Experiments Contributing to the Clusters of Convergence

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table A5

Brain Regions Showing Significant Convergence of Activity in Inhibition

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table A6

Single Experiments Contributing to the Inhibition Cluster of Convergence

8. References

  1. 1.↵
    Alvarez, J. A., & Emory, E. (2006). Executive Function and the Frontal Lobes: A Meta-Analytic Review. Neuropsychology Review, 16(1), 17–42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  2. 2.
    Anderson, N. D., Iidaka, T., Cabeza, R., Kapur, S., McIntosh, A. R., & Craik, F. I. M. (2000). The Effects of Divided Attention on Encoding- and Retrieval-Related Brain Activity: A PET Study of Younger and Older Adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 775–792. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900562598
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  3. 3.↵
    Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Jones, R. D., & Tranel, D. (1991). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Performance as a Measure of Frontal Lobe Damage. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13(6), 909–922. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688639108405107
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  4. 4.
    Anguera, J. A., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Willingham, D. T., & Seidler, R. D. (2011). Failure to engage spatial working memory contributes to age-related declines in visuomotor learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(1), 11–25.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  5. 5.↵
    Ansado, J., Monchi, O., Ennabil, N., Faure, S., & Joanette, Y. (2012). Load-dependent posterior–anterior shift in aging in complex visual selective attention situations. Brain Research, 1454, 14–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.061
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  6. 6.↵
    Anticevic, A., Cole, M. W., Murray, J. D., Corlett, P. R., Wang, X.-J., & Krystal, J. H. (2012). The role of default network deactivation in cognition and disease. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(12), 584–592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  7. 7.↵
    Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-Based Morphometry—The Methods. NeuroImage, 11(6), 805–821. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0582
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  8. 8.↵
    Bäckman, L., Karlsson, S., Fischer, H., Karlsson, P., Brehmer, Y., Rieckmann, A., MacDonald, S. W. S., Farde, L., & Nyberg, L. (2011). Dopamine D1 receptors and age differences in brain activation during working memory. Neurobiology of Aging, 32(10), 1849–1856. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.10.018
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  9. 9.↵
    1. G. H. Bower
    Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working Memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1
    OpenUrl
  10. 10.↵
    Baltes, P. B., & Lindenberger, U. (1997). Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: A new window to the study of cognitive aging? Psychology and Aging, 12(1), 12–21. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.12.1.12
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  11. 11.
    Bennett, I. J., Rivera, H. G., & Rypma, B. (2013). Isolating age-group differences in working memory load-related neural activity: Assessing the contribution of working memory capacity using a partial-trial fMRI method. NeuroImage, 72, 20–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.030
    OpenUrl
  12. 12.↵
    Bloemendaal, M., Zandbelt, B., Wegman, J., van de Rest, O., Cools, R., & Aarts, E. (2016). Contrasting neural effects of aging on proactive and reactive response inhibition. Neurobiology of Aging, 46, 96–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.06.007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. 13.↵
    Brass, & Cramon, D. Y. (2004). Decomposing Components of Task Preparation with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(4), 609–620. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892904323057335
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  14. 14.↵
    Brass, M., Derrfuss, J., Forstmann, B., & von Cramon, Y. D. (2005). Relation of the inferior frontal junction (IFJ) to existing cytoarchitectonic maps. 9(7), 314–316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.001
    OpenUrl
  15. 15.↵
    Brett, M., Johnsrude, I. S., & Owen, A. M. (2002). The problem of functional localization in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 243–249. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn756
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  16. 16.↵
    Bzdok, D., Heeger, A., Langner, R., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., Palomero-Gallagher, N., Vogt, B. A., Zilles, K., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2015). Subspecialization in the human posterior medial cortex. NeuroImage, 106, 55–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. 17.↵
    Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 85–100. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.85
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  18. 18.↵
    Camilleri, J. A., Müller, V. I., Fox, P., Laird, A. R., Hoffstaedter, F., Kalenscher, T., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2018). Definition and characterization of an extended multiple-demand network. NeuroImage, 165, 138–147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.020
    OpenUrl
  19. 19.↵
    Campbell, K. L., Grady, C. L., Ng, C., & Hasher, L. (2012). Age differences in the frontoparietal cognitive control network: Implications for distractibility. Neuropsychologia, 50(9), 2212–2223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.05.025
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  20. 20.↵
    Carp, J., Gmeindl, L., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2010). Age Differences in the Neural Representation of Working Memory Revealed by Multi-Voxel Pattern Analysis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 217. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00217
    OpenUrl
  21. 21.↵
    Cavanna, A. E., & Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus: A review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain, 129(3), 564–583. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  22. 22.
    Chee, M. W. L., Goh, J. O. S., Venkatraman, V., Tan, J. C., Gutchess, A., Sutton, B., Hebrank, A., Leshikar, E., & Park, D. (2006). Age-related changes in object processing and contextual binding revealed using fMR adaptation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(4), 495–507. PubMed. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.495
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  23. 23.↵
    Chen, G., Cox, R. W., Glen, D. R., Rajendra, J. K., Reynolds, R. C., & Taylor, P. A. (2018). A tail of two sides: Artificially doubled false positive rates in neuroimaging due to the sidedness choice with t-tests. Human Brain Mapping, 40, 1037–1043. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24399
    OpenUrl
  24. 24.
    Chmielewski, W. X., Yildiz, A., & Beste, C. (2014). The neural architecture of age-related dual-task interferences. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6, 193. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00193
    OpenUrl
  25. 25.↵
    Chumbley, J. R., & Friston, K. J. (2009). False discovery rate revisited: FDR and topological inference using Gaussian random fields. NeuroImage, 44(1), 62–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.021
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  26. 26.↵
    Cieslik, E. C., Mueller, V. I., Eickhoff, C. R., Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2015). Three key regions for supervisory attentional control: Evidence from neuroimaging meta-analyses. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 48, 22–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.11.003
    OpenUrl
  27. 27.
    Cliff, M., Joyce, D. W., Lamar, M., Dannhauser, T., Tracy, D. K., & Shergill, S. S. (2013). Aging effects on functional auditory and visual processing using fMRI with variable sensory loading. Cortex, 49(5), 1304–1313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.003
    OpenUrl
  28. 28.
    Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., & Scalf, P. (2005). The Implications of Cortical Recruitment and Brain Morphology for Individual Differences in Inhibitory Function in Aging Humans. Psychology and Aging, 20(3), 363–375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.363
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  29. 29.↵
    Cole, M. W., & Schneider, W. (2007). The cognitive control network: Integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions. NeuroImage, 37(1), 343–360. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.071
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  30. 30.↵
    Collette, F., Hogge, M., Salmon, E., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Exploration of the neural substrates of executive functioning by functional neuroimaging. Neuroscience, 139(1), 209–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.05.035
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  31. 31.↵
    Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A., Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., & Cabeza, R. (2007). Qué PASA? The Posterior–Anterior Shift in Aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1201–1209. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm155
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  32. 32.↵
    Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Holdnack, J. (2004). Reliability and validity of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: An update. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(2), 301–303. Cambridge Core. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704102191
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  33. 33.↵
    Derrfuss, J., Brass, M., Neumann, J., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2005). Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 22–34. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20127
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  34. 34.↵
    Derrfuss, J., Brass, M., & Yves von Cramon, D. (2004). Cognitive control in the posterior frontolateral cortex: Evidence from common activations in task coordination, interference control, and working memory. NeuroImage, 23(2), 604–612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  35. 35.↵
    Di, X., Rypma, B., & Biswal, B. B. (2014). Correspondence of executive function related functional and anatomical alterations in aging brain. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 48, 41–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.09.001
    OpenUrl
  36. 36.↵
    Diamond, A. (2013). Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  37. 37.↵
    DiGirolamo, G. J., Kramer, A. F., Barad, V., Cepeda, N. J., Weissman, D. H., Milham, M. P., Wszalek, T. M., Cohen, N. J., Banich, M. T., Webb, A., Belopolsky, A. V., & McAuley, E. (2001). General and task-specific frontal lobe recruitment in older adults during executive processes: A fMRI investigation of task-switching. NeuroReport, 12(9). https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200107030-00054
    OpenUrl
  38. 38.
    Dørum, E. S., Alnæs, D., Kaufmann, T., Richard, G., Lund, M. J., Tønnesen, S., Sneve, M. H., Mathiesen, N. C., Rustan, Ø. G., Gjertsen, Ø., Vatn, S., Fure, B., Andreassen, O. A., Nordvik, J. E., & Westlye, L. T. (2016). Age-related differences in brain network activation and coactivation during multiple object tracking. Brain and Behavior, 6(11), e00533. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.533
    OpenUrl
  39. 39.↵
    Drag, L. L., & Bieliauskas, L. A. (2010). Contemporary Review 2009: Cognitive Aging. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 23(2), 75–93. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891988709358590
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  40. 40.↵
    Duncan, J. (1986). Disorganisation of behaviour after frontal lobe damage. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3(3), 271–290. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643298608253360
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  41. 41.↵
    Duncan, J. (2010). The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: Mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(4), 172–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  42. 42.↵
    Duncan, J., & Owen, A. M. (2000). Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. 23(10), 475–483. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01633-7
    OpenUrl
  43. 43.↵
    Eich, T. S., Parker, D., Liu, D., Oh, H., Razlighi, Q., Gazes, Y., Habeck, C., & Stern, Y. (2016). Functional brain and age-related changes associated with congruency in task switching. Neuropsychologia, 91, 211–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.009
    OpenUrl
  44. 44.↵
    Eickhoff, S. B., Bzdok, D., Laird, A. R., Kurth, F., & Fox, P. T. (2012). Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. NeuroImage, 59(3), 2349–2361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.017
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  45. 45.↵
    Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Grefkes, C., Wang, L. E., Zilles, K., & Fox, P. T. (2009). Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: A random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Human Brain Mapping, 30(9), 2907–2926. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20718
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  46. 46.↵
    Eickhoff, S. B., Nichols, T. E., Laird, A. R., Hoffstaedter, F., Amunts, K., Fox, P. T., Bzdok, D., & Eickhoff, C. R. (2016). Behavior, sensitivity, and power of activation likelihood estimation characterized by massive empirical simulation. NeuroImage, 137, 70–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.072
    OpenUrl
  47. 47.↵
    Eickhoff, S. B., Paus, T., Caspers, S., Grosbras, M.-H., Evans, A. C., Zilles, K., & Amunts, K. (2007). Assignment of functional activations to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic areas revisited. NeuroImage, 36(3), 511–521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.060
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  48. 48.↵
    Eickhoff, S. B., Stephan, K. E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G. R., Amunts, K., & Zilles, K. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. NeuroImage, 25(4), 1325–1335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  49. 49.↵
    Emery, L., Heaven, T. J., Paxton, J. L., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Age-related changes in neural activity during performance matched working memory manipulation. NeuroImage, 42(4), 1577–1586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.021
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  50. 50.↵
    Engle, R. W., & Kane, M. J. (2004). Executive Attention, Working Memory Capacity, and a Two-Factor Theory of Cognitive Control. In The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, Vol. 44. (pp. 145–199). Elsevier Science.
    OpenUrlWeb of Science
  51. 51.↵
    Eslinger, P. J., & Damasio, A. R. (1985). Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation. Neurology, 35(12), 1731. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.35.12.1731
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. 52.
    Esposito, G., Kirkby, B. S., Van Horn, J. D., Ellmore, T. M., & Berman, K. F. (1999). Context-dependent, neural system-specific neurophysiological concomitants of ageing: Mapping PET correlates during cognitive activation. Brain, 122(5), 963–979. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/122.5.963
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  53. 53.
    Fakhri, M., Sikaroodi, H., Maleki, F., Ali Oghabian, M., & Ghanaati, H. (2012). Age-Related Frontal Hyperactivation Observed across Different Working Memory Tasks: An fMRI Study. Behavioural Neurology, 25, 824049. https://doi.org/10.3233/BEN-2012-120280
    OpenUrl
  54. 54.↵
    Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–347. PubMed. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361886
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  55. 55.↵
    Fedorenko, E., Duncan, J., & Kanwisher, N. (2013). Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(41), 16616. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1315235110
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. 56.
    Fernandes, M. A., Pacurar, A., Moscovitch, M., & Grady, C. (2006). Neural correlates of auditory recognition under full and divided attention in younger and older adults. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 2452–2464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.020
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  57. 57.↵
    Geerligs, L., Maurits, N. M., Renken, R. J., & Lorist, M. M. (2014). Reduced specificity of functional connectivity in the aging brain during task performance. Human Brain Mapping, 35(1), 319–330. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22175
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  58. 58.↵
    Geerligs, L., Renken, R. J., Saliasi, E., Maurits, N. M., & Lorist, M. M. (2014). A Brain-Wide Study of Age-Related Changes in Functional Connectivity. Cerebral Cortex, 25(7), 1987–1999. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu012
    OpenUrlPubMed
  59. 59.↵
    Giambra, L. M. (1989). Task-unrelated thought frequency as a function of age: A laboratory study. Psychology and Aging, 4(2), 136–143. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.4.2.136
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  60. 60.↵
    Goh, J. O. S. (2011). Functional Dedifferentiation and Altered Connectivity in Older Adults: Neural Accounts of Cognitive Aging. Aging and Disease, 2(1), 30–48. PubMed.
    OpenUrl
  61. 61.
    Grady, C. L., McIntosh, A., Horwitz, B., & Rapoport, S. I. (2010). Age-related changes in the neural correlates of degraded and nondegraded face processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17(1–3), 165–186. https://doi.org/10.1080/026432900380553
    OpenUrl
  62. 62.
    Grady, C. L., McIntosh, A. R., Bookstein, F., Horwitz, B., Rapoport, S. I., & Haxby, J. V. (1998). Age-Related Changes in Regional Cerebral Blood Flow during Working Memory for Faces. NeuroImage, 8(4), 409–425. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0376
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  63. 63.↵
    Grady, C. L., Springer, M. V., Hongwanishkul, D., McIntosh, A. R., & Winocur, G. (2006). Age-related changes in brain activity across the adult lifespan. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(2), 227–241. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.2.227
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  64. 64.
    Grady, C. L., Yu, H., & Alain, C. (2007). Age-Related Differences in Brain Activity Underlying Working Memory for Spatial and Nonspatial Auditory Information. Cerebral Cortex, 18(1), 189–199. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm045
    OpenUrl
  65. 65.↵
    Greenwood, P. M. (2007). Functional plasticity in cognitive aging: Review and hypothesis. Neuropsychology, 21(6), 657–673. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.21.6.657
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  66. 66.↵
    Griffanti, L., Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Beckmann, C. F., Auerbach, E. J., Douaud, G., Sexton, C. E., Zsoldos, E., Ebmeier, K. P., Filippini, N., Mackay, C. E., Moeller, S., Xu, J., Yacoub, E., Baselli, G., Ugurbil, K., Miller, K. L., & Smith, S. M. (2014). ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. NeuroImage, 95, 232–247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  67. 67.↵
    Gur, R. C., Richard, J., Hughett, P., Calkins, M. E., Macy, L., Bilker, W. B., Brensinger, C., & Gur, R. E. (2010). A cognitive neuroscience-based computerized battery for efficient measurement of individual differences: Standardization and initial construct validation. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 187(2), 254–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.11.017
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  68. 68.↵
    Hartstra, E., Waszak, F., & Brass, M. (2012). The implementation of verbal instructions: Dissociating motor preparation from the formation of stimulus–response associations. NeuroImage, 63(3), 1143–1153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. 69.
    Haut, M. W., Kuwabara, H., Moran, M. T., Leach, S., Arias, R., & Knight, D. (2005). The Effect of Education on Age-Related Functional Activation During Working Memory. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 12(2), 216–229. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825580590969325
    OpenUrl
  70. 70.↵
    He, X., Qin, W., Liu, Y., Zhang, X., Duan, Y., Song, J., Li, K., Jiang, T., & Yu, C. (2014). Abnormal salience network in normal aging and in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Human Brain Mapping, 35(7), 3446–3464. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22414
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  71. 71.↵
    Head, D., Buckner, R. L., Shimony, J. S., Williams, L. E., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., McAvoy, M., Morris, J. C., & Snyder, A. Z. (2004). Differential Vulnerability of Anterior White Matter in Nondemented Aging with Minimal Acceleration in Dementia of the Alzheimer Type: Evidence from Diffusion Tensor Imaging. Cerebral Cortex, 14(4), 410–423. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  72. 72.↵
    Higo, T., Mars, R. B., Boorman, E. D., Buch, E. R., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2011). Distributed and causal influence of frontal operculum in task control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(10), 4230. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1013361108
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  73. 73.
    Huang, C.-M., Polk, T. A., Goh, J. O., & Park, D. C. (2012). Both left and right posterior parietal activations contribute to compensatory processes in normal aging. Neuropsychologia, 50(1), 55–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.10.022
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. 74.
    Hubert, V., Beaunieux, H., Chételat, G., Platel, H., Landeau, B., Viader, F., Desgranges, B., & Eustache, F. (2009). Age-related changes in the cerebral substrates of cognitive procedural learning. Human Brain Mapping, 30(4), 1374–1386. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20605
    OpenUrlPubMed
  75. 75.↵
    Jackson, J. D., & Balota, D. A. (2012). Mind-wandering in younger and older adults: Converging evidence from the sustained attention to response task and reading for comprehension. Psychology and Aging, 27(1), 106–119. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023933
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  76. 76.↵
    Jurado, M. B., & Rosselli, M. (2007). The Elusive Nature of Executive Functions: A Review of our Current Understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17(3), 213–233. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  77. 77.↵
    Korsch, M., Frühholz, S., & Herrmann, M. (2014). Ageing differentially affects neural processing of different conflict types—An fMRI study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6, 57. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00057
    OpenUrl
  78. 78.
    Kunimi, M., Kiyama, S., & Nakai, T. (2016). Investigation of age-related changes in brain activity during the divalent task-switching paradigm using functional MRI. Neuroscience Research, 103, 18–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2015.06.011
    OpenUrl
  79. 79.↵
    Kuptsova, S. V., Ivanova, M. V., Petrushevskiy, A. G., Fedina, O. N., & Zhavoronkova, L. A. (2016). Sex- and age-related characteristics of brain functioning during task switching (fMRI study). 42(4), 361–370. https://doi.org/10.1134/S0362119716040101
    OpenUrl
  80. 80.
    Kurth, S., Majerus, S., Bastin, C., Collette, F., Jaspar, M., Bahri, M. A., & Salmon, E. (2016). Effects of aging on task- and stimulus-related cerebral attention networks. Neurobiology of Aging, 44, 85–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.04.015
    OpenUrl
  81. 81.↵
    Lamar, M., Yousem, D. M., & Resnick, S. M. (2004). Age differences in orbitofrontal activation: An fMRI investigation of delayed match and nonmatch to sample. NeuroImage, 21(4), 1368–1376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.018
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  82. 82.↵
    Lancaster, J. L., Tordesillas-Gutiérrez, D., Martinez, M., Salinas, F., Evans, A., Zilles, K., Mazziotta, J. C., & Fox, P. T. (2007). Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Human Brain Mapping, 28(11), 1194–1205. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20345
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  83. 83.
    Langenecker, S. A., Nielson, K. A., & Rao, S. M. (2004). FMRI of healthy older adults during Stroop interference. NeuroImage, 21(1), 192–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.027
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  84. 84.↵
    Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2013). Sustaining attention to simple tasks: A meta-analytic review of the neural mechanisms of vigilant attention. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 870–900. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030694
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. 85.
    Lecouvey, G., Quinette, P., Kalpouzos, G., Guillery-Girard, B., Bejanin, A., Gonneaud, J., Abbas, A., Viader, F., Eustache, F., & Desgranges, B. (2015). Binding in working memory and frontal lobe in normal aging: Is there any similarity with autism? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 90. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00090
    OpenUrl
  86. 86.
    Lee, T. M. C., Zhang, J. X., Chan, C. C. H., Yuen, K. S. L., Chu, L. W., Cheung, R. T. F., Chan, Y. S., Fox, P. T., & Gao, J.-H. (2006). Age-related differences in response regulation as revealed by functional MRI. Brain Research, 1076(1), 171–176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.124
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  87. 87.↵
    Leech, R., Kamourieh, S., Beckmann, C. F., & Sharp, D. J. (2011). Fractionating the Default Mode Network: Distinct Contributions of the Ventral and Dorsal Posterior Cingulate Cortex to Cognitive Control. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(9), 3217. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5626-10.2011
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  88. 88.↵
    Lehto, J. (1996). Are Executive Function Tests Dependent on Working Memory Capacity? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 49(1), 29–50. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755616
    OpenUrl
  89. 89.↵
    Li, S.-C., Lindenberger, U., & Sikström, S. (2001). Aging cognition: From neuromodulation to representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(11), 479–486. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01769-1
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  90. 90.↵
    Li, S.-C., & Sikström, S. (2002). Integrative neurocomputational perspectives on cognitive aging, neuromodulation, and representation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 795–808. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00066-0
    OpenUrl
  91. 91.↵
    Lustig, C., Snyder, A. Z., Bhakta, M., O’Brien, K. C., McAvoy, M., Raichle, M. E., Morris, J. C., & Buckner, R. L. (2003). Functional deactivations: Change with age and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(24), 14504. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2235925100
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  92. 92.↵
    Madden, D. J., Costello, M. C., Dennis, N. A., Davis, S. W., Shepler, A. M., Spaniol, J., Bucur, B., & Cabeza, R. (2010). Adult age differences in functional connectivity during executive control. NeuroImage, 52(2), 643–657. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.249
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  93. 93.↵
    Madden, D. J., Turkington, T. G., Provenzale, J. M., Denny, L. L., Hawk, T. C., Gottlob, L. R., & Coleman, R. E. (1999). Adult age differences in the functional neuroanatomy of verbal recognition memory. Human Brain Mapping, 7(2), 115–135. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)7:2<115::AID-HBM5>3.0.CO;2-N
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  94. 94.↵
    Madden, D. J., Turkington, T. G., Provenzale, J. M., Denny, L. L., Langley, L. K., Hawk, T. C., & Coleman, R. E. (2002). Aging and attentional guidance during visual search: Functional neuroanatomy by positron emission tomography. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 24–43. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.24
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  95. 95.
    Madden, D. J., Turkington, T. G., Provenzale, J. M., Hawk, T. C., Hoffman, J. M., & Coleman, R. E. (1997). Selective and divided visual attention: Age-related changes in regional cerebral blood flow measured by H215O PET. Human Brain Mapping, 5(6), 389–409. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1997)5:6<389::AID-HBM1>3.0.CO;2-#
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. 96.
    Mak, L. E., Minuzzi, L., MacQueen, G., Hall, G., Kennedy, S. H., & Milev, R. (2016). The Default Mode Network in Healthy Individuals: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Brain Connectivity, 7(1), 25–33. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2016.0438
    OpenUrl
  97. 97.
    Meinzer, M., Flaisch, T., Wilser, L., Eulitz, C., Rockstroh, B., Conway, T., Gonzalez-Rothi, L., & Crosson, B. (2009). Neural signatures of semantic and phonemic fluency in young and old adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(10), 2007–2018. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21219
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  98. 98.↵
    Milham, M. P., Erickson, K. I., Banich, M. T., Kramer, A. F., Webb, A., Wszalek, T., & Cohen, N. J. (2002). Attentional Control in the Aging Brain: Insights from an fMRI Study of the Stroop Task. Brain and Cognition, 49(3), 277–296. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.2001.1501
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  99. 99.↵
    Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  100. 100.↵
    Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  101. 101.↵
    Mountain, M. A., & Snow, W. G. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test as a measure of frontal pathology: A review. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7(1), 108–118. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854049308401893
    OpenUrl
  102. 102.↵
    Müller, V. I., Cieslik, E. C., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., Tench, C. R., Yarkoni, T., Nichols, T. E., Turkeltaub, P. E., Wager, T. D., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2018). Ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 84, 151–161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.012
    OpenUrl
  103. 103.↵
    Müller, V. I., Cieslik, E. C., Serbanescu, I., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2017). Altered Brain Activity in Unipolar Depression Revisited: Meta-analyses of Neuroimaging Studies. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(1), 47–55. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2783
    OpenUrl
  104. 104.↵
    Müller, V. I., Langner, R., Cieslik, E. C., Rottschy, C., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2015). Interindividual differences in cognitive flexibility: Influence of gray matter volume, functional connectivity and trait impulsivity. Brain Structure and Function, 220(4), 2401–2414. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0797-6
    OpenUrl
  105. 105.↵
    Nooner, K., Colcombe, S., Tobe, R., Mennes, M., Benedict, M., Moreno, A., Panek, L., Brown, S., Zavitz, S., Li, Q., Sikka, S., Gutman, D., Bangaru, S., Schlachter, R. T., Kamiel, S., Anwar, A., Hinz, C., Kaplan, M., Rachlin, A., … Milham, M. (2012). The NKI-Rockland Sample: A Model for Accelerating the Pace of Discovery Science in Psychiatry. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, 152. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00152
    OpenUrl
  106. 106.↵
    1. R. J. Davidson,
    2. G. E. Schwartz,
    3. D. Shapiro
    Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in research Vol. IV ( R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, D. Shapiro, pp. 1–18). Plenum Press.
    OpenUrl
  107. 107.
    O’Connell, R. G., Balsters, J. H., Kilcullen, S. M., Campbell, W., Bokde, A. W., Lai, R., Upton, N., & Robertson, I. H. (2012). A simultaneous ERP/fMRI investigation of the P300 aging effect. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(10), 2448–2461. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.021
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  108. 108.
    O’Halloran, L., Cao, Z., Ruddy, K., Jollans, L., Albaugh, M. D., Aleni, A., Potter, A. S., Vahey, N., Banaschewski, T., Hohmann, S., Bokde, A. L. W., Bromberg, U., Büchel, C., Quinlan, E. B., Desrivières, S., Flor, H., Frouin, V., Gowland, P., Heinz, A., … Whelan, R. (2018). Neural circuitry underlying sustained attention in healthy adolescents and in ADHD symptomatology. NeuroImage, 169, 395–406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.030
    OpenUrl
  109. 109.
    Oren, N., Ash, E. L., Tarrasch, R., Hendler, T., Giladi, N., & Shapira-Lichter, I. (2017). Neural patterns underlying the effect of negative distractors on working memory in older adults. Neurobiology of Aging, 53, 93–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.01.020
    OpenUrl
  110. 110.↵
    Owen, A. M., Downes, J. J., Sahakian, B. J., Polkey, C. E., & Robbins, T. W. (1990). Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 28(10), 1021–1034. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(90)90137-D
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  111. 111.↵
    Park, D C, Polk, T. A., Mikels, J. A., Taylor, S. F., & Marshuetz, C. (2001). Cerebral aging: Integration of brain and behavioral models of cognitive function. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 3(3), 151–165. PubMed.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  112. 112.↵
    Park, D., Polk, T., Hebrank, A., & Jenkins, L. (2010). Age differences in default mode activity on easy and difficult spatial judgment tasks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 75. https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.075.2009
    OpenUrl
  113. 113.↵
    Park, Denise C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17(2), 299–320. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.299
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  114. 114.↵
    Park, Denise C., Polk, T. A., Park, R., Minear, M., Savage, A., & Smith, M. R. (2004). Aging reduces neural specialization in ventral visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(35), 13091. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0405148101
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  115. 115.↵
    Park, Denise C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2008). The Adaptive Brain: Aging and Neurocognitive Scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 173–196. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656
    OpenUrl
  116. 116.
    Paxton, J. L., Barch, D. M., Racine, C. A., & Braver, T. S. (2007). Cognitive Control, Goal Maintenance, and Prefrontal Function in Healthy Aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1010–1028. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm135
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  117. 117.↵
    Persson, J., Lustig, C., Nelson, J. K., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2007). Age Differences in Deactivation: A Link to Cognitive Control? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(6), 1021–1032. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.1021
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  118. 118.
    Piefke, M., Onur, Ö. A., & Fink, G. R. (2012). Aging-related changes of neural mechanisms underlying visual-spatial working memory. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(7), 1284–1297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.10.014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  119. 119.↵
    Podell, J. E., Sambataro, F., Murty, V. P., Emery, M. R., Tong, Y., Das, S., Goldberg, T. E., Weinberger, D. R., & Mattay, V. S. (2012). Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in working memory updating. NeuroImage, 62(3), 2151–2160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.066
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  120. 120.↵
    Prakash, R. S., Heo, S., Voss, M. W., Patterson, B., & Kramer, A. F. (2012). Age-related differences in cortical recruitment and suppression: Implications for cognitive performance. Behavioural Brain Research, 230(1), 192–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.01.058
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  121. 121.↵
    Raichle, M. E. (2015). The Brain’s Default Mode Network. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 38(1), 433–447. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  122. 122.
    Raye, C. L., Mitchell, K. J., Reeder, J. A., Greene, E. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2008). Refreshing one of several active representations: Behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging differences between young and older adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(5), 852–862. PubMed. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20508
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  123. 123.↵
    Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Williamson, A., Dahle, C., Gerstorf, D., & Acker, J. D. (2005). Regional Brain Changes in Aging Healthy Adults: General Trends, Individual Differences and Modifiers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(11), 1676–1689. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhi044
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  124. 124.↵
    Resnick, S. M., Pham, D. L., Kraut, M. A., Zonderman, A. B., & Davatzikos, C. (2003). Longitudinal Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies of Older Adults: A Shrinking Brain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(8), 3295. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-08-03295.2003
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  125. 125.↵
    Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive Aging and the Compensation Hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 177–182. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00570.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  126. 126.↵
    Rottschy, C., Langner, R., Dogan, I., Reetz, K., Laird, A. R., Schulz, J. B., Fox, P. T., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2012). Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 60(1), 830–846. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.050
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  127. 127.↵
    Salat, D. H., Buckner, R. L., Snyder, A. Z., Greve, D. N., Desikan, R. S. R., Busa, E., Morris, J. C., Dale, A. M., & Fischl, B. (2004). Thinning of the Cerebral Cortex in Aging. Cerebral Cortex, 14(7), 721–730. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh032
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  128. 128.↵
    Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Douaud, G., Beckmann, C. F., Glasser, M. F., Griffanti, L., & Smith, S. M. (2014). Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: Combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. NeuroImage, 90, 449–468. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  129. 129.↵
    Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Smith, S. M., Keltner, J. R., Wager, T. D., & Nichols, T. E. (2009). Meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: A comparison of image-based and coordinate-based pooling of studies. NeuroImage, 45(3), 810–823. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.039
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  130. 130.
    Salthouse, T. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103(3), 403–428. PubMed. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.403
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  131. 131.↵
    Satterthwaite, T. D., Elliott, M. A., Gerraty, R. T., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J., Calkins, M. E., Eickhoff, S. B., Hakonarson, H., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., & Wolf, D. H. (2013). An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity data. NeuroImage, 64, 240–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.052
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  132. 132.↵
    Schulte, T., Müller-Oehring, E. M., Chanraud, S., Rosenbloom, M. J., Pfefferbaum, A., & Sullivan, E. V. (2011). Age-related reorganization of functional networks for successful conflict resolution: A combined functional and structural MRI study. Neurobiology of Aging, 32(11), 2075–2090. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.12.002
    OpenUrlPubMed
  133. 133.
    Sebastian, A., Baldermann, C., Feige, B., Katzev, M., Scheller, E., Hellwig, B., Lieb, K., Weiller, C., Tüscher, O., & Klöppel, S. (2013). Differential effects of age on subcomponents of response inhibition. Neurobiology of Aging, 34(9), 2183–2193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.03.013
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  134. 134.↵
    Shallice, Tim, & Burgess, P. W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114(2), 727–741. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/114.2.727
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  135. 135.↵
    Shallice, Timothy, Broadbent, D. E., & Weiskrantz, L. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 199–209. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1982.0082
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  136. 136.
    Smith, E. E., Geva, A., Jonides, J., Miller, A., Reuter-Lorenz, P., & Koeppe, R. A. (2001). The neural basis of task-switching in working memory: Effects of performance and aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(4), 2095. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.98.4.2095
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  137. 137.↵
    Spreng, R. N., & Schacter, D. L. (2011). Default Network Modulation and Large-Scale Network Interactivity in Healthy Young and Old Adults. Cerebral Cortex, 22(11), 2610–2621. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr339
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  138. 138.↵
    Spreng, R. N., Wojtowicz, M., & Grady, C. L. (2010). Reliable differences in brain activity between young and old adults: A quantitative meta-analysis across multiple cognitive domains. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(8), 1178–1194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.009
    OpenUrl
  139. 139.
    Steffener, J., Gazes, Y., Habeck, C., & Stern, Y. (2016). The Indirect Effect of Age Group on Switch Costs via Gray Matter Volume and Task-Related Brain Activity. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8, 162. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00162
    OpenUrl
  140. 140.
    Stuss, D. T., Shallice, T., Alexander, M. P., & Picton, T. W. (1995). A multidisciplinary approach to anterior attentional functions. In Structure and functions of the human prefrontal cortex. (pp. 191–211). New York Academy of Sciences.
  141. 141.↵
    Stuss, Donald T. (2006). Frontal lobes and attention: Processes and networks, fractionation and integration. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(2), 261–271. Cambridge Core. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617706060358
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  142. 142.
    Stuss, Donald T. (2011). Functions of the Frontal Lobes: Relation to Executive Functions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(5), 759–765. Cambridge Core. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000695
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  143. 143.↵
    1. K. M. Heilman &
    2. S. E. Nadeau
    Stuss, Donald T., & Craik, F. I. M. (2019). Alterations in Executive Functions with Aging. In K. M. Heilman & S. E. Nadeau (Eds.), Cognitive Changes and the Aging Brain (pp. 168–187). Cambridge University Press; Cambridge Core. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108554350.012
  144. 144.↵
    Talairach, J., & Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. Thieme.
  145. 145.↵
    Teuber, H. (1972). Unity and diversity of frontal lobe functions. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 32(2), 615–656. PubMed.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  146. 146.↵
    Toepper, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Gebhardt, H., Beblo, T., Bauer, E., Woermann, F. G., Driessen, M., & Sammer, G. (2014). The impact of age on prefrontal cortex integrity during spatial working memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 59, 157–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.04.020
    OpenUrl
  147. 147.↵
    Townsend, J., Adamo, M., & Haist, F. (2006). Changing channels: An fMRI study of aging and crossmodal attention shifts. NeuroImage, 31(4), 1682–1692. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.045
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  148. 148.↵
    Turkeltaub, P. E., Eden, G. F., Jones, K. M., & Zeffiro, T. A. (2002). Meta-Analysis of the Functional Neuroanatomy of Single-Word Reading: Method and Validation. NeuroImage, 16(3, Part A), 765–780. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1131
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  149. 149.↵
    Turkeltaub, P. E., Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Fox, M., Wiener, M., & Fox, P. (2012). Minimizing within-experiment and within-group effects in activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses. Human Brain Mapping, 33(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21186
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  150. 150.↵
    Turner, G. R., & Spreng, R. N. (2012). Executive functions and neurocognitive aging: Dissociable patterns of brain activity. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(4), 826.e1-826.e13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.005
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  151. 151.↵
    Utevsky, A. V., Smith, D. V., & Huettel, S. A. (2014). Precuneus Is a Functional Core of the Default-Mode Network. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(3), 932. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4227-13.2014
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  152. 152.↵
    Van Impe, A., Coxon, J. P., Goble, D. J., Wenderoth, N., & Swinnen, S. P. (2011). Age-related changes in brain activation underlying single- and dual-task performance: Visuomanual drawing and mental arithmetic. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2400–2409. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.016
    OpenUrl
  153. 153.
    Vellage, A.-K., Becke, A., Strumpf, H., Baier, B., Schönfeld, M. A., Hopf, J.-M., & Müller, N. G. (2016). Filtering and storage working memory networks in younger and older age. Brain and Behavior, 6(11), e00544. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.544
    OpenUrl
  154. 154.↵
    Wen, W., & Sachdev, P. (2004). The topography of white matter hyperintensities on brain MRI in healthy 60- to 64-year-old individuals. NeuroImage, 22(1), 144–154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.027
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  155. 155.↵
    Worringer, B., Langner, R., Koch, I., Eickhoff, S. B., Eickhoff, C. R., & Binkofski, F. C. (2019). Common and distinct neural correlates of dual-tasking and task-switching: A meta-analytic review and a neuro-cognitive processing model of human multitasking. Brain Structure and Function, 224(5), 1845–1869. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-019-01870-4
    OpenUrl
  156. 156.
    Worthy, D. A., Davis, T., Gorlick, M. A., Cooper, J. A., Bakkour, A., Mumford, J. A., Poldrack, R. A., & Todd Maddox, W. (2016). Neural correlates of state-based decision-making in younger and older adults. NeuroImage, 130, 13–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.12.004
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  157. 157.↵
    Xia, M., Wang, J., & He, Y. (2013). BrainNet Viewer: A network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PloS One, 8(7), e68910–e68910. PubMed. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068910
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  158. 158.↵
    Yang, Y. K., Chiu, N. T., Chen, C. C., Chen, M., Yeh, T. L., & Lee, I. H. (2003). Correlation between fine motor activity and striatal dopamine D2 receptor density in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 123(3), 191–197. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0925-4927(03)00066-0
    OpenUrl
  159. 159.↵
    Zanto, T. P., Rubens, M. T., Thangavel, A., & Gazzaley, A. (2011). Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual processing and working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 14(5), 656–661. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2773
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  160. 160.
    Zhu, D. C., Zacks, R. T., & Slade, J. M. (2010). Brain activation during interference resolution in young and older adults: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 50(2), 810–817. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.087
    OpenUrl
  161. 161.↵
    Zysset, S., Schroeter, M. L., Neumann, J., & Yves von Cramon, D. (2007). Stroop interference, hemodynamic response and aging: An event-related fMRI study. Neurobiology of Aging, 28(6), 937-946. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.05.008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted July 17, 2020.
Download PDF
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
The Aging Brain and Executive Functions Revisited: Implications from Meta-Analytic and Functional-Connectivity Evidence
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
The Aging Brain and Executive Functions Revisited: Implications from Meta-Analytic and Functional-Connectivity Evidence
Marisa K. Heckner, Edna C. Cieslik, Simon B. Eickhoff, Julia A. Camilleri, Felix Hoffstaedter, Robert Langner
bioRxiv 2020.07.15.204941; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.15.204941
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
The Aging Brain and Executive Functions Revisited: Implications from Meta-Analytic and Functional-Connectivity Evidence
Marisa K. Heckner, Edna C. Cieslik, Simon B. Eickhoff, Julia A. Camilleri, Felix Hoffstaedter, Robert Langner
bioRxiv 2020.07.15.204941; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.15.204941

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Neuroscience
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (2533)
  • Biochemistry (4977)
  • Bioengineering (3486)
  • Bioinformatics (15232)
  • Biophysics (6910)
  • Cancer Biology (5395)
  • Cell Biology (7753)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (4539)
  • Ecology (7159)
  • Epidemiology (2059)
  • Evolutionary Biology (10234)
  • Genetics (7517)
  • Genomics (9794)
  • Immunology (4863)
  • Microbiology (13234)
  • Molecular Biology (5144)
  • Neuroscience (29465)
  • Paleontology (203)
  • Pathology (838)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (1466)
  • Physiology (2142)
  • Plant Biology (4756)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (1013)
  • Synthetic Biology (1338)
  • Systems Biology (4014)
  • Zoology (768)