Revealing the structure of affective schematic representations in the medial prefrontal cortex ============================================================================================== * Philipp C. Paulus * Ian Charest * Roland G. Benoit ## Abstract Adaptive cognition is fostered by knowledge about the structure and value of our environment. Here, we hypothesize that these two kinds of information are inherently intertwined as affective schemas in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). Schemas (e.g., of a social network) emerge by extracting commonalities across experiences and can be understood as graphs comprising nodes (e.g., people) and edges (e.g., their relationships). We sampled information about unique real-life environments (i.e., about personally familiar people and places) and probed their neural representations with fMRI. Using representational similarity analysis, we show that the mPFC encodes both, the nodes and the edges of the schema. Critically, the strength of the edges is not only determined by experience and centrality of a given node but also by its affective value. We thus account for the involvement of the mPFC in seemingly disparate functions and suggest that valuation emerges naturally from encoded memory representations. ## Introduction Our rich knowledge of the past allows us to readily make sense of the present. It also facilitates adaptive planning for the future, for example by supporting simulations of prospective events (1–5). Critically, these capacities are not exclusively dependent on individual memories of unique past experiences. Instead, they are also based on generalized knowledge about our environment that is derived from multiple experiences (e.g., about relationships between familiar people)(3, 6). An example of such generalized knowledge structures are memory schemas (7–9). These representations of our environment can be understood as graphs comprising information about nodes (e.g., individual people) and their edges (i.e., their relationships) (8, 10–12). Schemas are formed by extracting commonalities across related events. They thereby reduce the complexity of our experience into simplified models of the world (8, 13, 14). Such models, in turn, foster planning and facilitate adaptive decisions (15–17). However, beyond a representation of the environment’s structure, adaptive cognition also requires a representation of what’s valuable within that environment (18). Here, we test the hypothesis that these two kinds of information are inherently intertwined in the rostral and ventral medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) (19–22). As detailed below, this proposal accounts for the apparent conundrum that this region is involved in two seemingly disparate functions: representing *memory schemas* and *affective value*. Evidence from humans (23, 24) and rodents(20, 25) indicates a critical role for the mPFC in mediating *memory schemas* (26, 27). Activity patterns in this region have been shown to code for individual nodes of the environment, such as familiar people (19, 28) and places(19, 29). These representations also entail information about some properties of the nodes, including how central individual people are to their social network (11, 30). However, it remains unclear whether the mPFC encodes representations of the nodes in isolation or whether these representations also entail information about their relationships (i.e., the edges). A largely independent line of research has associated the mPFC with the representation of *affective value* (21, 31–33). Activity in this region tracks the value of objects, people, or places that we currently perceive or imagine (19, 31, 34–36). Moreover, focal lesions lead to inconsistent value judgements (37, 38). The mPFC has thus been argued to represent value in a common currency that can then guide decisions (31, 32, 39). Notably, evidence from human neuroimaging (1, 19, 34, 36, 40) and rodent single cell-recordings (20, 22) has shown that representations of memories and of affective value are supported by *overlapping* parts of the mPFC. We thus reconcile the common attribution of these functions by hypothesizing that the schemas encoded by this region inherently also code for information about affective value. This hypothesis proposes that the mPFC encodes representations that are unique to individual nodes (e.g., individual familiar people), yet also entail information about their edges (e.g., the overall associations between the nodes). Critically, we suggest that nodes that are more important for a person exhibit stronger edges in the respective schema. We propose that the importance of a given node is jointly determined by three features: Given that schemas build up with the repeated experience of related events (8), we first expect that more familiar nodes should be more prominently embedded into the overall graph (34). Secondly, for the same reason, we also expect stronger embedding of nodes that are more central to the respective environment (11). Finally, given the role of the mPFC in affect and valuation (21, 31), we propose that more important nodes possess greater affective value (19, 20, 30). The encoded representations would thus emphasize connections of behaviorally relevant elements of the environment, reminiscent of the hippocampal weighting of rewarded locations (41, 42). Here, we test this hypothesis by probing the neural representations of two distinct and individually unique schemas: about people’s social networks and about places from their everyday environment. Participants provided names of people and places they personally know and arranged these names in circular arenas according to their associations (43) (Fig. 1 d). This allowed us to quantify the centrality of each exemplar (e.g., a person) to its respective schema (e.g., the social network). Participants also indicated their familiarity with each person and place (as an index of experience) and their liking of each of these exemplars (as an index of affective value). In a subsequent session, we measured their brain activity using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while they imagined interacting with each person and being at each place. We took the ensuing activity patterns to assess the neural representations of the individual nodes and their edges using representational similarity analysis (RSA) (44). ![Fig. 1.](http://biorxiv.org/https://www.biorxiv.org/content/biorxiv/early/2020/08/21/2020.08.21.260950/F1.medium.gif) [Fig. 1.](http://biorxiv.org/content/early/2020/08/21/2020.08.21.260950/F1) Fig. 1. Representations in the mPFC and PCC code for the nodes of participants’ real-life schemas. **a** We examined whether the mPFC encodes representations of the nodes by testing for the replicability of activity patterns for the same people and places across the two functional runs. Each row and column of the representational similarity matrix corresponds to a single simulation trial. **b** Regions coding for the nodes should show more similar activity patterns for the repeated simulation of the same person or place (same-item similarity) than for simulations entailing different nodes of the same category (different-item similarity). **c** The searchlight analysis identified regions coding for the nodes of real-life schemas. These entailed the mPFC and PCC. **d** Participants arranged the familiar people and places on circular arenas according to their associations, thus providing a measure of centrality. Participants also provided measures of experience and affective value by indicating their familiarity with the nodes as well as their liking. **e** Centrality, experience, and affective value load on a common principal component as indicated by significant positive correlations (* - *p* < .05, \***| - *p* < .001; one-tailed; *df* = 35). mPFC = medial prefrontal cortex, PCC = posterior cingulate cortex, CE = centrality, EX = experience, AV = affective value, PC = principal component. First, we hypothesized that the mPFC encodes unique representations of the nodes that get reinstated during mental simulation (19, 28, 29). We thus predicted similar activity patterns to emerge in the mPFC whenever participants imagine the same person or place. Second, we hypothesized that the structure of neural activity patterns *between* nodes further reflects the structure of their edges. We reasoned that any pair of nodes that is more strongly connected (i.e., that exhibits a stronger edge) is encoded by more overlapping neuronal populations (1, 45–47). This, in turn, should be reflected in overall greater neural similarity for nodes with particularly strong edges. As a consequence, if more important nodes have stronger edges, they should also exhibit overall greater neural similarity. In addition, we further gauge the regional specificity of affective schemas to the mPFC. Therefore, we contrast our findings with the structure of the neural activity patterns in the posterior cingulate cortex and the hippocampus, two regions that have been associated with similar memory (48–51) and evaluative processes (31, 35, 52). ## Results The medial prefrontal cortex codes for the nodes of participants’ real-life schemas. We first examined the hypothesis that the medial prefrontal cortex encodes representations of personally familiar people and places, i.e., the nodes of the respective schemas. Whenever we simulate an event involving a particular node, its representation should get reinstated in the mPFC. We thus took the ensuing fMRI activity patterns as proxies of their respective neural representations (19, 53) and examined their replicability using an RSA searchlight approach (radius = 8 mm, 4 voxels) (44). In regions that encode the nodes of the schema, we predicted overall greater pattern similarity for simulations featuring the same node (same-item similarity) than for simulations featuring different nodes (different-item similarity) (54). Note that the different-item measure was only based on the similarity of activity patterns for nodes of the same kind (i.e., either people *or* places) to ascertain that it is not deflated by potential categorical differences in the representation of people versus places (Fig. 1 a, b) (19, 53). Corroborating our previous finding (19), we obtained this effect in the mPFC. This region thus yielded replicable activity patterns that were specific to individual exemplars (Fig. 1 c, Supplement 1). Moreover, we also observed evidence for such replicable pattern reinstatement in a number of other brain regions that are typically engaged during the recollection of past memories and the simulation of prospective events (50, 55, 56). These regions include the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), the precuneus and parts of the lateral parietal and temporal cortices. Notably, there was no evidence for pattern reinstatement in the hippocampus. The data thus support our hypothesis that the mPFC encodes unique representations of individual nodes. In the following, we further examine the edges *between* nodes in the mPFC and PCC regions of interest (ROI) identified by this analysis. We also test for these effects in the hippocampus, even though this region showed no evidence for node coding. Note that the subsequent analyses of the edges are based on different parts of the neural representational similarity matrix (RSM) than the ones used to determine node coding. They are further based on comparisons of model RSMs that are also independent of the node coding model. In the supplement, we provide complementary and consistent results based on anatomically defined masks (see Supplementary Tables 2 – 4; Supplementary Figure 1 and 2). ### Centrality, experience, and affective value load on a common principal component that quantifies importance We had hypothesized that a node’s centrality, experience, and affective value contribute to its importance. These three features may thus share a common latent factor. First, to assess *centrality*, participants positioned the names of the people and places in circular arenas (Fig. 1 d). They were instructed to arrange nodes close together if they associate them strongly with each other and far apart if they do not (43). We calculated the centrality of each node as the sum of its inverse distances to all other nodes. Participants then arranged the people and places on continuous scales providing estimates of their familiarity with each node (as an index of *experience*) and of their liking (as an index of *affective value*). All three features were assessed separately for people and places. To test whether centrality, experience, and affective value load on a common latent factor, we *z*-scored each vector of values separately for each category (people, places) and within each participant. This approach prevents between-participant variance to influence the factor solution. We then performed principal component analyses, separately for the people and places. The respective first principal component explained, across all participants, 61% of variance for people and 46% for places. Critically, as predicted, both of these principal components exhibited significant positive correlations with each of the three features (Fig. 1 e). They thus quantify the importance of each individual node to its respective schema. In the next step, we used the individual importance values of the respective PC to predict the structure of the schemas’ edges. ### The medial prefrontal cortex encodes the edges of affective schematic representations We had hypothesized that nodes exhibit stronger edges that are more important to the schema. We had reasoned that the strength of edges between nodes is reflected in their patterns of neural similarity, assuming that more strongly connected nodes are also encoded by more overlapping neuronal populations (1, 20, 57). Our hypothesis thus predicts overall greater neural similarity for more important nodes. We tested this prediction by constructing models of the expected structure of representations in the mPFC. The models were based on the importance values derived from the respective principal component. Specifically, we predicted the similarity between any two nodes by the product of their respective principal component scores (i.e., importance values). Thus, we expected nodes that are more important to yield overall greater pattern similarity (Fig. 2 b). ![Fig. 2.](http://biorxiv.org/https://www.biorxiv.org/content/biorxiv/early/2020/08/21/2020.08.21.260950/F2.medium.gif) [Fig. 2.](http://biorxiv.org/content/early/2020/08/21/2020.08.21.260950/F2) Fig. 2. Only the structure of representations in the mPFC is best accounted for by the principal component. **a** Construction of the neural RSM. Each row and column of the matrix corresponds to a single simulation trial. In this analysis, we examine the similarity of activity patterns elicited by simulations of different people or places. **b** Construction of the model predictions. We predicted more similar representations for people and places with overall higher principal component scores, given that these more important exemplars should be more strongly embedded in their overall schema. To this end, we computed the combined importance of any two people or places from the product of their principal component scores. **c** Correlation of neural RSM and model prediction. Asterisks denote significant positive correlations as tested in a *t*-test on the *Fisher-z* transformed correlation coefficients (\***| - *p* < .001; one-tailed; *df* = 35). **d** Comparisons of linear mixed models further support the hypothesis: only the structure of representations in the mPFC is best explained by the principal component. The figure displays Log Evidence Ratios (LER). Smaller values indicate better fit. By definition, the best model assumes a value of zero. The dotted red line demarks a relative LER difference of two, regarded as decisive. **e** Comparisons with noise null models. Data points depict the mean model performance in comparisons with 1,000 random noise models (N) and sorted noise models (N+), whiskers indicate the standard deviation of the model performance. Shaded areas correspond to the region > N (darker shade) and > N+ (lighter shade). mPFC = medial prefrontal cortex, HPC = hippocampus, PCC = posterior cingulate cortex, CE = centrality, EX = experience, AV = affective value, PC = principal component, N = random noise, N+ = sorted noise. We then determined the neural similarity structure in the identified mPFC and PCC ROIs as well as in the hippocampus (Fig. 2 a). We constrained the broader cluster containing the PCC to the parts covering this region using an anatomical mask (58). We also used an anatomical mask from the same atlas to examine the representational structure for the hippocampus. All analyses were conducted in subject space. Finally, we tested for the correspondence between our prediction and the actual structure of neural representations by computing the correlation of the respective parts of the lower triangular vectors of both matrices (Fig. 2 a-c). This was done separately for people and places to examine whether the effect is present for either category. Using Kendall’s *τ**a* as a conservative estimate (59), we indeed observed significant correlations in the mPFC for both people (*mean τ**a* = 0.039, tested with a Wilcoxon test, *W* = 562, *p* < .001, *d* = 0.63, one-tailed, due to a deviation from normality indicated by a Shapiro-Wilk test, *W* = 0.92, *p* = 0.01) and places (*mean τ**a* = 0.026, *t*(35) = 3.72, *p* < .001, *d* = 0.62, one-tailed) - with no significant differences between the two (*t*(35) = 1.04, *p* = .307, *d* = 0.17, two-tailed). Similarly, the correlations were also significant in the PCC for people (*mean τ**a* = 0.046, *t*(35) = 3.91, *p* < .001, *d* = 0.65, one-tailed) and places (*mean τ**a* = 0.026, *t*(35) = 3.61, *p* < .001, *d* = 0.60, one-tailed), again with no significant differences between the two (*t*(35) = 1.42, *p* = .165, *d* = 0.24, two-tailed). However, the same analyses of the hippocampal data did not yield evidence for a match between the predicted and actual structure of representations for either people (*mean τ**a* = 0.003, *t*(35) = 0.41, *p* = .341, *d* = 0.07, one-tailed) or places (*mean τ**a* = 0.007, *t*(35) = 1.15, *p* = .13, *d* = 0.19, one-tailed). We obtained qualitatively identical results in analyses based on purely anatomically defined ROIs (see Supplementary Table 2 and 3; Supplementary Fig. 1). The results are also in accordance with a whole-brain searchlight analysis (radius = 8 mm, 4 voxels) (see Supplementary Table 5). We thus show that representations in the mPFC generally align with the predicted structure of affective schematic representations. However, for each of our ROIs it remains to be determined whether importance is indeed the best model to account for the structure of representations. ### The principal component is the best model to account for the structure of representations only in the mPFC Does the structure of representations predicted from the principal component account best for the data or would any of the individual contributing features provide a comparable or even a better fit? If the mPFC does encode affective schemas of the environment, we would expect the model based on importance to outperform models only based on centrality, experience, or affective value. Furthermore, we would expect some degree of regional specificity, i.e., that only representations in the mPFC are best accounted for by importance. We formally test these predictions by setting up alternative models that are only based on either centrality, experience, or affective value. We then test these models against the importance model based on the principal component. In brief, we set up linear mixed effects models to account for the structure of representations as a function of either one of these individual features. In each of these models, we included a factor of category (people, places) and the maximum possible random effects structure that would converge across all models and regions of interest. We thus accounted for between participant variance by including a random intercept per participant and run, as well as random slopes for our fixed effects of category and the respective predictor (e.g., the principal component scores). We then performed model comparisons within each ROI to determine the model that best fits the neural similarity structure. The comparisons were based on Log Evidence Ratios (*LER*) derived from Akaike’s Information Criterion (60). We regard *LER* differences greater than two as decisive evidence for the better model (61). Consistent with our hypothesis, in the mPFC, the principal component model accounted best for the data. It performed decisively better than affective value (*LER* = 4.19), experience (*LER* = 14.8) and centrality (*LER* = 35.16) (Fig. 2 d). The model parameters of this winning model entailed a significant main effect of category, reflecting overall higher neural pattern similarity for people than places *β**category_place* = −0.026, *SE* = 0.008, *χ*2 = 11.61, *p* < .001). Critically, they also included a significant positive parameter estimate for the principal component, indicating overall greater neural pattern similarity for nodes of greater importance (*β**PC* = 0.048, *SE* = 0.012, *χ*2 = 17.12, *p* < .001). Moreover, the main effect of the principal component did not interact with category (*β**category_place*:*PC* = −0.005, *SE* = 0.008, *χ*2 = 0.36, *p* = .546). By contrast, in both control regions, other models are better suited to account for the structure of representations. For the hippocampus, the model comparison yielded the best fit for centrality, though there was only a minimal advantage for this model over affective value (*LER* = 0.49). Notably, both models performed decisively better than the ones based on either the principal component (*LER* = 2.68) or experience (*LER* = 3.46). However, of the model parameters, only the main effect of category was significant, indicating overall higher pattern similarity for places than for people (*β**category_place* = 0.02, *SE* = 0.004, *χ*2 = 31.49, *p* < .001). There was neither a main effect of centrality (*β**CE* = −0.007, *SE* = 0.004, *χ*2 = 0.9, *p* = .342) nor an interaction of category with centrality (*β**category_place*:*CE* = 0.007, *SE* = 0.004, *χ*2 = 3.04, *p* = .081). The same pattern (i.e., only a main effect of category) also emerged for the model based on affective value (see Supplementary Table 4). For the PCC, the model based on affective value performed decisively better than any other model: principal component (*LER* = 4.14), experience (*LER* = 6.82), and centrality (*LER* = 33.99) (Fig. 2 d). The main effect of affective value was significant, indicating overall greater neural pattern similarity for nodes of higher affective value (*β**AV* = 0.026, *SE* = 0.007, *χ*2 = 10.71, *p* = .001). There was no main effect of category (*β**category_place* = 0.014, *SE* = 0.009, *χ*2 = 1.51, *p* = .219), but an interaction of affective value with category, reflecting a stronger effect of affective value for people than places (*β**category_place*:*AV* = −0.01, *SE* = 0.005, *χ*2 = 3.92, *p* = .048) (see Supplementary Table 4 for all model parameters). Note that we obtained consistent results when examining the structure of representations in the purely anatomically defined ROIs (see Supplementary Fig. 2; Supplementary Table 4). We finally sought to ensure that the winning models in each ROI perform better than noise null models. To this end, for each familiar person and place, we randomly sampled a value from a standard normal distribution. We then performed the identical processing steps for these vectors of values as for the other predictors to construct a noise model. Moreover, we derived a second noise model by sorting the noise vector in descending order prior to constructing the model. This was done to account for the order of columns in the neural RSM that was not randomly determined. Instead, the order reflected the serial position of the people and places as provided by the participants. Participants tend to start listing people and places with the ones that they like and know better. By sorting the noise vectors in descending order, we thus imposed a similar dependence between the noise values and their serial positions. Separately for each ROI, we then compared model performance of random noise and sorted noise against the winning model – or, if there were more than one candidate winning models with a *LER* < 2, against all candidate winning models. We repeated this process 1,000 times to obtain an estimate of the expected performance of the noise models and the winning model. As expected, in the mPFC, the principal component remained the best model (*mean LER* = 0, *SD* = 0.04), performing decisively better than sorted noise (*mean LER* = 15.56, *SD* = 2.51) and random noise (*mean LER* = 25.81, *SD* = 6.03). For the hippocampus, the initial model comparisons had provided only minimal evidence for centrality over affective value. We therefore compared both of these models with the noise models. Indeed, both models performed decisively better than sorted noise (*mean LER* = 4.4, *SD* = 1.09) and random noise (*mean LER* = 5.32, *SD* = 2.38). Again, there was minimal evidence for superiority of centrality (*mean LER* = 0.09, *SD* = 0.53) over affective value (*mean LER* = 0.58, *SD* = 0.53). The model comparisons in the PCC revealed strong though not decisive evidence for a superiority of affective value (*mean LER* = 1.15, *SD* = 2.1) over sorted noise (*mean LER* = 2.89, *SD* = 3.37). However, both did perform decisively better than random noise (*mean LER* = 29.46, *SD* = 9.01) (see Fig. 2 e). The results thus provide further evidence for the hypothesis that the mPFC encodes both the nodes and the edges of affective schematic representations. The model comparison more-over supports this account with some regional specificity. ## Discussion Human adaptive cognition is fostered by representations of the structure of our environment (7, 15, 17). Such structured representations act as templates that allow us to facilitate recollections of the past, to make sense of the present, and to flexibly anticipate the future (6, 8, 14, 34, 62). Structured representations have been described in the mPFC for various domains, ranging from spatial and conceptual to abstract state spaces (63–66). Our results support the hypothesis that the mPFC supports a specific form of such structured representations: affective schemas of our environment. Generally, the mPFC has long been argued to mediate memory schemas (26, 27, 62), yet the exact contribution of this region has remained unclear. It has been suggested that the mPFC serves to detect congruency of incoming information with schematic knowledge that is represented in posterior areas (27). This region would thus not necessarily represent any kind of schematic knowledge by itself. Our data show that the contribution of the mPFC goes beyond congruency detection: it directly encodes schematic representations of the environment (11, see also 19). These representations could act as pointer functions that guide the reinstatement of relevant distributed information (26, 27, 67, 68). This suggestion fits with broader accounts that situate the mPFC on top of a cortical hierarchy as a convergence zone (48, 69) that integrates information from diverse brain networks (34, 55). Critically, our results support the hypothesis that schematic representations in the mPFC (e.g., of one’s social network) inherently entail the value of the encoded nodes (e.g., the value of individual people). That is, the structure of the edges could best be accounted for by a model based on a latent factor that quantifies the importance of the nodes. As predicted, this factor was not only influenced by the nodes’ centrality (11, 30) and familiarity (19, 29, 34), but also by their affective value (19–21). The model based on this importance factor yielded a better fit to the data than alternative models based solely on either centrality, experience, or affective value. We obtained this pattern across schemas for personally familiar people and places, demonstrating that this coding scheme generalizes to different kinds of environmental representations. The model comparison also suggested some degree of regional specificityf or affective schemas. T he importance model was neither the best fit to the data obtained from the PCC nor from the hippocampus. Whereas even the best model did not outperform a noise model in the PCC, there was some evidence that the structure of the edges in the hippocampus could best be accounted for by either centrality or affective value. These results are consistent with evidence showing that the hippocampus encodes map-like representations of relational abstract (45, 70) and social (71) knowledge and that it is involved in value learning (72, 73). More broadly, a functional dissociation between the hippocampus and mPFC is also consistent with the suggested involvement of these regions in two complementary learning systems: Whereas the hippocampus is critical for the retention of individual episodes, the mPFC may extract commonalities across similar events and bind these into consolidated representations (14, 17, 41, 74, cf. 75). The mPFC would thus reduce the complexity of our experience into schematic summary representations. Indeed, a recent study provided convergent evidence for such dimension reduction in this region. It demonstrated that the mPFC compresses rich perceptual input to only those features that are currently task-relevant – akin to a principal component analysis (13). While such dimension reduction entails the loss of specific details, it also affords generaliz-ability and cognitive flexibility (8, 76). These representations can thus augment planning (6, 77, 78) and also be flexibly used for the construction and valuation of novel events (1, 34). The emergence of affective schemas in the mPFC could be fostered by hippocampal replay of past events (79–81). Such replay, mediated by monosynaptic hippocampal efferent projections (82), can potentially provide a teaching signal for the mPFC and thus facilitate neocortical consolidation (41). Moreover, to the degree that replay is biased towards valuable information, it may lead to a stronger weighting of those experiences that are of particular importance (41, 42). However, the mPFC likewise receives direct projections from areas such as the amygdala and striatum (83) that can also contribute to an affective shaping of the schematic representations (see also 21). Importantly, the highlighted structure of representations in the mPFC provides a common account for the involvement of this region in both memory schemas and valuation. That is, when we think about an individual element from our environment (e.g., a known person), its representation in the mPFC is activated. This activation then spreads throughout the network of connected nodes. Critically, we suggest that there is a wider spread from nodes that are more valuable and that are thus more strongly embedded in their overarching schema. This wider spread, in turn, may manifest as greater regional univariate activity. According to this account, the valuation signal that has been attributed to the mPFC (31, 35) thus constitutes an emergent property of the structure of its encoded representations. This interpretation similarly accounts for the stronger engagement of the mPFC when individuals think about themselves as compared to others (84, 85). The self can be considered a super-ordinate schema that entails abstracted representations of all our personal experiences (67). Instantiating this schema would thus presumably lead to wide spread activity, whereas thinking about specific other people would only co-activate neural representations of more restricted nodes. Moreover, the net activity would be lower for other people that we feel less connected to and that we have less experience with (34, 86–88). To conclude, this study provides evidence that the medial pre-frontal cortex represents the structure of our environment in the form of affective schemas. These schemas reflect our experience with individual nodes as well as their centrality. Critically, they also inherently encode information about their affective value. Affective schematic representations thus prioritize information that is critical for adaptive planning and ultimately promotes our well-being and survival. ## Methods ### Participants We recruited 39 right handed healthy unmedicated adults (23 females; age: *mean* = 25.4 years, *SD* = 2.6 years) from the study database of the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences. All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision, provided written informed consent and received monetary compensation for their participation. The experimental protocol was approved by the local ethics committee (Ethical Committee at the Medical Faculty, Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany). Three participants had to be excluded from analysis either because of a recording error (*n* = 1), or excessive movement (*n* = 2). Excessive movement was defined as absolute movement 3 mm within either run or a total of 5 episodes of movement 0.5 mm. We thus included 36 participants (22 females; age: *mean* = 25.2 years, *SD* = 2.5 years) in the analysis. ### Task and procedures The procedure, adapted from ref. 19, comprised two sessions. During the first session, participants provided names of personally familiar people and of such places. Participants tend to start by listing people and places that they are most familiar with and that they like the most. We therefore asked them to provide us with 90 people and places and then randomly sampled 30 of each category to ensure a greater variability in these variables of interest. ### Assessing centrality, experience, and affective value To quantify the centrality of each node to the schema, participants arranged the names of the people and places on two-dimensional circular arenas using the multiple arrangements task (43) (Fig. 1 d). We instructed participants to position names closer to each other that they also associate more strongly. The inverse of the distance thus serves as a measure of associatedness between any two nodes. We quantified the centrality of each person and place to its schema by computing their centrality, i.e., the sum of their associatedness values. We then assessed how much experience participants had with each person and place. The participants therefore placed the names on continuous familiarity scales ranging from “not at all familiar” to “very much familiar” (Fig. 1 d). Finally, participants provided a measure of affective value for each person and place by arranging their names on continuous liking scales ranging from “not at all liked” to “very much liked”. All arrangements were done separately for people and places. ### Assessing neural representations The participants returned for a separate session (*median* delay: 1 day; *range*: 1–4 days) to complete the episodic simulation task in the fMRI scanner. Each trial of the simulation task began with a fixation period of 0.5 s, followed by the name of a person or a place for 7.5 s. During this time, participants imagined interacting with the person in a typical manner or being at the place engaging in a location specific activity. Participants were instructed to imagine the episode as vividly as possible, so that they have a clear mental picture of the respective person or place. Participants then rated the vividness of their imagination on a five-point scale within a maximum of 3 s. Trials for which participants failed to press a button within that time period were later removed from analysis. If there was time left from the response window, it was added to the subsequent inter trial interval. This lasted for at least 3 s plus an additional jittered period (0 to 8 s in 2 s intervals). The screen during the inter trial interval was blank. Each person and place was presented once in each of the two functional runs that followed different random orders. Before entering the scanner, participants practiced the simulation task with people and places that they had previously provided but that did not feature in the simulation task proper. After the simulation task, participants were presented with people-places and faces-places localizers. Outside the scanner, they provided further information regarding the associations and identities of the individual people and places, including their addresses and locations. They also completed a number of standard questionnaires. These data were not analyzed for the current study. ### fMRI data acquisition Participants were scanned with a 3 Tesla Siemens Magnetom PRISMA MRI scanner with a 32-channel head coil. We acquired anatomical images with a T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo sequence (MPRAGE, 256 sagittal slices, TR = 2300 ms, TE = 2.98 ms, flip angle = 9°, 1 × 1 × 1 mm3 voxels, FoV = 240 mm, GRAPPA factor = 2). For each of the two functional runs of the simulation task, we acquired 469 volumes of blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) data with a T2*-weighted echo-planar imaging (EPI) pulse sequence (89, 90). This sequence employed multiband RF pulses with the following parameters: 72 interleaved axial-oblique slices (angled 15° towards coronal from AC-PC), TR = 2000 ms, TE = 25 ms, flip angle = 90°, 2 × 2 × 2 mm3 voxels, 6/8 partial Fourier, FoV = 192 mm, MF = 3). The first five volumes of each run were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration effects. fMRI data analysis. Data were analyzed using SPM12 ([www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm](http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm)) in Matlab (version 9.3). The functional images were corrected for slice acquisition times, realigned, corrected for field d istortions, a nd co-registered with the anatomical scan. Correction for field distortions was achieved using FSL topup (91, 92) as implemented in FSL 5.0 ([https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/](https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/)). We then decomposed the variance in the BOLD time-series using a general linear model (GLM) in SPM12 (93). Each model included six regressors representing residual movement artifacts, plus regressors modeling the intercepts of block and session. The additional regressors in the GLM coded for the effects-of-interest. Specifically, we modeled each trial as aseparate condition yielding a total of 120 regressors – one for each of the two simulations of the 30 people and 30 places. The trial regressors were convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function. A 1/128-Hz high-pass filter was applied to the data and the model. We computed *t*-maps for the estimated parameters of interest (i.e., for each simulation) against the implicit baseline. The ensuing parameters were used for representational similarity analysis (RSA) (44, 59). ### Identifying regions that code for nodes of the schema To identify brain regions that encode representations of individual people and places (i.e., the nodes of the schema), we employed an RSA searchlight analysis (spheres with a radius of 8mm, 4 vx.) across all grey matter voxels. This analysis was based on the toolbox by ref. 59 and compared activity patterns across functional runs (54, 94). It identified regions where two simulations of the same person or place yielded more similar activity patterns (same-item similarity) than any two simulations of different people or places (different-item similarity). Specifically, we assessed same-item similarity as the Pearson correlation between the activity pattern of the initial simulation of any given node in the first and its repeated simulation in the second run. Different-item similarity was computed as the average correlation of the initial simulation of a node in the first run with all other nodes of the same category (people, places) in the second run. By constraining the different-item similarity to items of the same category, we ensure that it is not affected by general differences in the neural representation of people versus places. We finally determined the magnitude of the node coding as the difference score between same- and different-item similarity (19, 54). This searchlight analysis yielded a node-coding map for each individual participant. For second level analyses, we *Fisherz*-transformed these maps, normalized them into MNI space using the DARTEL (95) estimated deformation fields, and smoothed them with a Gaussian Kernel of 8 mm radius at full-width-half-max. We then masked the smoothed map with the normalized grey matter masks and tested the significance of the node-coding effect using a simple *t*-contrast at each voxel. We used voxel-level inference at *p* < 0.05 (family-wise-error-corrected), and regarded only clusters that comprised at least 30 contiguous voxels. ### Examining the structure of the edges The second RSA examined whether regions that code for the nodes of the schema also code for the predicted relationships between the nodes (i.e., their edges). This analysis thus examined data from regions-of-interest based on the thresholded nodecoding map. Note that the two sets of analyses are based on different parts of the neural RSM and on comparisons of model RSMs that are independent from the node-coding model. For the mPFC, we joined the two rostral and ventral clusters. For the PCC, we took the conjunction of a broad cluster that included this region and an anatomical PCC mask from the Brainnetome atlas (58) (regions 175, 176, 181, 182). For the hippocampal ROI, we merged its rostral and caudal parts of the same atlas (regions 215 – 218). Voxels were included if they had at least 50% probability of being part of the mask and grey matter. For complementary analyses, we also examined the data solely based on an anatomical mask of the ventral mPFC used by ref. 19 (see also 96), a more spatially extended mask including the rostral mPFC (comprising Brainnetome regions 13, 14, 41, 42, 47 – 50, 187, 188), and said anatomical mask of the PCC. All masks were inverse normalized into subject space using the DARTEL estimated deformation fields and constrained using the implicit mask estimated from the first level GLMs. ### Extraction of the importance weights using Principal Component Analysis We had hypothesized that centrality, experience, and affective value would jointly contribute to the importance of a node and expected that they would share a common latent factor. We thus applied principal component analysis (PCA) to the three features and computed the latent factor that explains most variance. The PCA were conducted separately for people and places and based on values of each variable that had been *z*-scored for each participant. This approach ascertained that neither between-category variance nor between-participant variance would bias the factor solution. We then extracted, across all participants, the respective first principal component for people and places. We take these PCs to reflect the relative importance of each individual node to its affective schema. ### Predicting the structure of affective schematic representations We used the importance values to predict the structure of affective schematic representations. We had hypothesized that more important nodes should, overall, exhibit greater neural similarity with the other nodes. We thus predicted the similarity for any pair of nodes by the product of their respective importance values. We scaled the vectors to the interval of zero (lowest importance) and one (highest importance) prior to multiplication. We then arranged the combined importance values in square matrices for each category (people, places). In initial analyses, we examined the structure of representations separately for the schemas comprising people and places. Note that all analyses are only based on the lower triangular vector of the representational similarity matrices. ### Testing the structure of the neural representations using Linear Mixed Effects Models We set up a series of linear mixed effects models in R (version 3.5.1, [www.r-project.org](http://www.r-project.org)), using LME4 (97), to test which of several alternative predictors accounted best for the structure of representations in the ROIs. These models accounted for the neural similarity data as a function of the full fixed effects of category (people, places) and predictor of interest (i.e., centrality, experience, affective value, or the principal component). We further accounted for between participant variance by including random effects: one random intercept for participant and run as well as random slopes for category and the respective predictor. Hence, all models were of the form: ![Figure3](http://biorxiv.org/https://www.biorxiv.org/content/biorxiv/early/2020/08/21/2020.08.21.260950/F3.medium.gif) [Figure3](http://biorxiv.org/content/early/2020/08/21/2020.08.21.260950/F3) We estimated the models separately for each ROI and subsequently performed model comparisons based on the relative Log Evidence Ratios (*LER*) derived from Akaike’s Information Criterion (60). The best model assumes, by definition, a relative *LER* of zero, and we regard relative *LER* differences greater than two as *decisive* evidence for the better model (61). We further examined whether the winning models in each ROI are also substantially superior to models based on random Gaussian noise. We thus created null models by randomly sampling 30 values from a standard normal distribution for both people and places. We then rescaled these values to the interval from zero to one. Subsequently, we constructed a noise null model by computing the product of every two combination of values, just as we had done for our predictors of interest. We also created a second null model by sorting the same random noise values in descending order prior to multiplication. This was done to account for the inherent order of the original lists of people and places provided by the participants that tended to start with more familiar and pleasant exemplars. Thus, people and places that were named first always received larger random numbers than those named later. We then fit linear mixed effect models for these two noise null models, and performed a model comparison with the winning model(s) from the respective ROI. We repeated this estimation process 1,000 times to compute average model performance. Critically, if the winning model(s) in each ROI constitute(s) a good approximation of the structure of neural representations, they should consistently outperform both the random noise and the sorted noise model. ## Supporting information Supplementary material [[supplements/260950_file02.pdf]](pending:yes) ## AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS PCP and RGB designed the study with input from IC. PCP collected the data and wrote the analysis code in consultation with IC and RGB. PCP and RGB analyzed the data and wrote the paper incorporating feedback from IC. ## COMPETING INTERESTS STATEMENT The authors declare no competing interests. ## ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work was supported by a Max Planck Research Group (awarded to RGB). We thank Roxanne Eisenbeis for assistance in data collection, Ruud Berkers for help in setting up preprocessing, Davide Stramaccia for discussing the linear mixed effects models, and Angharad Williams for comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. * Received August 21, 2020. * Revision received August 21, 2020. * Accepted August 21, 2020. * © 2020, Posted by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory This pre-print is available under a Creative Commons License (Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs 4.0 International), CC BY-NC-ND 4.0, as described at [http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/](http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/) ## References 1. 1.Barron, H. C., Dolan, R. J. & Behrens, T. E. J. Online evaluation of novel choices by simultaneous representation of multiple memories. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1492–1498 (2013). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/nn.3515&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=24013592&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 2. 2.Hassabis, D. & Maguire, E. A. Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 299–306 (2007). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.001&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17548229&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000248156700007&link_type=ISI) 3. 3.Irish, M., Addis, D. R., Hodges, J. R. & Piguet, O. Considering the role of semantic memory in episodic future thinking: evidence from semantic dementia. Brain 135, 2178–2191 (2012). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/brain/aws119&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=22614246&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000305826300017&link_type=ISI) 4. 4.Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G. & Szpunar, K. K. Episodic future thinking: mechanisms and functions. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 17, 41–50 (2017). 5. 5.Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M. C. The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behav. Brain. Sci. 30, 299–313; discussion 313–351 (2007). [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000254847800034&link_type=ISI) 6. 6.Addis, D. R. Mental time travel? A neurocognitive model of event simulation. Rev. Phil. Psych. 11, 233–259 (2020). 7. 7.Bartlett, F. C. Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1932). 8. 8.Ghosh, V. E. & Gilboa, A. What is a memory schema? A historical perspective on current neuroscience literature. Neuropsychologia 53, 104–114 (2014). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.11.010&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=24280650&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000331663100011&link_type=ISI) 9. 9.Piaget, J. The origins of intelligence in children (W.W. Norton & Co, New York, 1952). 10. 10.Chen, C., Lu, Q., Beukers, A., Baldassano, C. & Norman, K. A. Learning to perform role-filler binding with schematic knowledge. arXiv:1902.09006 *[cs]* (2020). URL [http://arxiv.org/abs/1902.09006](http://arxiv.org/abs/1902.09006). 11. 11.Parkinson, C., Kleinbaum, A. M. & Wheatley, T. Spontaneous neural encoding of social network position. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 0072 (2017). 12. 12.1. Anderson, R., 2. Spiro, R. & 3. Montague, W. Rumelhart, D. & Ortony, A. The representation of knowledge in memory. In Anderson, R., Spiro, R. & Montague, W. (eds.) Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge, 99–135 (Erlbaum, Hilsdale, 1976). 13. 13.Mack, M. L., Preston, A. R. & Love, B. C. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex compression during concept learning. Nat. Commun. 11, 1–11 (2020). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/s41467-019-13889-6&link_type=DOI) 14. 14.Moscovitch, M., Cabeza, R., Winocur, G. & Nadel, L. Episodic memory and beyond: the hippocampus and neocortex in transformation. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 105–134 (2016). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143733&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=26726963&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 15. 15.Behrens, T. E. J. et al. What is a cognitive map? Organizing knowledge for flexible behavior. Neuron 100, 490–509 (2018). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.002&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=30359611&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 16. 16.Bulley, A. & Schacter, D. L. Deliberating trade-offs with the future. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 238–247 (2020). 17. 17.Morton, N. W., Sherrill, K. R. & Preston, A. R. Memory integration constructs maps of space, time, and concepts. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 17, 161–168 (2017). 18. 18.O’Doherty, J. P., Cockburn, J. & Pauli, W. M. Learning, reward, and decision making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 68, 73–100 (2017). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044216&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=27687119&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 19. 19.Benoit, R. G., Paulus, P. C. & Schacter, D. L. Forming attitudes via neural activity supporting affective episodic simulations. Nat. Commun. 10, 2215 (2019). 20. 20.Farovik, A. et al. Orbitofrontal cortex encodes memories within value-based schemas and represents contexts that guide memory retrieval. J. Neurosci. 35, 8333–8344 (2015). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Njoiam5ldXJvIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEwOiIzNS8yMS84MzMzIjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6NDg6Ii9iaW9yeGl2L2Vhcmx5LzIwMjAvMDgvMjEvMjAyMC4wOC4yMS4yNjA5NTAuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 21. 21.Roy, M., Shohamy, D. & Wager, T. D. Ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical systems and the generation of affective meaning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 147–156 (2012). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.005&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=22310704&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000301635500008&link_type=ISI) 22. 22.Zhou, J. et al. Rat orbitofrontal ensemble activity contains multiplexed but dissociable representations of value and task structure in an odor sequence task. Curr. Biol. 29, 897–907.e3 (2019). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.cub.2019.01.048&link_type=DOI) 23. 23.Brod, G. & Shing, Y. L. Specifying the role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in memory formation. Neuropsychologia 111, 8–15 (2018). 24. 24.Ghosh, V. E., Moscovitch, M., Colella, B. M. & Gilboa, A. Schema representation in patients with ventromedial PFC lesions. J. Neurosci. 34, 12057–12070 (2014). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Njoiam5ldXJvIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjExOiIzNC8zNi8xMjA1NyI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjQ4OiIvYmlvcnhpdi9lYXJseS8yMDIwLzA4LzIxLzIwMjAuMDguMjEuMjYwOTUwLmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 25. 25.Tse, D. et al. Schema-dependent gene activation and memory encoding in neocortex. Science 333, 891–895 (2011). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Mzoic2NpIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEyOiIzMzMvNjA0NC84OTEiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czo0ODoiL2Jpb3J4aXYvZWFybHkvMjAyMC8wOC8yMS8yMDIwLjA4LjIxLjI2MDk1MC5hdG9tIjt9czo4OiJmcmFnbWVudCI7czowOiIiO30=) 26. 26.Gilboa, A. & Marlatte, H. Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 618–631 (2017). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.013&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=28551107&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 27. 27.van Kesteren, M. T. R., Ruiter, D. J., Fernández, G. & Henson, R. N. How schema and novelty augment memory formation. Trends Neurosci. 35, 211–219 (2012). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.tins.2012.02.001&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=22398180&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000302971900001&link_type=ISI) 28. 28.Thornton, M. A. & Mitchell, J. P. Theories of person perception predict patterns of neural activity during mentalizing. Cereb. Cortex 28, 3505–3520 (2018). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/cercor/bhx216&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=28968854&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 29. 29.Robin, J., Buchsbaum, B. R. & Moscovitch, M. The primacy of spatial context in the neural representation of events. J. Neurosci. 38, 2755–2765 (2018). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Njoiam5ldXJvIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEwOiIzOC8xMS8yNzU1IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6NDg6Ii9iaW9yeGl2L2Vhcmx5LzIwMjAvMDgvMjEvMjAyMC4wOC4yMS4yNjA5NTAuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 30. 30.Zerubavel, N., Bearman, P. S., Weber, J. & Ochsner, K. N. Neural mechanisms tracking popularity in real-world social networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, 15072–15077 (2015). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoicG5hcyI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czoxMjoiMTEyLzQ5LzE1MDcyIjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6NDg6Ii9iaW9yeGl2L2Vhcmx5LzIwMjAvMDgvMjEvMjAyMC4wOC4yMS4yNjA5NTAuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 31. 31.Bartra, O., McGuire, J. T. & Kable, J. W. The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value. Neuroimage 76, 412–427 (2013). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.063&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=23507394&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000319090300038&link_type=ISI) 32. 32.Peters, J. & Büchel, C. Neural representations of subjective reward value. Behav. Brain. Res. 213, 135–141 (2010). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.031&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20420859&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000279748600001&link_type=ISI) 33. 33.Winecoff, A. et al. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex encodes emotional value. J. Neurosci. 33, 11032–11039 (2013). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Njoiam5ldXJvIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjExOiIzMy8yNy8xMTAzMiI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjQ4OiIvYmlvcnhpdi9lYXJseS8yMDIwLzA4LzIxLzIwMjAuMDguMjEuMjYwOTUwLmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 34. 34.Benoit, R. G., Szpunar, K. K. & Schacter, D. L. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex supports affective future simulation by integrating distributed knowledge. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111, 16550–16555 (2014). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoicG5hcyI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czoxMjoiMTExLzQ2LzE2NTUwIjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6NDg6Ii9iaW9yeGl2L2Vhcmx5LzIwMjAvMDgvMjEvMjAyMC4wOC4yMS4yNjA5NTAuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 35. 35.Clithero, J. A. & Rangel, A. Informatic parcellation of the network involved in the computation of subjective value. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1289–1302 (2014). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/scan/nst106&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=23887811&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 36. 36.Lin, W.-J., Horner, A. J., Bisby, J. A. & Burgess, N. Medial prefrontal cortex: adding value to imagined scenarios. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 1957–1967 (2015). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access\_num=10.1162/jocn_a_00836&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=26042501&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 37. 37.Camille, N., Griffiths, C. A., Vo, K., Fellows, L. K. & Kable, J. W. Ventromedial frontal lobe damage disrupts value maximization in humans. J. Neurosci. 31, 7527–7532 (2011). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Njoiam5ldXJvIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEwOiIzMS8yMC83NTI3IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6NDg6Ii9iaW9yeGl2L2Vhcmx5LzIwMjAvMDgvMjEvMjAyMC4wOC4yMS4yNjA5NTAuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 38. 38.Fellows, L. K. The functions of the frontal lobes: evidence from patients with focal brain damage. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 163, 19–34 (2019). 39. 39.Lim, S.-L., O’Doherty, J. P. & Rangel, A. Stimulus value signals in ventromedial PFC reflect the integration of attribute value signals computed in fusiform gyrus and posterior superior temporal gyrus. J. Neurosci. 33, 8729–8741 (2013). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Njoiam5ldXJvIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEwOiIzMy8yMC84NzI5IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6NDg6Ii9iaW9yeGl2L2Vhcmx5LzIwMjAvMDgvMjEvMjAyMC4wOC4yMS4yNjA5NTAuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 40. 40.Shenhav, A., Barrett, L. F. & Bar, M. Affective value and associative processing share a cortical substrate. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 13, 46–59 (2013). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.3758/s13415-012-0128-4&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=23090717&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 41. 41.Kumaran, D., Hassabis, D. & McClelland, J. L. What learning systems do intelligent agents need? Complementary learning systems theory updated. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 512–534 (2016). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.004&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=27315762&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 42. 42.Schafer, M. & Schiller, D. Navigating social space. Neuron 100, 476–489 (2018). 43. 43.Kriegeskorte, N. & Mur, M. Inverse MDS: inferring dissimilarity structure from multiple item arrangements. Front. Psychol. 3, 245 (2012). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00245&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=22848204&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 44. 44.Kriegeskorte, N., Mur, M. & Bandettini, P. Representational similarity analysis – connecting the branches of systems neuroscience. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2(2008). 45. 45.Garvert, M. M., Dolan, R. J. & Behrens, T. E. A map of abstract relational knowledge in the human hippocampal–entorhinal cortex. eLife 6, e17086 (2017). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.7554/eLife.17086&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=28448253&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 46. 46.Josselyn, S. A. & Frankland, P. W. Memory allocation: mechanisms and function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 41, 389–413 (2018). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-061956&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=29709212&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 47. 47.Sawamura, H., Orban, G. A. & Vogels, R. Selectivity of neuronal adaptation does not match response selectivity: a single-cell study of the fMRI adaptation paradigm. Neuron 49, 307–318 (2006). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuron.2005.11.028&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16423703&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000234979900016&link_type=ISI) 48. 48.Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R. & Buckner, R. L. Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron 65, 550–562 (2010). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20188659&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000275359800013&link_type=ISI) 49. 49.Baldassano, C., Hasson, U. & Norman, K. A. Representation of real-world event schemas during narrative perception. J. Neurosci. 38, 9689–9699 (2018). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Njoiam5ldXJvIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEwOiIzOC80NS85Njg5IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6NDg6Ii9iaW9yeGl2L2Vhcmx5LzIwMjAvMDgvMjEvMjAyMC4wOC4yMS4yNjA5NTAuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 50. 50.Benoit, R. G. & Schacter, D. L. Specifying the core network supporting episodic simulation and episodic memory by activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia 75, 450–457 (2015). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.034&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=26142352&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 51. 51.Maguire, E. A., Frith, C. D. & Morris, R. G. M. The functional neuroanatomy of comprehension and memory: the importance of prior knowledge. Brain 122, 1839–1850 (1999). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/brain/122.10.1839&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10506087&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000083243400004&link_type=ISI) 52. 52.Grueschow, M., Polania, R., Hare, T. A. & Ruff, C. C. Automatic versus choice-dependent value representations in the human brain. Neuron 85, 874–885 (2015). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.054&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=25640078&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 53. 53.Charest, I., Kievit, R. A., Schmitz, T. W., Deca, D. & Kriegeskorte, N. Unique semantic space in the brain of each beholder predicts perceived similarity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111, 14565–14570 (2014). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoicG5hcyI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czoxMjoiMTExLzQwLzE0NTY1IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6NDg6Ii9iaW9yeGl2L2Vhcmx5LzIwMjAvMDgvMjEvMjAyMC4wOC4yMS4yNjA5NTAuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 54. 54.Nili, H., Walther, A., Alink, A. & Kriegeskorte, N. Inferring exemplar discriminability in brain representations. PLoS ONE 15, e0232551 (2020). 55. 55.Ritchey, M., Libby, L. A. & Ranganath, C. Cortico-hippocampal systems involved in memory and cognition: the PMAT framework. Prog. Brain. Res. 219, 45–64 (2015). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.04.001&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=26072233&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 56. 56.Rugg, M. D. & Vilberg, K. L. Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 255–260 (2013). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.005&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=23206590&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 57. 57.Milivojevic, B., Vicente-Grabovetsky, A. & Doeller, C. F. Insight reconfigures hippocampal-prefrontal memories. Curr. Biol. 25, 821–830 (2015). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.033&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=25728693&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 58. 58.Fan, L. et al. The Human Brainnetome Atlas: a new brain atlas based on connectional architecture. Cereb. Cortex 26, 3508–3526 (2016). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/cercor/bhw157&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=27230218&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 59. 59.Nili, H. et al. A toolbox for representational similarity analysis. PLoS Comput. Biol. 10, e1003553 (2014). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003553&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=24743308&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 60. 60.Snipes, M. & Taylor, D. C. Model selection and Akaike Information Criteria: an example from wine ratings and prices. J. Wine. Econ. 3, 3–9 (2014). 61. 61.Kass, R. E. & Raftery, A. E. Bayes Factors. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 90, 773–795 (1995). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.2307/2291091&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10783804&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1995RA10400045&link_type=ISI) 62. 62.Burgess, P. W. & Shallice, T. Confabulation and the control of recollection. Memory 4, 359–411 (1996). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1080/096582196388906&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8817460&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1996UY48400002&link_type=ISI) 63. 63.Constantinescu, A. O., OReilly, J. X. & Behrens, T. E. J. Organizing conceptual knowledge in humans with a gridlike code. Science 352, 1464–1468 (2016). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Mzoic2NpIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEzOiIzNTIvNjI5Mi8xNDY0IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6NDg6Ii9iaW9yeGl2L2Vhcmx5LzIwMjAvMDgvMjEvMjAyMC4wOC4yMS4yNjA5NTAuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 64. 64.Doeller, C. F., Barry, C. & Burgess, N. Evidence for grid cells in a human memory network. Nature 463, 657–661 (2010). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/nature08704&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20090680&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000274193900035&link_type=ISI) 65. 65.Schapiro, A. C., Rogers, T. T., Cordova, N. I., Turk-Browne, N. B. & Botvinick, M. M. Neural representations of events arise from temporal community structure. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 486–492 (2013). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/nn.3331&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=23416451&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 66. 66.Schuck, N., Cai, M., Wilson, R. & Niv, Y. Human orbitofrontal cortex represents a cognitive map of state space. Neuron 91, 1402–1412 (2016). 67. 67.Gilboa, A. & Moscovitch, M. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex generates pre-stimulus theta coherence desynchronization: a schema instantiation hypothesis. Cortex 87, 16–30 (2017). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.cortex.2016.10.008&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=27890323&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 68. 68.Ciaramelli, E., De Luca, F., Monk, A. M., McCormick, C. & Maguire, E. A. What “wins” in VMPFC: Scenes, situations, or schema? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 100, 208–210 (2019). 69. 69.Margulies, D. S. et al. Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 113, 12574–12579 (2016). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoicG5hcyI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czoxMjoiMTEzLzQ0LzEyNTc0IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6NDg6Ii9iaW9yeGl2L2Vhcmx5LzIwMjAvMDgvMjEvMjAyMC4wOC4yMS4yNjA5NTAuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 70. 70.Theves, S., Fernandez, G. & Doeller, C. F. The hippocampus encodes distances in multidimensional feature space. Curr. Biol. 29, 1226–1231.e3 (2019). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.cub.2019.02.035&link_type=DOI) 71. 71.Tavares, R. M. et al. A map for social navigation in the human brain. Neuron 87, 231–243 (2015). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.011&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=26139376&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 72. 72.Stachenfeld, K. L., Botvinick, M. M. & Gershman, S. J. The hippocampus as a predictive map. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1643–1653 (2017). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/nn.4650&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=28967910&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 73. 73.Wimmer, G. E. & Shohamy, D. Preference by association: how memory mechanisms in the hippocampus bias decisions. Science 338, 270–273 (2012). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Mzoic2NpIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEyOiIzMzgvNjEwNC8yNzAiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czo0ODoiL2Jpb3J4aXYvZWFybHkvMjAyMC8wOC8yMS8yMDIwLjA4LjIxLjI2MDk1MC5hdG9tIjt9czo4OiJmcmFnbWVudCI7czowOiIiO30=) 74. 74.Norman, K. A. & O’Reilly, R. C. Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: a complementary-learning-systems approach. Psychol. Rev. 110, 611–646 (2003). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/0033-295X.110.4.611&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=14599236&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000186332700001&link_type=ISI) 75. 75.Schapiro, A. C., Turk-Browne, N. B., Botvinick, M. M. & Norman, K. A. Complementary learning systems within the hippocampus: a neural network modelling approach to reconciling episodic memory with statistical learning. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 372, 20160049 (2017). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1098/rstb.2016.0049&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=27872368&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 76. 76.Bowman, C. R. & Zeithamova, D. Abstract memory representations in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus support concept generalization. J. Neurosci. 38, 2605–2614 (2018). [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Njoiam5ldXJvIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEwOiIzOC8xMC8yNjA1IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6NDg6Ii9iaW9yeGl2L2Vhcmx5LzIwMjAvMDgvMjEvMjAyMC4wOC4yMS4yNjA5NTAuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 77. 77.Momennejad, I. Learning structures: predictive representations, replay, and generalization. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 32, 155–166 (2020). 78. 78.Günseli, E. & Aly, M. Preparation for upcoming attentional states in the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. eLife 9, e53191 (2020). 79. 79.Carr, M. F., Jadhav, S. P. & Frank, L. M. Hippocampal replay in the awake state: a potential substrate for memory consolidation and retrieval. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 147–153 (2011). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/nn.2732&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=21270783&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000286595400010&link_type=ISI) 80. 80.Michelmann, S., Staresina, B. P., Bowman, H. & Hanslmayr, S. Speed of time-compressed forward replay flexibly changes in human episodic memory. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 143–154 (2019). 81. 81.Wimmer, G. E., Liu, Y., Vehar, N., Behrens, T. E. J. & Dolan, R. J. Episodic memory retrieval success is associated with rapid replay of episode content. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 1025–1033 (2020). 82. 82.Eichenbaum, H. Prefrontal–hippocampal interactions in episodic memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 547–558 (2017). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/nrn.2017.74&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=28655882&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 83. 83.Price, J. L. & Drevets, W. C. Neurocircuitry of mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacol. 35, 192–216 (2010). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/npp.2009.104&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19693001&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000272784600011&link_type=ISI) 84. 84.Northoff, G. et al. Self-referential processing in our brain - A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31, 440–457 (2006). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16466680&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000238012200043&link_type=ISI) 85. 85.Overwalle, F. V. Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 30, 829–858 (2009). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/hbm.20547&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18381770&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000264015900013&link_type=ISI) 86. 86.Benoit, R. G., Gilbert, S. J., Volle, E. & Burgess, P. W. When I think about me and simulate you: medial rostral prefrontal cortex and self-referential processes. Neuroimage 50, 1340–1349 (2010). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.091&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20045478&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000275408200049&link_type=ISI) 87. 87.Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N. & Banaji, M. R. Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron 50, 655–663 (2006). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.040&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16701214&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000237875200015&link_type=ISI) 88. 88.Rameson, L. T., Satpute, A. B. & Lieberman, M. D. The neural correlates of implicit and explicit self-relevant processing. Neuroimage 50, 701–708 (2010). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.098&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20045472&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000274948400038&link_type=ISI) 89. 89.Feinberg, D. A. et al. Multiplexed echo planar imaging for sub-second whole brain fMRI and fast diffusion imaging. PLoS ONE 5, e15710 (2010). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1371/journal.pone.0015710&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=21187930&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 90. 90.Moeller, S. et al. Multiband multislice GE-EPI at 7 tesla, with 16-fold acceleration using partial parallel imaging with application to high spatial and temporal whole-brain fMRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 63, 1144–1153 (2010). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/mrm.22361&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20432285&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 91. 91.Andersson, J. L. R., Skare, S. & Ashburner, J. How to correct susceptibility distortions in spin-echo echo-planar images: application to diffusion tensor imaging. Neuroimage 20, 870–888 (2003). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00336-7&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=14568458&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000186260700020&link_type=ISI) 92. 92.Smith, S. M. et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23, 208–219 (2004). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051&link_type=DOI) 93. 93.Friston, K. J. et al. Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited. Neuroimage 2, 45–53 (1995). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1006/nimg.1995.1007&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9343589&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1995QY54000006&link_type=ISI) 94. 94.Alink, A., Walther, A., Krugliak, A., Bosch, J. J. F. v. d. & Kriegeskorte, N. Mind the drift-improving sensitivity to fMRI pattern information by accounting for temporal pattern drift. bioRxiv 032391 (2015). URL [https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/032391v2](https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/032391v2). 95. 95.Ashburner, J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113 (2007). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17761438&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) [Web of Science](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000249992900011&link_type=ISI) 96. 96.Liu, Z.-X., Grady, C. & Moscovitch, M. Effects of prior-knowledge on brain activation and connectivity during associative memory encoding. Cereb. Cortex 27, 1991–2009 (2017). [CrossRef](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/cercor/bhw047&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://biorxiv.org/lookup/external-ref?access_num=26941384&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fbiorxiv%2Fearly%2F2020%2F08%2F21%2F2020.08.21.260950.atom) 97. 97.Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Soft. 67(2015).