Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

An Agent-based model of the gradual emergence of modern linguistic complexity

View ORCID ProfileMarcel Ruland, View ORCID ProfileAlejandro Andirkó, Iza Romanowska, View ORCID ProfileCedric Boeckx
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.11.12.380683
Marcel Ruland
aUniversitat de Barcelona
bUniversität Osnabrück
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Marcel Ruland
Alejandro Andirkó
aUniversitat de Barcelona
cUniversity of Barcelona Institute of Complex Systems (UBICS)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Alejandro Andirkó
Iza Romanowska
dAarhus Institute of Advanced Studies (AIAS)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Cedric Boeckx
aUniversitat de Barcelona
cUniversity of Barcelona Institute of Complex Systems (UBICS)
eCatalan Institute for Research and Advanced Studies (ICREA)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Cedric Boeckx
  • For correspondence: cedric.boeckx@ub.edu
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Data/Code
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

A central question in the evolution of human language is whether it emerged as a result of one specific event or from a mosaic-like constellation of different phenomena and their interactions. Three potential processes have been identified by recent research as the potential primum mobile for the origins of modern linguistic complexity: Self-domestication, characterized by a reduction in reactive aggression and often associated with a gracilization of the face; changes in early brain development manifested by globularization of the skull; and demographic expansion of H. sapiens during the Middle Pleistocene. We developed an agent-based model to investigate how these three factors influence transmission of information within a population. Our model shows that there is an optimal degree of both hostility and mental capacity at which the amount of transmitted information is the largest. It also shows that linguistic communities formed within the population are strongest under circumstances where individuals have high levels of cognitive capacity available for information processing and there is at least a certain degree of hostility present. In contrast, we find no significant effects related to population size.

Background

By any metric, the modern human mental capacity for language—our ability to construct grammars, accumulate vast vocabularies and flexibly deploy them in relaying meaning—generates complex outputs that go far beyond what other animal communication systems display. For a long time linguists, archaeologists and researchers in adjacent disciplines looked for a single element or event that could have triggered the emergence of the modern human capacity for language (Klein, 2009; Berwick and Chomsky, 2016), taken as the signature of ‘cognitive modernity’. Despite its appeal, this view of a single cause appears to be increasingly implausible (Martins and Boeckx, 2019; De Boer et al., 2020) and a wide range of factors are thought to have shaped our language abilities, ranging from the biological to the cultural and the ecological (Tallerman and Gibson, 2012), more in line with the mosaic nature of the evolutionary trajectory of our species (Scerri et al., 2018).

Understanding the interdependence of multiple factors and revealing their particular roles in shaping the complexity of linguistic features remains a challenge, requiring robust modelling tools to untangle the complex interactions. Here we use an agent-based model to explore for the first time how a combination of prosocial behaviors (here examined from the perspective of ‘self-domestication’), brain development (reflected in the ‘globularization’ of modern skulls) and demographic transitions could have created the context that gave rise to modern linguistic complexity.

The evolution of Homo sapiens before the last major out of Africa event around 60 kya is the product of a complex demographic, cultural, environmental and genetic heritage that is not yet fully understood (Scerri et al., 2018). However, the fossil record allows us to identify at least two major transition events that differentiate the anatomy of Homo sapiens (Stringer, 2016), relative to their extinct cousins: (i) gracilization/self-domestication: an overall retraction of the face (Lacruz et al., 2019) and (ii) globuralization: a change in brain growth trajectory, leading to an expansion of specific brain regions and giving rise to a more globular cranium (Hublin et al., 2015).

The retraction of the face has been linked to a reduced migration rate and production of neural crest cells (Zanella et al., 2019), features also found in domesticated animals (Wilkins et al., 2014). This major anatomical change has been argued to be the result of a (self-)domestication process (Hare and Woods, 2020), and is accompanied by morphological and behavioral consequences, chiefly a reduction in reactive aggression (Wrangham, 2019) observed across several animal species, such as dogs (Canis lupus familiaris), cattle (Bos taurus), pigs (Sus domesticus), foxes (Vulpes vulpes), among others.

It has been independently demonstrated in the Bengalese Finches (Lonchura striata domestica), the domesticated variant of the vocal learning White-rumped Munia (Lonchura striata) (Okanoya, 2004), that domestication impacts learned behaviors and communicative outputs (see also for data on marmosets reaching a similar conclusion). It is therefore plausible to hypothesize that facial gracilization (self-domestication) enhanced human communicative complexity by boosting prosocial behavior.

The second major anatomical change, globularization, refers to the early period of brain development where the growth of the brain gives rise to the distinctive globular aspect of the skull. This developmental phase, during which “the frontal area becomes taller, the parietal areas bulge, the side walls become parallel, and the occipital area becomes rounder and less overhanging” (Neubauer et al., 2018), is now understood to be the result of a differential growth of brain regions such as the cerebellum, the precuneus and parts of the temporal lobe, and the basal ganglia (Pereira-Pedro et al., 2020). These areas are likely to be associated with modifications of specific memory circuits implicated in handling complex mental sequences, which we find in language (Boeckx, 2017; Gunz et al., 2019; Wynn and Coolidge, 2011).

These two anatomical transitions did not take place at the same time: while the modern face has very ancient roots, with some gracilization traits reaching back to Homo antecessor around 1 Mya (Lacruz et al., 2019; Welker et al., 2020; Lacruz et al., 2019), the globularization of the neurocranium appears to be a much more recent event, emerging only in the last 100 kya (Neubauer et al., 2018). For example, the earliest Homo sapiens individual, the fossil from Jebel Irhoud (Morocco) dated ca 300 kya, has a distinctive modern face but still exhibits an archaic, elongated braincase (Hublin et al., 2017).

Lastly, demography has been repeatedly argued to be critical for understanding the origins of language. In particular, it has been claimed that small populations of speakers exhibit less systematicity (Raviv et al., 2019; Wray and Grace, 2007; Lupyan and Dale, 2010). Similarly, the complexity of material culture has been claimed to be affected by group size (Powell et al., 2009b; Derex et al., 2013; Muthukrishna et al., 2014), with smaller groups struggling to develop and maintain cultural complexity, in part due to drift (but see (Vaesen et al., 2016)).

Here we present a formal model of how these three factors—self-domestication, globularization, and demographic expansion—interact with each other to influence the evolutionary trajectory of linguistic complexity. By modeling the evolutionary mechanisms using simulation, we seek to reveal the importance of the ordering and the contribution of each of these transitions to the evolution of our species’ linguistic capacity. Our approach also allows us to gain insight into the formation of linguistic communities, understood as population clusters with shared vocabularies. Agentbased modelling is a particularly well-suited simulation technique thanks to its ability to integrate individual behaviour leading to emergent outcomes at a population level—such as individual speakers’ interactions culminating in a population level phenomenon: the complexification of language.

Model description

The model consists of agents characterized by four state variables: hostility, memorysize, knowns, needs. At every time step, agents are arranged in pairs at random and individually decide whether or not they wish to communicate with their partner, based on their hostility, kinship level, and incentive for communication. Successful communication events lead to transmission of skills and to reproduction, where the child inherits the state variable values of one of the parents chosen at random. Agents live for 30 time steps. A simulation runs for 500 time steps. Output measurements are taken after the last time step has been executed.

Agents are organized in a three-level hierarchical group structure, where every intimate group (5 agents) is a subset of an effective group (25 agents), which is in turn a subset of an extended group (250 agents) (Gamble, 1998). If any group exceeds its maximum size, a new group of the same level is created with half of the agents assigned to it.

At the heart of the model is the agents’ ability to know and exchange skills. Skills are represented by integers. Every agent has a list of active skills (known) and desirable skills (needs) which it wishes (needs) to learn. The known variable is initialized in such a way that two agents share a fraction of their skills proportional to their kinship proximity, i.e. agents’ skillsets within an intimate group will resemble each other more than those within the same extended, but different intimate group. We consider these known skills to be an agent’s active vocabulary. The needs variable starts empty, increasing by 2 randomly drawn skills at every time step. The maximum length of both the knowns and needs variables are determined by an agent’s memorysize.

For communication to happen, both members of a pair (A and B) independently calculate their individual cost and incentive for communication.

The cost function takes into account the agents’ group relation to its partner as well as its level of hostility. Communication with a distantly related partner (e.g. extended group) is more expensive than with a closely related partner (e.g. intimate group). The extent to which cost increases as a function of group affiliation is proportional to the agent’s hostility. Agents with low hostility will increase their communication cost more moderately than those with high hostility when asked to communicate with a distantly related partner.

The incentive function takes into account the percentage of needed skills that the agent can learn from its partner (with a high percentage leading to a high incentive) as well as the number of time steps that have passed since the agent last learned anything (with a longer time increasing the incentive).

The output of the incentive function is subsequently increased or decreased by 2 % depending on whether the partner’s memory size is larger or smaller than the population mean.

If incentive outweighs cost for both agents, then they will communicate. A learns all its needs skills that are present in B’s known variable and vice versa. The length of these transferred signals is one of our key output metrics: signal length. A successful communication event produces a child which inherits one of the parents state variable values, chosen at random. In children, the inherited hostility and memorysize variable values are subject to random mutation, increasing or decreasing by at most 1%. The flow diagram in Fig. 1 provides a visual representation of what happens once two agents have paired up.

Figure 1:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 1:

Diagram of communication in the model with example values.

Four global parameters were varied to test a range of scenarios (table 1). populationsize is the maximum number of agents that exist at any given time. If the agent population exceeds the parameter value, then the oldest agents are removed until the value is no longer exceeded. hostility is the mean of the normal distribution from which agents’ initial hostility is drawn during initialization (std = 0.1). memorysize works equivalently (std = 1). numskills is the global number of distinct skills that exist in the simulation.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1:

Tested parameter ranges.

Results

Across all runs and parameter configurations, the model reliably showed a significant (p < 0.001) decrease in mean hostility across the population, at a rate of approximately 0.1 per 500 time steps.

Population growth exceeded death rates under virtually all circumstances. The notable exception, however, was when memorysize was low (approx. < 17) causing population growth to stagnate at low values. This is shown in Fig. 2, where population size is very small for low values of memorysize, but then suddenly increases, showing a clear exponential relation. With the tested values for memorysize being [1, 21, 41, 61, 81] (table 1), only simulations with the lowest value [1] for memorysize were affected by this relation. In all other cases, population growth hit the ceiling imposed by the populationsize parameter. We recognized several recurring patterns across the tested scenarios related to (i) the signal length and (ii) the emergence of linguistic communities.

Figure 2:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 2:

Exponential growth effects of population size in relation to memory size.

Signal length

An individual signal’s length is defined by the number of skills it contains. One of our key metrics is the mean length of all signals transmitted in the population (measured after the last time step). As a short hand, we refer to this mean value simply as signal length. Thus, signal length measures the mean quantity of information shared in individual communication acts across the population. This metric is particularly influenced by both population size and hostility.

Population size influences variation in signal length. As shown in Fig. 3, the variation is high for small populations but, as population size increases, signal length reliably converges until it can almost be considered a constant.

Figure 3:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 3:

Each data point represents the means of 100 runs with the given population size. Small populations have strong variation in the amount of information they transfer. As population size grows, this variation goes down to virtually zero.

Equally significant is the influence of hostility on signal length, which shows a clear peak of maximum signal length for hostility values around 0.7, above and below which signal length drops (Fig. 4). This result reveals an interesting mechanism arising from the nonlinear interactions between agents.

Figure 4:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 4:

Effects of hostility and memory size on signal length. Both show a clear maximum above and below which signal length decreases.

If hostility is high, the cost of communication outside one’s own intimate group will be high, almost always outweighing the incentive. This leads to communication only happening within, then somewhat isolated, intimate groups. But because agents within the same intimate group also share a majority of their skills, many skills will simply not be present within a given intimate group and communication therefore is limited to short signals. If hostility is low, then in most cases the cost of communication will remain lower than the incentive, leading to frequent communication events. This, in turn, means that agents get to learn all needed skills almost instantaneously and the number of their needs is never large enough to translate into events where long signals can be transmitted.

It must be noted that the position of the maximum, here at 0.75, shifts depending on the values of model parameters. Accordingly, we do not regard the precise position of the maximum to be of great importance, but instead the existence and shape of the maximum itself.

Plotting memorysize against signal length shows a curve similar to the one from hostility (Fig. 4). Again, the existence of optimal conditions for memorysize can be understood through the interactions of different processes. Below the maximum, memorysize acts as an upper bound on the length of agents’ needs, which in turn acts as an upper bound on signal length. Above the maximum, agents will have the majority of skills already stored in their known variable and therefore have no need to learn them, which decreases the amount of skills learned on average and, in turn, decreases signal length.

Linguistic communities

Our model reveals circumstances where linguistic communities of varying sizes and strengths form. For this analysis, we use the agents’ known variable as a proxy for their individual vocabulary. We applied hierarchical clustering (UPGMA) to the agents, using as our distance metric the edit (Levenshtein) distance between the agents’ (sorted) vocabularies. We define a strongly clustered population (i.e. a population exhibiting linguistic communities) as a population in which there are multiple subsets of agents who share similar vocabulary (low linguistic distance between group members) but the vocabulary in between these subsets differs significantly (high linguistic distance). Such a population will result in a dendrogram similar to Fig. 5b, indicating a clear, distinct jump in linguistic distance between subclusters.

Figure 5:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 5:

Top: We measure the formation of linguistic communities by flattening a normalized dendrogram onto a line and taking the length of the longest segment on this line. Weak linguistic communities have no sudden jumps in vocabulary distance, leading to the longest segment being small; strong linguistic communities do have sudden jumps in vocabulary distance, leading to the longest segment being large. Bottom: Parameter values leading to formation of linguistic communities. Data points are bins of size 0.02; all parameters were normalized.

By contrast, we take as weakly clustered a population in which the vocabularies of any two agents have roughly the same edit distance. Such a population will result in a dendrogram similar to Fig. 5a, with gradually increasing distances between subclusters and no sudden jumps. We therefore use the length of the longest segment in a dendrogram, as described in Fig. 5, as a metric indicating how strong the linguistic communities in a population are.

The plot in Fig. 5c plots the strength of linguistic communities against model parameters. The results reveal that linguistic communities are strongest under conditions where the memory size is high but the number of skills is low. In other words, it is under conditions where any individual could easily acquire the entire vocabulary in terms of mental capacity, that we observe the strongest formation of linguistic communities, the strongest competition for vocabulary.

Unsurprisingly, low hostility—which facilitates communication across distant social groups—is related to well mixed populations with no distinct linguistic groups. Increasing hostility translates into stronger linguistic groups only up to a point, after which a further increase shows little impact. This is likely to be directly linked to the same process that causes the global maximum of signal length described before. In contrast, population size has no influence on the strength of linguistic groups.

In addition, we calculated the correlation between an agent pair’s vocabulary similarity and its group similarity across the parameter space. The higher this correlation is, the more does a close group relation coincide with having similar vocabulary. The results of a linear regression analysis, summarized in table 2, show that memorysize and numskills explain by far the largest part of the variation. In between those two parameters, numskills not only has the highest R2-value, but also a coefficient almost twice as high as that of memorysize (0.0043 vs 0.0023). By contrast, both hostility and popsize explain little variation, and the latter also shows a lower significance level. These results converge with the hierarchical clustering described previously, indicating that both the formation of linguistic groups as well as their co-occurrence with social groups is chiefly driven by changes in mental capacity at the level of the individual, and much less so by demographic changes at the population level.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2:

Results of linear regression.

To summarize, the results identify a narrow band of conditions in which the average signal length can increase— a moderate to high level of hostility and moderate range of vocabulary capacity (memorysize)—indicating that linguistic expansion could only occur after the processes of self-domestication and globularization were completed. In addition, they suggest a strong effect of memory size on population growth. Finally, the origins of distinctive linguistic communities are also associated with specific circumstances, namely a high vocabulary capacity (memorysize) coupled with lower number of individual units present in the language (numskills).

Discussion

Based on the archaeological record and findings in related disciplines researchers have highlighted three important processes in the evolutionary trajectory of our species: self-domestication, globularization and demographic expansion. Each of these three events is incorporated into the model. The agents’ hostility (or lack thereof) serves as a proxy for self-domestication since lower aggression towards strangers is its most characteristic behavioral phenotype (Wrangham, 2019). The increase of agents’ memory size serves as a proxy for globularization (Boeckx, 2017), while an increase in population size is the equivalent to demographic expansion. Through these three proxies, we modelled the underlying processes in as simple as possible terms to investigate the interactions between them and their impact on the evolution of modern linguistic complexity. We do not claim that these proxies capture all the details and aspects of the transitions that took place, but even this simple model reveals how the various factors we modelled conspire to boost linguistic complexity in a highly non-linear and complex way.

All together the model demonstrates the causal chain effect of self-domestication on the evolution of modern linguistic complexity and a temporal ordering of selfdomestication causing globularization, which in turn triggers demographic expansion (Fig. 6). A decrease in hostility over time is a robust pattern across all tested scenarios. Decreasing hostility eventually facilitates communication and leads to an increase in signal length. The necessity of higher memory size to transmit longer signals makes it plausible that evolutionary pressure for longer signal transmission manifests itself as pressure for higher memory size and as a result - increased cognitive capacity.

Figure 6:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 6:

Our model results are compatible with the archaeological record, suggesting a temporal order of the three main transitional events.

As seen in Fig. 2, an increase in memory size correlates with exponential population growth (demographic expansion), completing the sequence of events. Crucially, each parameter in the model was shown to impact distinct aspects of the phenotype, strongly suggesting that a full understanding of linguistic complexity requires taking into account several factors and mechanisms, each impacting the evolutionary process in a specific way and all of them interacting with each other.

The model’s results and in particular the temporal ordering of the processes are highly compatible with the evolutionary timeline that can be inferred from the fossil record. The process of face retraction is well documented already in early hominin lineages, together with an increase in pro-social behaviour - a trend that continues throughout human evolution. Globularization on the other hand is associated with much more recent human past, ca. 350,000-35,000 years ago (Neubauer et al., 2018; Hodgson, 2013). Finally, the end point of the process of endocranial globularisation correlates with the most dramatic demographic expansion of our species approximately 45,000-35,000 years ago (Powell et al., 2009a). The same temporal order of the three processes emerges from our model suggesting that the timing and sequence of these events is not coincidental and, although previously regarded as independent processes, they are in fact closely intertwined.

The emergence of linguistic communities is of wide academic interest. Prior to the use of DNA as the basis of the human phylogenetic tree, linguistic affinity was a primary source of models concerned with the longterm trajectories of human groups (Cavalli-Sforza et al., 1988). In both cases, their association with particular archaeologically or historically identified communities has been a source of much debate (Gimbutas, 1997; Gray and Atkinson, 2003; Haak et al., 2015). The origins of the formation of such distinctive socio-cultural-linguistic units is therefore of great relevance. The diversification into regional “traditions” during the European Upper Palaeolithic has been hailed as one of the landmarks of modern human culture (Gamble, 1998). Our model allows us to gain insight into the conditions that could lead to the development and maintenance of such cultural traditions.

The combination between large language capacity (high memorysize) combined with small number of modular language units (low numskills) has been suggested as a critical feature of modern human language (Nowak and Krakauer, 1999). Our model indicates that the existence of distinctive language communities constitutes a good proxy for high language complexity.

Supplementary information

All data, including all code and raw model output data, have been made available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 license at http://www.doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4270200.

Footnotes

  • http://www.doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4270200

References

  1. ↵
    Berwick, Robert C. and Chomsky, Noam (2016): Why only us: Language and evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  2. ↵
    Boeckx, Cedric (2017): “The language-ready head: Evolutionary considerations.” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 24 (1): 194–199.
    OpenUrl
  3. ↵
    Cavalli-Sforza, Luigi Luca; Piazza, Alberto; Menozzi, Paolo; and Mountain, Joanna (1988): “Reconstruction of human evolution: bringing together genetic, archaeological, and linguistic data.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 85 (16): 6002–6006.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    De Boer, Bart; Thompson, Bill; Ravignani, Andrea; and Boeckx, Cedric (2020): “Evolutionary dynamics do not motivate a single-mutant theory of human language.” Scientific reports 10 (1): 1–9.
    OpenUrl
  5. ↵
    Derex, Maxime; Beugin, Marie-Pauline; Godelle, Bernard; and Raymond, Michel (2013): “Experimental evidence for the influence of group size on cultural complexity.” Nature 503 (7476): 389–391. URL: http://www.nature.com/articles/nature12774
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  6. ↵
    Gamble, Clive (1998): “Palaeolithic society and the release from proximity: A network approach to intimate relations.” World archaeology 29 (3): 426–449.
    OpenUrlWeb of Science
  7. ↵
    Gimbutas, Marija (1997): The Kurgan Culture and the Indo-Europeanization of Europe: Selected Articles From 1952 to 1993. Journal of Indo-European Studies Monograph Series No. 18. Institute for the Study of Man.
  8. ↵
    Gray, Russell D. and Atkinson, Quentin D. (2003): “Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo-European origin.” Nature 426 (6965): 435–439.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  9. ↵
    Gunz, Philipp; Tilot, Amanda K; Wittfeld, Katharina; Teumer, Alexander; Shapland, Chin Yang; Van Erp, Theo GM; Dannemann, Michael; Vernot, Benjamin; Neubauer, Simon; Guadalupe, Tulio et al. (2019): “Ne-andertal introgression sheds light on modern human endocranial globularity.” Current Biology 29 (1): 120–127.
    OpenUrl
  10. ↵
    Haak, Wolfgang; Lazaridis, Iosif; Patterson, Nick; Rohland, Nadin; Mallick, Swapan; Llamas, Bastien; Brandt, Guido; Nordenfelt, Susanne; Harney, Eadaoin; Stewardson, Kristin et al. (2015): “Massive migration from the steppe was a source for Indo-European languages in Europe.” Nature 522 (7555): 207–211.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Hare, Brian and woods, Vanesa (2020): Survival of the Friendliest: Understanding our origins and rediscovering our common humanity. Penguin Random House.
  12. ↵
    hodgson, Derek (2013): “Cognitive evolution, population, transmission, and material culture.” Biological Theory 7 (3): 237–246.
    OpenUrl
  13. ↵
    Hublin, Jean-Jacques; Ben-Ncer, Abdelouahed; Bailey, Shara E; Freidline, Sarah E; Neubauer, Simon; Skinner, Matthew M; Bergmann, Inga; Le Cabec, Adeline; Benazzi, Stefano; Harvati, Katerina et al. (2017): “New fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco and the pan-African origin of Homo sapiens.” Nature 546 (7657): 289–292.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    Hublin, Jean-Jacques; Neubauer, Simon; and Gunz, Philipp (2015): “Brain ontogeny and life history in Pleistocene hominins.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 370 (1663): 20140062.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Klein, Richard G. (2009): The human career: Human biological and cultural origins. University of Chicago Press.
  16. ↵
    Lacruz, Rodrigo S; Stringer, Chris B; Kimbel, William H; wood, Bernard; Harvati, Katerina; O’Higgins, Paul; Bromage, Timothy G; and Arsuaga, Juan-Luis (2019): “The evolutionary history of the human face.” Nature ecology & evolution 3 (5): 726–736.
    OpenUrl
  17. ↵
    Lupyan, Gary and Dale, Rick (2010): “Language Structure Is Partly Determined by Social Structure.” PLOS ONE 5 (1): e8559. URL: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0008559
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    Martins, Pedro Tiago and Boeckx, Cedric (2019): “Language evolution and complexity considerations: The no half-Merge fallacy.” PLoS biology 17 (11): e3000389.
    OpenUrl
  19. ↵
    Muthukrishna, Michael; Shulman, Ben W.; Vasilescu, Vlad; and Henrich, Joseph (2014): “Sociality influences cultural complexity.” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281 (1774): 20132511. URL: https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rspb.2013.2511s
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Neubauer, Simon; Hublin, Jean-Jacques; and Gunz, Philipp (2018): “The evolution of modern human brain shape.” Science advances 4 (1): eaao5961.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    Nowak, Martin A and Krakauer, David C (1999): “The evolution of language.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 96 (14): 8028–8033.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    Okanoya, Kazuo (2004): “The Bengalese Finch: A window on the behavioral neurobiology of birdsong syntax.” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1016 (1): 724–735.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  23. ↵
    Pereira-Pedro, Ana Sofia; Bruner, Emiliano; Gunz, Philipp; and Neubauer, Simon (2020): “A morphometric comparison of the parietal lobe in modern humans and Neanderthals.” Journal of Human Evolution 142: 102770. URL: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0047248420300312
    OpenUrl
  24. ↵
    Powell, A; Shennan, S; and Thomas, M (2009a): “Demography, Skill Accumulation, and the Origins of Behavioural Modernity.” Science 324: 1298–1301.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Powell, A.; Shennan, S.; and Thomas, M. G. (2009b): “Late Pleistocene Demography and the Appearance of Modern Human Behavior.” Science 324 (5932): 1298–1301. URL: https://www.sciencemag.org/lookup/doi/10.1126/science.1170165
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    Raviv, Limor; Meyer, Antje; and Lev-Ari, Shiri (2019): “Larger communities create more systematic languages.” Proceedings of the Royal Society B 286 (1907): 20191262.
    OpenUrl
  27. ↵
    Scerri, Eleanor M.L.; Thomas, Mark G.; Manica, Andrea; Gunz, Philipp; Stock, Jay T.; Stringer, Chris; Grove, Matt; Groucutt, Huw S.; Timmermann, Axel; Rightmire, G. Philip; d’Errico, Francesco; Tryon, Christian A.; Drake, Nick A.; Brooks, Alison S.; Dennell, Robin W.; Durbin, Richard; Henn, Brenna M.; Lee-Thorp, Julia; deMenocal, Peter; Petraglia, Michael D.; Thompson, Jessica C.; Scally, Aylwyn; and Chikhi, Lounès (2018): “Did our species evolve in subdivided populations across Africa, and why does it matter?” Trends in Ecology & Evolution 33 (8): 582–594.
    OpenUrl
  28. ↵
    Stringer, Chris (2016): “The origin and evolution of Homo sapiens.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371 (1698): 20150237.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Tallerman, Maggie and Gibson, Kathleen R. (2012): The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford University Press.
  30. ↵
    Vaesen, Krist; Collard, Mark; Cosgrove, Richard; and Roebroeks, Wil (2016): “Population size does not explain past changes in cultural complexity.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113 (16): E2241–E2247.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    Welker, Frido; Ramos-Madrigal, Jazmín; Gutenbrunner, Petra; Mackie, Meaghan; Tiwary, Shivani; Jersie-Christensen, Rosa Rakownikow; Chiva, Cristina; Dickinson, Marc R; Kuhlwilm, Martin; de Manuel, Marc et al. (2020): “The dental proteome of Homo antecessor.” Nature 1–4.
  32. ↵
    Wilkins, Adam S.; Wrangham, Richard W.; and Fitch, W. Tecumseh (2014): “The “Domestication Syndrome” in mammals: A unified explanation based on neural crest cell behavior and genetics.” Genetics 197 (3): 795–808.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    Wrangham, Richard (2019): The Goodness Paradox: The Strange Relationship Between Virtue and Violence in Human Evolution. Pantheon.
  34. ↵
    Wray, Alison and Grace, George W. (2007): “The consequences of talking to strangers: Evolutionary corollaries of socio-cultural influences on linguistic form.” Lingua 117 (3): 543–578.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  35. ↵
    Wynn, Thomas and Coolidge, Frederick L. (2011): “The implications of the working memory model for the evolution of modern cognition.” International Journal of Evolutionary Biology 2011.
  36. ↵
    Zanella, Matteo; Vitriolo, Alessandro; Andirkó, Alejandro; Martins, Pedro T; Sturm, Stefanie; O’Rourke, Thomas; Laugsch, Magdalena; Malerba, Natascia; Skaros, Adrianos; Trattaro, Sebastiano; Germain, Pierre-Luc; Mihailovic, Marija; Merla, Giuseppe; Rada-Iglesias, Alvaro; Boeckx, Cedric; and Testa, Giuseppe (2019): “Dosage analysis of the 7q11.23 Williams region identifies BAZ1B as a major human gene patterning the modern human face and underlying selfdomestication.” Science Advances 5 (12): eaaw7908.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted November 15, 2020.
Download PDF
Data/Code
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
An Agent-based model of the gradual emergence of modern linguistic complexity
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
An Agent-based model of the gradual emergence of modern linguistic complexity
Marcel Ruland, Alejandro Andirkó, Iza Romanowska, Cedric Boeckx
bioRxiv 2020.11.12.380683; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.11.12.380683
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
An Agent-based model of the gradual emergence of modern linguistic complexity
Marcel Ruland, Alejandro Andirkó, Iza Romanowska, Cedric Boeckx
bioRxiv 2020.11.12.380683; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.11.12.380683

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Animal Behavior and Cognition
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (2536)
  • Biochemistry (4983)
  • Bioengineering (3487)
  • Bioinformatics (15242)
  • Biophysics (6914)
  • Cancer Biology (5404)
  • Cell Biology (7756)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (4543)
  • Ecology (7162)
  • Epidemiology (2059)
  • Evolutionary Biology (10240)
  • Genetics (7522)
  • Genomics (9803)
  • Immunology (4869)
  • Microbiology (13250)
  • Molecular Biology (5151)
  • Neuroscience (29496)
  • Paleontology (203)
  • Pathology (838)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (1468)
  • Physiology (2143)
  • Plant Biology (4759)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (1013)
  • Synthetic Biology (1339)
  • Systems Biology (4015)
  • Zoology (770)