Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

A preliminary assessment of allostatic load in male mantled howler monkeys

View ORCID ProfilePedro Américo D. Dias, Alejandro Coyohua-Fuentes, David Roberto Chavira-Ramírez, Domingo Canales-Espinosa, View ORCID ProfileAriadna Rangel-Negrín
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.22.432350
Pedro Américo D. Dias
1Primate Behavioral Ecology Lab, Instituto de Neuroetología, Universidad Veracruzana, 91190, Xalapa, Mexico
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Pedro Américo D. Dias
  • For correspondence: pdias@uv.mx
Alejandro Coyohua-Fuentes
1Primate Behavioral Ecology Lab, Instituto de Neuroetología, Universidad Veracruzana, 91190, Xalapa, Mexico
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David Roberto Chavira-Ramírez
2Departamento de Biología de la Reproducción, Instituto Nacional de Ciencias Médicas y Nutrición Salvador Zubirán, 14080, Ciudad de México, Mexico
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Domingo Canales-Espinosa
1Primate Behavioral Ecology Lab, Instituto de Neuroetología, Universidad Veracruzana, 91190, Xalapa, Mexico
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ariadna Rangel-Negrín
1Primate Behavioral Ecology Lab, Instituto de Neuroetología, Universidad Veracruzana, 91190, Xalapa, Mexico
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Ariadna Rangel-Negrín
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

Allostatic load refers to the wear and tear of the organism associated with the repeated activation of mechanisms that allow for homeostasis. Although allostasis (i.e., maintaining stability through change) is achieved through the orchestration of multiple systems, assessments of allostatic load in wildlife have usually relied on the measurement of single or few physiological mediators. In the present study, we aimed at constructing an allostatic load index (ALI) for male mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) based on seven physiological mediators (C-peptide, triiodothyronine, glucocorticoids, testosterone, amount of activity, foraging effort, and participation in agonistic interactions). We then examined if variation in this ALI was associated with social, ecological, and abiotic challenges. For five years, we studied ten adult males belonging to two groups. We assessed physiological mediators via behavioral observations (3,364 hours of samplings), and both urine (1,500 assayed samples) and fecal (1,500 assayed samples) sampling. We calculated an ALI based on mediator specific risk scores. Seasonality, encounters with extragroup males, mating, and centrality were positively related with male allostatic load, though only the first two had significant effects on ALI. ALI reflected the physiological and behavioral responses of males to social, ecological, and abiotic challenges. It remains for future research to determine if the observed trends correspond to normal fluctuations in mechanisms that allow male mantled howler monkeys to cope with their environment.

Introduction

Allostatic processes support homeostasis by achieving stability through change (McEwen & Wingfield, 2003). Adjustments in hormones of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA) and catecholamines, for instance, result in a cascade of physiological and behavioral events that favor homeostasis in the face of stressful situations (Sapolsky, Romero & Munck, 2000). Therefore, allostasis allows an organism to cope with life history and environmental challenges, both predictable and unpredictable (Landys, Ramenofsky & Wingfield 2006; Romero & Wingfield, 2016). However, the repeated activation of allostatic processes entails costs in terms of wear and tear on the organism (i.e., allostatic load: McEwen & Stellar, 1993), which do not necessarily lead to pathology (e.g., Boonstra, 2013). However, it does affect the likelihood of developing pathologies, as well as the timing of their expression (McEwen, 2007). Understanding the determinants of allostatic load may therefore assist in predicting individual performance in terms of survival and reproduction (Bonier, Moore, Martin & Robertson, 2009; Breuner, Patterson & Hahn, 2008; Romero, 2012).

Allostatic load may be determined through the assessment of the multiple systems involved in allostasis. The actions of immune, cardiovascular, or central nervous systems may be assessed via several physiological mediators (sensu Romero & Wingfield, 2016), such as prostaglandins, heart rate, and neurotransmitter concentrations, respectively (Romero & Wingfield, 2016). With a few notable exceptions (e.g., Edes, Wolfe & Crews, 2016a, 2016b), such assessments in non-humans have relied on the measurement of single allostatic load physiological mediators. In humans, in contrast, many allostatic load indexes (ALI), in which several physiological mediators are combined in a composite measure, have been constructed. Because they are multisystem measures, ALI reflect somatic dysregulation over time, and have proven to be robust predictors of future morbidity and mortality (Edes & Crews, 2017). The incidence of cardiovascular disease, arthritis, and diabetes, for instance, increase with increasing ALI (e.g., Mattei, Demissie, Falcon, Ordovas & Tucker, 2010; Seeman, Singer, Ryff, Love & Levy-Storms, 2002), and mortality probabilities are also positively correlated with ALI (e.g., Glei, Goldman, Rodríguez & Weinstein, 2014; Levine & Crimmins, 2014), independently of other risk factors, such as age, ethnicity, or income (Seeman, McEwen, Rowe, & Singer, 2001). Given that several physiological processes are evolutionarily conserved (e.g., Cavigelli & Caruso, 2015), it is plausible to assume that ALI could be valuable indicators of allostatic load in animals (Edes & Crews, 2017).

A multisystem assessment of the wear and tear of the organism, such as ALI, could also benefit a better understanding of interspecific variation in the mechanisms of allostasis. In nonhuman primates, for instance, there is abundant evidence that the HPA axis is modulated by a variety of biotic and abiotic challenges, both predictable and unpredictable (Beehner & Bergman, 2017). Therefore, glucocorticoid hormones have been used as an indicator of allostatic load (e.g., MacLarnon et al., 2014). There is, however, considerable interspecific variation in the glucocorticoid responses of individuals to challenges. For example, in some species dominant individuals have consistently higher glucocorticoid levels, whereas in other species the opposite trend is observed (Abbott et al., 2003). In addition, at the intraspecific level, whereas some studies have highlighted the metabolic role of glucocorticoids (e.g., Dunn, Cristóbal-Azkarate, Schulte-Herbrüggen, Chavira, & Veà, 2013), others have found that these hormones are mainly responsive to psychosocial challenges (e.g., Dias, Coyohua-Fuentes, Canales-Espinosa, Chavira-Ramírez & Rangel-Negrín, 2017). Combining glucocorticoid measurements with other physiological mediators, as well as using scale-free measures of allostatic load (McLarnon et al. 2014), could reconcile this variation, and provide an evolutionary perspective about the mechanisms of allostasis.

In the present study, we examine whether an ALI reflects the impact of biotic and abiotic factors on a Neotropical primate species, the mantled howler monkey (Alouatta palliata). Mantled howler monkeys have one of the widest geographical distributions among primates (Mexico to Peru), and can be found in highly diverse ecological conditions (e.g., 0 to 2,000 masl: Baumgarten & Williamson, 2007; annual rainfall of 1,431 to 4,900 mm: Dias & Rangel-Negrín, 2015a). They live in multi-male, multi-female cohesive groups, which vary in size from 2 to 45 individuals (Di Fiore & Campbell, 2011). Although intragroup social interactions are infrequent and subtle (e.g., Wang & Milton, 2003), and extragroup relationships are mainly based on long distance vocal communication (Kitchen, da Cunha, Holzmann & de Oliveira, 2015), both intra and extragroup competition among males is intense, and aggression events (mostly with extragroup males) frequently result in physical injury and death (Cristóbal-Azkarate, Dias & Veà, 2004; Dias, Rangel-Negrín, Veà & Canales-Espinosa, 2010; Ryan, Starks, Milton & Getz, 2008). Dominance relationships among males are non-linear, but some males (central males) typically show priority of access to resources (females and food), are more active in vocal contests with extragroup individuals, and have a spatially central position in groups (Dias et al., 2010; Jones, 1980,1985; Wang & Milton, 2003; but see Corewyn, 2015). Males of this species are therefore exposed to a large array of social, ecological, and abiotic challenges, which should represent major sources of allostatic load.

Our aims were two-fold. First, to construct an ALI for male mantled howler monkeys based on the measurement of several physiological mediators. Second, to analyze if this ALI varied as a function of social, ecological, and abiotic challenges faced by males. Previous research has demonstrated that C-peptide, the thyroid hormone triiodothyronine, glucocorticoids, and testosterone may be non-invasively measured in wild mantled howler monkeys, and are responsive to social, ecological, and abiotic demands (e.g., Cristóbal-Azkarate, Chavira, Boeck, Rodríguez-Luna & Veà, 2007; Cano-Huertes et al., 2017; Dias et al., 2017; Gómez-Espinosa, Rangel-Negrín, Chavira-Ramírez, Canales-Espinosa & Dias, 2014; Thompson et al., 2017). Similarly, the amount of activity, foraging effort, and participation in agonistic interactions vary as a function of those same demands (e.g., group size: Arroyo-Rodríguez, Dias & Cristóbal-Azkarate, 2011; food availability: Dias & Rodríguez-Luna, 2006; Dunn, Cristóbal-Azkarate & Veà, 2010; ambient temperature: Kahrig & Halloran, 2010). Therefore, we constructed an ALI based on these seven physiological mediators, and predicted that allostatic load should increase with increasing investment in social competition (i.e., centrality, participation in contests with extragroup males, and mating), with decreasing food availability, and during cooler periods with less daylight. Because allostatic responses are highly dynamic and respond to multiple individual attributes (Romero & Wingfield, 2016), we also predicted that ALI should vary among years and individuals.

Methods

Ethical note

This study was non-invasive and followed the “Guidelines for the treatment of animals in behavioural research and teaching” (Guidelines for the Use of Animals, 2012). Research protocols were approved by the Secretaria de Medio Ambiente y Recursos Naturales (permits SGPA/DGVS/10637/11 and SGPA/DGVS/04999/14) and adhered to the legal requirements of the Mexican law (NOM-059-SEMARNAT-2010).

Study site and subjects

The study was conducted at La Flor de Catemaco, Veracruz, México (18°26’39” N, 95°02’57” W). The study site is a 250-ha ranch that includes approximately 100 ha of semi-deciduous tropical forest, and its understory is used for cultivating sustainable plantations of parlour palm (Shedden-González & Rodríguez-Luna, 2010). The climate is warm and humid with mean annual temperatures of 25°C and rainfall of 2,600 mm (Soto & Gama, 1997). There is a dry season in the months of February-May and a rainy season from June to January. Part of the rainy season (September to January) is a period of storms (locally known as “nortes”), characterized by strong northerly winds and a considerable fall in ambient temperature (Soto & Gama, 1997).

Mantled howler monkeys living at La Flor de Catemaco have been studied since 2004 (Aguilar-Cucurachi et al., 2010; Cano-Huertes et al., 2017; Dias et al., 2017; Reynoso-Cruz, Rangel-Negrín, Coyohua-Fuentes, Canales-Espinosa & Dias, 2016; Shedden-González & Rodríguez-Luna, 2010). By the end of this study, 29 individuals lived at the site divided into three groups, and four individuals live solitarily. Two groups have been followed daily since 2012. Subjects are fully habituated to the presence of researchers (i.e., ignore our presence: Williamson & Feistner, 2011). We identify individuals through anatomical and physiognomic characteristics, including body size and proportions, scars, broken fingers, and genital morphology and pigmentation. In the current study, we focused on the ten adult males that resided in our two main study groups from 2012 to 2016. These were fully grown males, with ages ranging between 10 and 22 years (ages determined through direct observations of births and inspection of dental condition during capture).

Assessment of climatic, ecological, and abiotic factors

We determined weekly mean ambient temperatures (temperature, hereafter) from daily recordings performed at 1-h intervals with a handheld thermometer (Kestrel 3500 Weather Meter; Nielsen-Kellerman Company, PA, USA). We collected temperature data for 535 fieldwork days.

To assess food availability at this site, each Monday we sampled 397 trees belonging to 59 plant taxa described as food sources for these primates (Cano-Huertes et al., 2017). In each tree, we quantified the presence of young and mature leaves, unripe and ripe fruits, and flowers through a visual estimation of the percentage of stems bearing each phenological phase using the following abundance scores: 0 = none, 0.25 = 1–25%, 0.5 = 26–50%, 0.75 = 51–75%, 1 = 76–100%. Every week we estimated young leaf and fruit (the main food items consumed by mantled howler monkeys: Dias & Rangel-Negrín, 2015a) biomass by multiplying abundance scores of each food item by allometric formulae (described in Cano-Huertes et al., 2017), and we calculated weekly total food biomass as the sum of leaf (young and mature leaves) and fruit biomass (unripe and ripe fruits). We collected a total of 250 weekly phenological samplings

We obtained information on day length (photoperiod, hereafter) during the study period from the U.S. Naval Observatory (http://aa.usno.navy.mil/data). We gathered photoperiod data for 1,700 fieldwork days.

Assessment of behavioral measures

We used focal animal sampling with continuous recording (1-h samples) to study the time budgets of males during complete days of following (i.e., 06:00-07:00 to 17:00-18:00 h, depending on the time of year). We chose all males in a group randomly as focal animals, and resampled males only when we had sampled all of them once. We recorded time budgets using EZrecord for HP iPaq116, and categorized behavioral observations into the following: feeding (inspection of food, bringing food to mouth, chewing and swallowing, moving while feeding within a food patch), resting (sleep or static without interaction), moving (movement to a new area or tree), and socializing (social interactions: Dias & Rangel-Negrín, 2015b). We also recorded all occurrences of participation of males (either as actors or receivers) in agonistic and sexual interactions (Dias & Rangel-Negrín, 2015b), as well as involvement in vocal, visual and physical encounters with extragroup individuals (solitary or from other groups). We collected a mean ± SD of 277.3 ± 76.8 h of focal samples per male (55.5 ± 15.4 h per male per year), and 3,364 h of all-occurrences sampling.

Assessment of physiological measures

We collected fecal and urine samples from males immediately after deposition whenever samples were unequivocally matched to subject identity. We collected fecal samples uncontaminated by urine from the forest floor and deposited them in polyethylene bags labeled with the identity of each individual. We collected fresh urine samples uncontaminated by soil, feces, or urine from other individuals by pipetting urine from a plastic sheet positioned underneath males during urination or directly from vegetation. We deposited urine samples in 5 ml snap-cap polypropylene microtubes. We kept samples in a cooler with frozen gel packs while in the field and stored them at the end of the day in a freezer at −20 °C until processing.

We extracted fecal triiodothyronine, glucocorticoid, and testosterone metabolites (for simplicity, triiodothyronine, glucocorticoid, and testosterone levels hereafter) following a modification of the method described in Wasser et al. (2000) for the extraction of steroids. Briefly, we shook 0.6 g of homogenized, lyophilized and pulverized feces for 20 h in 4.0-ml analytical-grade methanol. We centrifuged extracts (460 g for 30 min) and recovered the supernatant. After complete evaporation of the solvent in a water bath at 60 °C for 20 h, pellets were reconstituted with 3-ml albumin buffer and used for triiodothyronine, glucocorticoid, and testosterone assays.

C-peptide and glucocorticoid levels were determined using a radioimmunoassay (Human C-Peptide, Merck Millipore, Billerica, MA, USA and Izotop, Institute of Isotopes Ltd., Budapest, HU, respectively) and a gamma counter (Cobra Model E5005, Packard, Inc., Downers Grove, IL, USA). We measured triiodothyronine and testosterone with a chemiluminescent enzyme immunoassay commercial kit (Total T3 Immulite, Siemens, CA, USA and Immulite Total Testosterone, Siemens, Los Angeles, CA, USA, respectively) and an Immulite 1000 analyzer (Siemens, CA, USA).

We performed analytical validations for all assays as described in Rangel-Negrín, Flores-Escobar, Chavira, Canales-Espinosa & Dias (2014; Table 1). The protocols that we used to extract and quantify C-peptide levels in urine and both triiodothyronine and glucocorticoid levels in feces have been previously validated to detect insulin and thyroid function, and the activation of the HPA axis, respectively, in mantled howler monkeys (Cano-Huertes et al., 2017, Dias et al., 2017; Gómez-Espinosa et al., 2014). Circulating testosterone levels are higher in male than in female primates (Dixson, 2013). Based on this physiological difference, we compared the testosterone levels of five adult males and five adult females to determine if our assays reflected natural variation in fecal testosterone metabolites. Mean (± SE) testosterone levels in male samples (2,564.8 ± 301.4 ng/g, N = 15) were nearly 370 % higher than those in female samples (571.9 ± 66.8 ng/g, N = 15; Mann-Whitney w = 2, P < 0.001).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1.

Analytical validation of assays performed on hormonal physiological mediators of male mantled howler monkeys’ allostatic load

All samples were run in duplicate. We standardized C-peptide measurements by dividing C-peptide concentrations by creatinine concentrations (measured via enzyme immunoassays with an auto analyzer: Gallery, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA). C-peptide levels are therefore reported as pg/mg of creatinine. Triiodothyronine, glucocorticoids, and testosterone are reported as ng/g of dry feces. We analyzed a total of 1,500 fecal and 1,500 urine samples (30 samples per male per year per sample type).

Data analysis

We analyzed seven physiological mediators that may be informative of male allostatic load: C-peptide, triiodothyronine, glucocorticoids, testosterone, activity, foraging, and agonism (Table 2). We calculated activity as the ratio of time spent feeding, moving, and socializing to time resting per focal sample (Dias et al., 2017), and foraging as the percentage of time spent feeding in each focal sample. We calculated agonism as the frequency of agonistic interactions divided by observation time in each observation day. All physiological mediator values were averaged (mean values) per climatic season (i.e., dry, rainy, and storm seasons) in each year in order to analyze annual variation in allostatic load per individual (see below). Therefore, each male was represented in the dataset by 15 data points (i.e., three climatic seasons x five study years).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2.

Physiological mediators of the allostatic load of male mantled howler monkeys analyzed in this study

Following previous estimations of allostatic load in humans (e.g., Seeman et al., 2001, 2002) and gorillas (Edes & Crews, 2017; Edes, Wolfe & Crews, 2016), we calculated quartiles for each physiological mediator, and considered that individuals faced high risk (i.e., higher allostatic load) when they were in either the highest (triiodothyronine, glucocorticoids, testosterone, activity, foraging, and agonism) or lowest (C-peptide) quartile. For each physiological mediator, males were scored 1 or 0 depending on whether they were in the highest or other quartile, respectively. Therefore, this ALI could vary between 0 and 7, although in each season ALI varied between 0 and 5.

For each season, we calculated mean total food biomass, young leaf biomass, fruit biomass, temperature, photoperiod, and rate of participation in encounters with extragroup males as predictors of ALI (Table 3). Furthermore, we classified the involvement of males in mating and centrality as dichotomous variables (i.e., yes/no, central/non-central, respectively). Centrality was assessed on the basis of proximity to females, mating frequency, and vocal coordination of the group during movements and encounters with extragroup individuals (Van Belle, Estrada & Strier, 2008; Van Belle, Estrada, Ziegler & Strier, 2009; Wang & Milton, 2003).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 3.

Predictors of ALI (allostatic load index) in male mantled howler monkeys analyzed in this study

Temperature, total food biomass, young leaf biomass, fruit biomass, and photoperiod were correlated (Pearson correlations P < 0.05; Fig. 1a). To avoid multicollinearity among predictors in models of male allostatic load, we ran a PCA with these variables. The PCA resulted in two components with eigenvalues ≥ 1 and that explained 95.3% of the variance in the variables ambient temperature, total food biomass, young leaf biomass, fruit biomass, and photoperiod. With the exception of young leaf biomass, all variables had strong positive loadings on the first component (eigenvalue = 3.7, percentage of explained variance = 74.4%; Fig. 1b). Young leaf biomass, had a strong positive loading on the second component (eigenvalue = 1.04, percentage of explained variance = 20.9%). Because ambient temperature, total food biomass, fruit biomass, and photoperiod may be considered as variables that characterize temporal variation in the climatic, ecological, and abiotic environment that males experience, we named component 1 “seasonality”. We transformed this component to its inverse (i.e., multiplied by −1) to convert it to a more intuitive measure (i.e., higher values representing harsher conditions). Therefore, we used as predictors for variation in ALI component 1 (i.e., seasonality), component 2 (young leaf biomass), male centrality, involvement in sexual interactions, and rate of extragroup encounters.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 1.

Correlograms depicting the relationships: A) among predictors of male mantled howler monkeys’ allostatic load index that were correlated; B) among predictors of male mantled howler monkeys’ allostatic load index that were correlated and components originating from a principal components analysis. Color scale (defined on the right) corresponds to variation in Pearson correlation coefficients.

To determine which factors better explained variation in ALI, we used a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM: West, Welch & Galecki, 2014) for a Poisson response variable (and log link function), that accounted for the repeated sampling of males within and between years. Besides the already mentioned climatic, ecological, and abiotic predictors of ALI, we included individual identity, year, and the interaction between identity and year as predictors to determine intra-individual, inter-individual, and annual variation in ALI. We furthermore included the interaction between climatic, ecological, and abiotic predictors and both male identity and year to understand if intra-individual, inter-individual, and annual variation in ALI co-varied with those factors. We used seasons nested within male identity and within year as a random factor to account for the hierarchical nature of our analytical design. This baseline model was run in R (R Core Team, 2017) with the ‘Ime4’ package and function ‘glmer’ (Bates, Maechle, Bolker & Walker, 2017).

We followed an information-theoretic approach (Burnham & Anderson, 2013) to generate a set of models based on Akaike information criteria (AICc), and defined the best model for variation in ALI by averaging parameter estimates of models receiving substantial empirical support (i.e., Δi, < 2). We also calculated the relative importance for each term included in this model set. Importance is the sum of the Akaike weights wi of all models which include the term in question. As model weights represent the probability of a model to be the best model in the model set and thus reflect model uncertainty, importance can be understood as the likelihood of a term to be included in the best model. We used R package ‘MuMln’ (Barton, 2017) for this analysis. We used function ‘glmer’ to obtain the parameters of the averaged best model, and verified the underlying assumptions of the model visually with Q-Q plots of residuals fitted against predicted values.

For significance testing, we started by using a likelihood ratio test to compare a null model including only the random factor with the best model (R package ‘stats’: R Core Team, 2017). We then calculated the significance of each factor included in the best model with function ‘anova’ (‘car’ package: Fox & Weisberg, 2017), and confidence intervals with function ‘confint’ (package ‘stats’: R Core Team, 2017). Finally, we used function ‘r2beta’ to calculate the predictive value of the best model (i.e., pseudo R2: package ‘r2glmm’: Jaeger, 2017).

Results

Description of variation in physiological mediators

In general, all physiological mediators varied notably throughout the study, showing no clear annual or seasonal patterns (Fig. 2). Within- and between-season variation was also notable for some physiological mediators, such as triiodothyronine, glucocorticoids, and foraging, reaching 230, 431, and 643% of variation, respectively. Several physiological mediators were highly correlated, except for testosterone, which was in general poorly correlated with other variables (Fig. 3).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 2.

Temporal variation in male mantled howler monkeys’ physiological mediators: A) C-peptide; B) triiodothyronine; C) glucocorticoids; D) testosterone; E) activity; F) foraging; G) agonism. Vertical dashed lines represent the end of a study year. In each year, individuals were studied in three seasons (dry, rainy, and storms). Center lines show the medians; box limits indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles; whiskers extend 1.5 times the interquartile range from the 25th and 75th percentiles; data points are plotted as red circles.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 3.

Correlogram depicting the relationships among male mantled howler monkeys’ physiological mediators. Color scale (defined on the right) corresponds to variation in Pearson correlation coefficients.

Variation in male allostatic load

Variation in ALI was best explained by an averaged model including three models with Δi < 2 and the predictors seasonality, encounters with extragroup males, mating, and centrality (Table 4). This averaged model was significantly different from a null model including only the intercept and the random factor (likelihood ratio test: χ24 = 42.1, P < 0.001) and explained 83% (R2 = 0.83) of variation in ALI. Model 1 included seasonality, encounters with extragroup males, and mating, and was two times more plausible than Model 2 (excluding term mating) and Model 3 (including term centrality), respectively.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 4.

Model-averaged GLMM of factors affecting allostatic load in male mantled howler monkeys.

ALI increased during seasons when males faced harsher conditions (Fig. 4a) and more frequent encounters with extragroup males (Fig. 4b). Additionally, ALI was higher in central (mean ± SE = 0.28 ± 0.12) than in non-central males (mean ± SE = −0.18 ± 0.14; Fig. 4c), and when males were involved (mean ± SE = 0.99 ± 0.15) in mating than when they were not (mean ±SE = −0.33 ±0.09; Fig. 4d).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 4.

Variation in allostatic load index (ALI) residuals of mantled howler monkeys as a function of: A) seasonality; B) encounters with extragroup males; C) mating; and D) centrality. Residuals were obtained from the full model excluding the variable of interest. In A and B trend lines are represented for illustrative purposes only with 95% confidence intervals. In C and D, center lines show the medians; black diamonds are means; box limits indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles; whiskers extend 1.5 times the interquartile range from the 25th and 75th percentiles; notches give roughly 95% confidence that two medians differ. Data points are plotted as solid circles.

Discussion

In the present study we constructed, for the first time, an allostatic load index (ALI) for a wild primate species, the mantled howler monkey. This ALI was based on seven physiological mediators, which reflected the metabolic, thyroid, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal, reproductive, and behavioral functioning of males. We furthermore examined whether this ALI responded to social, ecological, and abiotic factors which have been previously reported to represent challenges to the homeostasis of male mantled howler monkeys. We found that seasonality, encounters with extragroup males, mating, and centrality were the main determinants of male allostatic load, though only the first two had significant effects on ALI. Although the analysis presented in this paper is based on a small sample, we believe that the long duration of the study and the detailed sampling regime resulted in a dataset that provides a realistic picture of the allostatic load faced by males in this species.

Seasonality and encounters with extragroup males had the strongest effects on ALI. Seasonality was a composite measure that included ambient temperature, total and fruit biomass, and photoperiod. Although seasonal variation in abiotic factors (temperature, photoperiod) and in plant biomass are lower in the tropics than in other regions (van Schaik, Terborgh & Wright, 1993), it has been demonstrated that they still affect animal populations (e.g., behavior, reproduction: Brockman & van Schaik, 2005). Furthermore, we have recently reported in this same population that triiodothyronine is positively related to fruit intake (Dias et al., 2017), which in turn is higher when more fruit is available (Gómez-Espinosa et al., 2014), and the energetic condition of females is positively correlated with food biomass (Cano-Huertes et al., 2017). It is possible that the higher allostatic load of male mantled howler monkeys during periods of lower food abundance, lower ambient temperatures, and shorter days is associated with increased difficulties of finding food (Dunn et al., 2010, 2013) and increased thermoregulatory effort (Thompson et al., 2017). This increase in allostatic load was probably independent of mating effort, as sexual behavior was evenly distributed throughout climatic seasons. Therefore, ecological and abiotic factors significantly impact the allostatic load of male mantled howler monkeys.

Encounters with extragroup males were positively related with male allostatic load. This is in line with studies demonstrating that intrasexual competition represents a strong challenge for primate males (e.g., Emery Thompson & Georgiev, 2014), including howler monkeys (Cristóbal-Azkarate et al., 2004; Cristóbal-Azkarate, Chavira, R., Boeck, Rodríguez-Luna & Veà, 2006; Crockett & Pope, 1988; Ryan et al., 2008). Extragroup males, either living solitarily or in other groups, represent competitors for resident males for the access to receptive females and for the limited number of reproductive positions available in groups (Van Belle & Bicca-Marques, 2015). Additionally, extragroup males may commit infanticide which has a negative impact on the reproductive success of residents (Crockett, 2003). Extragroup males are therefore usually chased and attacked by resident males (Glander, 1992). Visual and physical encounters with extragroup males are infrequent, but involve high risks, as they may escalate into direct fights that may result in physical injury or death (Cristóbal-Azkarate et al., 2004; Dias et al., 2010). Vocal encounters are less risky, but entail time and energy investment (Da Cunha & Byrne, 2006; Van Belle, Estrada & Garber, 2014). Our results suggest that intrasexual competition in the form of encounters with extragroup males represents an important source of allostatic load for males of this species.

Mating and centrality resulted in higher allostatic load. Aside from potential somatic costs associated with sustaining of reproductive behavior via testosterone actions (e.g., inhibition of immune system: Klein, 2000; Schmid-Hempel, 2003; Prall & Muehlenbein, 2014), mating may involve other costs, such as energetic (e.g., energy expenditure: Higham & Maestripieri, 2014; Lane, Boutin, Speakman & Humphries, 2010), social (e.g., investment in alliance formation and maintenance: Muller & Mitani, 2005), or health (e.g., exposure to infectious diseases: Nunn et al., 2014) costs, all of which could increase allostatic load. Additionally, in mantled howler monkeys central males may have priority of access to receptive females (Jones, 1985), but such priority usually involves mate guarding (Corewyn, 2015; Jones, 1995), a behavior that entails time and energy costs resulting from vigilance of other males (Alberts, Altmann & Wilson, 1996; Matsubara, 2003). Non-central males, in turn, have to overcome the reproductive strategies of central males through alternative behaviors (e.g., sneaking copulations), but do attain a share of reproduction in their groups (Corewyn, 2015; Jones 1995). Therefore, centrality and reproductive success are not related in this species (Ellsworth, 2000; Milton, Lozier & Lacey, 2009), and our results suggest that perhaps as a consequence of such non-skewed reproductive system, all males (i.e., identity was not a predictor of ALI) experience increases in allostatic load when receptive females are available.

We predicted that ALI should vary among males based on the demonstrated effects of individual factors, such as age, ontogenetic trajectory, or rank on allostatic load in a variety of organisms (reviewed in Romero & Wingfield, 2016). Such variation could also bolster interindividual responses to environmental factors, such as food abundance or predation risk. It is possible that we could not detect those effects due the small sample of males that was analyzed. However, in addition to the already explained putative influence of reproductive strategies on allostatic load, the fact that ALI did not vary within and between years suggests that males are able to recover from perturbations, such as seasonal food shortages, competition events, or mating. As we do not have evidence that males in this population experience homeostatic overload (McEwen & Wingfield, 2003), we propose that the observed trends correspond to normal fluctuations in behavioral and physiological mechanisms that allow individuals to cope with challenges in their environment. A demonstration of this proposal, besides requiring a larger sample of males, should include the study of individuals living under more diverse environmental conditions. Armed with a quantitative understanding of variation in allostatic load provided by the ALI defined here, we are currently conducting such a study in the Los Tuxtlas region.

Acknowledgements

We thank P. Cruz-Miros, A. Martínez, M. Rayón, A. Moctezuma, E. Reynoso, S. Cañadas, B. Cano, E. Ceccarelli and several volunteers for their support during fieldwork, and thanks E. Gómez-Espinosa and O. Dominguez for their support in the laboratory. La Flor de Catemaco S.A. de C. V. granted permission to work at this site, and Ing. J.L. Ponce Puente facilitated our work in a variety of ways. We thank A. Molina for her vital administrative support to our project. We thank C.M. Schaffner for her very constructive comments on earlier versions of the manuscript. A. Rangel-Negrín and P.A.D. Dias thank Mariana and Fernando for constant support inspiration to study primate behavior.

References

  1. ↵
    Abbott, D. H., Keverne, E. B., Bercovitch, F. B., Shively, C. A., Mendoza, S. P., Saltzman, W., …Sapolsky, R. M. (2003). Are subordinates always stressed? A comparative analysis of rank differences in cortisol levels among primates. Hormones and Behavior 43, 67–82.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  2. ↵
    Aguilar-Cucurachi, M. S., Dias, P. A. D., Rangel-Negrín, A., Chavira, R., Boeck, L., & Canales-Espinosa, D. (2010). Accumulation of stress during translocation in mantled howlers. American Journal of Primatology 72, 805–810.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  3. ↵
    Alberts, S. C., Altmann, J., & Wilson, M. L. (1996). Mate guarding constrains foraging activity of male baboons. Animal Behaviour 51, 1269–1277.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  4. ↵
    1. Gama-Campillo, L.,
    2. Pozo-Montuy, G.,
    3. Contreras-Sánchez, W. M.,
    4. Arriaga-Weiss, S. L.
    Arroyo-Rodríguez, V., Dias, P. A. D. & Cristóbal-Azkarate, J. (2011). Group size and foraging effort in mantled howlers at Los Tuxtlas, Mexico: A preliminary test of the ecological-constraints model. In: Gama-Campillo, L., Pozo-Montuy, G., Contreras-Sánchez, W. M., Arriaga-Weiss, S. L. (Eds.), Perspectivas en Primatología Mexicana (pp. 103–116.) Universidad Juás;rez Autónoma de Tabasco, Villahermosa, México
  5. ↵
    Barton, K. (2017). ‘MuMln’: multi-model inference. R Package Version 1.40.0.
  6. ↵
    Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2017). ‘Ime4’: linear mixed-effects models using ‘Eigen’ and S4. R Package Version 1.1-14.
  7. ↵
    Baumgarten, A., & Williamson, G. B. (2007). The distributions of howling monkeys (Alouatta pigra and A. palliata) in southeastern Mexico and Central America. Primates 48, 310–315.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  8. ↵
    Beehner, J. C., & Bergman, T. J., 2017. The next step for stress research in primates: to identify relationships between glucocorticoid secretion and fitness. Hormones and Behavioral, 68–83.
  9. Bird, C. E., Seeman, T. E., Escarce, J. J., Basurto-Dávila, R., Finch, B. K., Dubowitz, T.,… Lurie, N. (2010). Neighbourhood socioeconomic status and biological ‘wear and tear’ in a nationally representative sample of US adults. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 64, 860–865.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    Bonier, F., Moore, I. T., Martin, P. R., & Robertson, R. J. (2009). The relationship between fitness and baseline glucocorticoids in a passerine bird. General and Comparative Endocrinology 163, 208–213.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  11. ↵
    Boonstra, R. (2013). Reality as the leading cause of stress: rethinking the impact of chronic stress in nature. Functional Ecology 27, 11–23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  12. ↵
    Breuner, C. W., Patterson, S. H., & Hahn, T. P. (2008). In search of relationships between the acute adrenocortical response and fitness. General and Comparative Endocrinology 157, 288–295.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  13. ↵
    Brockman, D. K., & van Schaik, C. P. (2005). Seasonality in Primates: Studies of Living and Extinct Human and Non-Human Primates. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  14. ↵
    Burnham, K. P., & Anderson, D. R. (2013). Model Selection and Multimodel Inference. Springer, New York.
  15. ↵
    Cano-Huertes, B., Rangel-Negrín, A., Coyohua-Fuentes, A., Chavira-Ramírez, D., Canales-Espinosa, D., & Dias, P. A. D. (2017). Reproductive energetics of female mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata). International Journal of Primatology 38, 942–961.
    OpenUrl
  16. ↵
    Cavigelli, S. A., & Caruso, M. J. (2015). Sex, social status and physiological stress in primates: The importance of social and glucocorticoid dynamics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 370, 20140103.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Corewyn, L. (2015). Dominance, access to females, and mating success among coresident male mantled howlers (Alouatta palliata) at La Pacifica, Costa Rica. American Journal of Primatology 77, 388–400.
    OpenUrl
  18. ↵
    Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., Dias, P. A. D., & Veà, J. J. (2004). Causes of intraspecific aggression in Alouatta palliata mexicana: evidence from injuries, demography, and habitat. International Journal of Primatology 25, 939–953.
    OpenUrl
  19. ↵
    Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., Chavira, R., Boeck, L., Rodríguez-Luna, E., & Veà, J. J. (2006). Testosterone levels of free-ranging resident mantled howler monkey males in relation to the number and density of solitary males: A test of the challenge hypothesis. Hormones and Behavior 49, 261–267.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  20. ↵
    Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., Chavira, R., Boeck, L., Rodríguez-Luna, E., & Veà, J. J. (2007). Glucocorticoid levels in free ranging resident mantled howlers: a study of coping strategies. American Journal of Primatology 69, 866–876.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  21. ↵
    Crockett, C. M., & Pope, T. R. (1988). Inferring patterns of aggression from red howler monkey injuries. American Journal of Primatology 15, 289–308.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  22. ↵
    1. Jones, C. B.
    Crockett, C. M. (2003). Re-evaluating the sexual selection hypothesis for infanticide by Alouatta males. In: Jones, C. B. (Ed.), Sexual Selection and Reproductive Competition in Primates: New Perspectives and Directions (pp. 327–365). Norman, The American Society of Primatologists.
  23. ↵
    Da Cunha, R. G. T., & Byrne, R. W. (2006). Roars of black howler monkeys (Alouatta caraya): evidence for a function in inter-group spacing. Behaviour 143, 1169–1199.
    OpenUrl
    1. Campbell, C. J.,
    2. Fuentes, A.,
    3. MacKinnon, K. C.,
    4. Panger, M.,
    5. Bearder, S. K.
    Di Fiore, A., & Campbell, C. J. (2007). The atelines: variation in ecology, behavior, and social organization. In: Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Panger, M., Bearder, S. K. (Eds.), Primates in Perspective (pp. 155–185). Oxford University Press, New York,
  24. ↵
    Dias, P. A. D., & Rodríguez-Luna, E. (2006). Seasonal changes in associative behavior and subgrouping patterns of mantled howler monkey males living on an island. International Journal of Primatology 27, 1635–1651.
    OpenUrl
  25. ↵
    Dias, P. A. D., Rangel-Negrín, A., Veà, J. J., & Canales-Espinosa, D. (2010). Coalitions and malemale behavior in Alouatta palliata. Primates 51:91–94.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Kowalewski, M. M.,
    2. Garber, P. A.,
    3. Cortés-Ortiz, L,
    4. Urbani, B., &
    5. Youlatos, D.
    Dias, P. A. D., & Rangel-Negrín, A. (2015a). Diets of howler monkeys. In: Kowalewski, M. M., Garber, P. A., Cortés-Ortiz, L, Urbani, B., & Youlatos, D. (Eds.), Howler Monkeys Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation (pp. 21–56). Springer, New York.
  27. ↵
    Dias, P. A. D., & Rangel-Negrín, A. (2015b). An ethogram of the social behavior of adult Alouatta palliata mexicana and A. pigra. http://www.uv.mx/personal/pdias. DOI: 10.13140/RG.2.1.1043.7840.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. ↵
    Dias, P. A. D., Coyohua-Fuentes, A., Canales-Espinosa, D., Chavira-Ramírez, D. R., & Rangel-Negrín, A. (2017). Hormonal correlates of energetic condition in mantled howler monkeys. Hormones and Behavior 94, 13–20.
    OpenUrl
  29. ↵
    Dunn, J. C., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Veà, J. J. (2010). Seasonal variations in the diet and feeding effort of two groups of howlers in different sized forest fragments. International Journal of Primatology 31, 887–903.
    OpenUrl
  30. ↵
    Dunn, J. C., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., Schulte-Herbrüggen, B., Chavira, R., & Veà, J. J. (2013). Travel time predicts fecal glucocorticoid levels in free-ranging howlers (Alouatta palliata). International Journal of Primatology 34, 246–259.
    OpenUrl
  31. ↵
    Edes, A. N., & Crews, D. E. (2017). Allostatic load and biological anthropology. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 162, 44–70.
    OpenUrl
  32. ↵
    Edes, A. N., Wolfe, B. A., & Crews, D. E. (2016a). Assessing stress in zoo-housed western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) using allostatic load. International Journal of Primatology 37, 241–259.
    OpenUrl
  33. ↵
    Edes, A. N., Wolfe, B. A., & Crews, D. E. (2016b). Rearing history and allostatic load in adult western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) in human care. Zoo Biology 35, 167–173.
    OpenUrl
  34. ↵
    Ellsworth, J. A. (2000). Molecular evolution, social structure, and phylogeography of the mantled howler monkey (Alouatta palliata) (dissertation). University of Nevada, Reno (NV).
  35. ↵
    Emery Thompson, M., & Georgiev, A. V. (2014). The high price of success: costs of mating effort in male primates. International Journal of Primatology 35, 609–627.
    OpenUrl
  36. ↵
    Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2017). ‘car’: companion to applied regression. R Package Version 2.1-5.
  37. ↵
    Glei, D. A., Goldman, N., Rodríguez, G., & Weinstein, M. (2014). Beyond self-reports: Changes in biomarkers as predictors of mortality. Population and Development Review 40, 331–360.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Gómez-Espinosa, R., Rangel-Negrín, A., Chavira-Ramírez, D. R., Canales-Espinosa, D., & Dias, P. A. D. (2014). Glucocorticoid response in mantled howlers (Alouatta palliata) in relation to energetic and psychosocial stressors. American Journal of Primatology 76, 362–373.
    OpenUrl
  39. ↵
    Guidelines for the Use of Animals (2012). Animal Behaviour 83, 301–309.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  40. ↵
    Higham, J. P., & Maestripieri, D. (2014). The costs of reproductive success in Cayo Santiago male rhesus macaques. International Journal of Primatology 35, 661–676.
    OpenUrl
  41. ↵
    Jaeger, B. (2017). ‘r2glmrr?: computes r squared for mixed (multilevel) model. R Package Version 0.1.2.
  42. ↵
    Jones, C. B. (1980). The functions of status in the mantled howler monkey, Alouatta palliata Gray: Intraspecific competition for group membership in a folivorous neotropical primate. Primates 21, 389–405.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  43. ↵
    Jones, C. B. (1985). Reproductive patterns in mantled howler monkeys: Estrus, mate choice and copulation. Primates 26, 130–142.
    OpenUrl
  44. ↵
    Jones, C. B. (1995). Alternative reproductive behaviors in the mantled howler monkey (Alouatta palliata Gray): testing Carpenter’s hypothesis. Boletín Primatologico Latinamericano 5, 1–5.
    OpenUrl
  45. ↵
    Kahrig, S., & Halloran, A. R. (2010). Effects of weather, temperature, and humidity on feeding patterns of mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata). American Journal of Physical Anthropology 141 Suppl. 50, 138.
    OpenUrl
  46. ↵
    1. Kowalewski, M. M.,
    2. Garber, P. A.,
    3. Cortés-Ortiz, L.,
    4. Urbani, B., &
    5. Youlatos, D.
    Kitchen, D. M., da Cunha, R. G. T., Holzmann, I., & de Oliveira, D. A. G. (2015). Function of loud calls in howler monkeys. In: Kowalewski, M. M., Garber, P. A., Cortés-Ortiz, L., Urbani, B., & Youlatos, D. (Eds.), Howler Monkeys: Adaptive Radiation, Systematics, and Morphology. (pp. 369–399). Springer, New York.
  47. ↵
    Klein, S. L. (2000). Hormones and mating system affect sex and species differences in immune function among vertebrates. Behavioral Processes 51, 149–166.
    OpenUrl
  48. ↵
    Landys, M. M., Ramenofsky, M., & Wingfield, J. C. (2006). Actions of glucocorticoids at a seasonal baseline as compared to stress-related levels in the regulation of periodic life processes. General and Compative Endocrinology 148, 132–149.
    OpenUrl
  49. ↵
    Lane, J. E., Boutin, S., Speakman, J. R., & Humphries, M. M. (2010). Energetic costs of male reproduction in a scramble competition mating system. Journal of Animal Ecology 79, 27–34.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  50. ↵
    Levine, M. E., & Crimmins, E. M. (2014). A comparison of methods for assessing mortality risk. American Journal of Human Biology 26, 768–776.
    OpenUrl
  51. ↵
    Matsubara, M. (2003). Costs of mate guarding and opportunistic mating among wild male Japanese macaques. International Journal of Primatology 24, 1057–1075.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  52. ↵
    Mattei, J., Demissie, S., Falcon, L. M., Ordovas, J. M., & Tucker, K. (2010). Allostatic load is associated with chronic conditions in the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study. Social Science & Medicine 70, 1988–1996.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    McEwen, B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: Central role of the brain. Physiology Reviews 87, 873–904.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  54. ↵
    McEwen, B. S., & Stellar. E. (1993). Stress and the individual: mechanisms leading to disease. Archives of Internal Medicine 153, 2093–2101.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  55. ↵
    McEwen, B. S., & Wingfield, J. C. (2003). The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Hormones and Behavior 43, 2–15.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  56. MacLarnon, A. M., Sommer, V., Goffe, A. S., Higham, J. P., Lodge, E., Tkaczynski, P., Ross, C. (2015). Assessing adaptability and reactive scope: Introducing a new measure and illustrating its use through a case study of environmental stress in forest-living baboons. General and Compative Endocrinology 215, 10–24.
    OpenUrl
  57. ↵
    Milton, K., Lozier, J. D., & Lacey, E. A. (2009). Genetic structure of an isolated population of mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) on Barro Colorado Island, Panama. Conservation Genetics 10, 347–358.
    OpenUrl
  58. ↵
    Muller, M. N., & Mitani, J. C. (2005). Conflict and cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Advances in the Study Behavior 35, 275–331.
    OpenUrl
  59. Nelson, R. J., & Kriegsfeld, L. J. (2016). An Introduction to Behavioral Endocrinology, 5Th Edition. Sinauer, New York.
  60. ↵
    Nunn, C. L., Scully, E. J., Kutsukake, N., Ostner, J., Schulke, O., & Thrall, P. H. (2014). Mating competition, promiscuity, and life history traits as predictors of sexually-transmitted disease risk in primates. International Journal of Primatology 35, 764–786.
    OpenUrl
  61. ↵
    Prall, S. P., & Muehlenbein, M. P. (2014). Testosterone and immune function in primates: A brief summary with methodological considerations. International Journal of Primatology 35, 805–824.
    OpenUrl
  62. ↵
    R Core Team (2017). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna.
  63. R Core Team (2017). The R Stats package. R Package Version 3.5.0.
  64. ↵
    Rangel-Negrín, A., Flores-Escobar, E., Chavira, R., Canales-Espinosa, D., & Dias, P. A. D. (2014). Physiological and analytical validations of fecal steroid hormone measures in black howler monkeys. Primates 55, 459–465.
    OpenUrl
  65. ↵
    Reynoso-Cruz, E., Rangel-Negrín, A., Coyohua-Fuentes, A., Canales-Espinosa, D., & Dias, P. A. D. (2016). Measures of food intake in mantled howling monkeys. Primates 57, 161–166.
    OpenUrl
  66. ↵
    Romero, L. M. (2012). Using the reactive scope model to understand why stress physiology predicts survival during starvation in Galapagos marine iguanas. General and Compative Endocrinology 176, 296–299.
    OpenUrl
  67. ↵
    Romero, L. M., & Wingfield, J. C. (2016). Tempests, Poxes, Predators, and People: Stress in Wild Animals and How They Cope. Oxford University Press, New York.
  68. Rosenberg, N., Park, C. G., Eldeirawi, K. (2014). Relationship of serum carotenoid concentrations with allostatic load as a measure of chronic stress among middle-aged adults in the USA. Public Health Nutr. 18, 313–321.
    OpenUrl
  69. ↵
    Ryan, S. J., Starks, P. T., Milton, K., & Getz, W. M. (2008). Intersexual conflict and group size in Alouatta palliata: a 23-year evaluation. International Journal of Primatology 29, 405–420.
    OpenUrl
  70. ↵
    Sapolsky, R. M., Romero, L. M., & Munck, A. U. (2000). How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine Research 21, 55–89.
    OpenUrl
  71. ↵
    Schmid-Hempel, P. (2003). Variation in immune defense as a question of evolutionary ecology. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 270, 357–366.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  72. ↵
    Seeman, T. E., McEwen, B. S., Rowe, J. W., & Singer, B. H. (2001). Allostatic load as a marker of cumulative biological risk: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 98, 4770–4775.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  73. ↵
    Seeman, T. E., Singer, B. H., Ryff, C. D., Love, G. D., & Levy-Storms, L. (2002). Social relationships, gender, and allostatic load across two age cohorts. Psychosomatic Medicine 64, 395–406.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  74. ↵
    Shedden-González, A., & Rodríguez-Luna, E. (2010). Responses of a translocated howler monkey Alouatta palliata group to new environmental conditions. Endangered Species Research 12, 25–30.
    OpenUrl
  75. ↵
    1. González Soriano, E.,
    2. Dirzo, R.,
    3. Vogt, R.
    Soto, M., & Gama, L. (1997). Climas. In: González Soriano, E., Dirzo, R., Vogt, R. (Eds.), Historia Natural de Los Tuxtlas (pp. 7–23). UNAM and CONABIO, Mexico City
  76. ↵
    Thompson, C. L., Powell, B. L., Williams, S. H., Hanya, G., Glander, K. E., & Vinyard, C. J. (2017). Thyroid hormone fluctuations indicate a thermoregulatory function in both a tropical (Alouatta palliata) and seasonally cold-habitat (Macaco fuscata) primate. American Journal of Primatology DOI: 10.1002/ajp.22714.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  77. ↵
    Van Belle, S., Estrada, A., & Strier, K. B. (2008). Social relationships among male Alouatta pigra. International Journal of Primatology 29, 1481–1498.
    OpenUrl
  78. ↵
    Van Belle, S., Estrada, A., Ziegler, T. E., & Strier, K. B. (2009). Social and hormonal mechanisms underlying male reproductive strategies in black howler monkeys (Alouatta pigra). Hormones and Behavior 56, 355–363.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  79. ↵
    Van Belle, S., Estrada, A., & Garber, P. A. (2014). The function of loud calls in black howler monkeys (Alouatta pigra): food, mate, or infant defense? American Journal of Primatology 76, 1196–1206.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Kowalewski, M. M.,
    2. Garber, P. A.,
    3. Cortés-Ortiz, L.,
    4. Urbani, B., &
    5. Youlatos, D.
    Van Belle, S., & Bicca-Marques, J. C. (2015). Insights into reproductive strategies and selection in howler monkeys. In: Kowalewski, M. M., Garber, P. A., Cortés-Ortiz, L., Urbani, B., & Youlatos, D. (Eds.), Howler Monkeys: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation (pp. 57–84). Springer Press, New York.
  81. ↵
    van Schaik, C. P., Terborgh, J. W., & Wright, S. J. (1993). The phenology of tropical forests: Adaptive significance and consequences for primary consumers. Annual Reviews of Ecology, Evolution and Systematics 24, 353–377.
    OpenUrl
  82. ↵
    Wang, E., & Milton, K. E. (2003). Intragroup social relationship of male Alouatta palliata on Barro Colorado Island, Republic of Panama. International Journal of Primatology 24, 1227–1243.
    OpenUrl
  83. ↵
    Wasser, S. K., Hunt, K. E., Brown, J. L., Cooper, K., Crockett, C. M., Bechert, U., …Monfort, S. L. (2000). A generalized fecal glucocorticoid assay for use in a diverse array of nondomestic mammalian and avian species. General and Compative Endocrinology 120, 260–275.
    OpenUrl
  84. ↵
    West, B. T., Welch, K. B., Galecki, A. T. (2014). Linear Mixed Models. A Practical Guide Using Statistical Software. Chapman & Hall, Boca Raton.
    1. Setchell, J. M., &
    2. Curtis, D. J.
    Williamson, E. A., & Feistner, A. T. C. (2003). Habituating primates: processes, techniques, variables and ethics. In: Setchell, J. M., & Curtis, D. J. (Eds.), Field and Laboratory Methods in Primatology: A Practical Guide (pp. 25–39). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted February 23, 2021.
Download PDF
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
A preliminary assessment of allostatic load in male mantled howler monkeys
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
A preliminary assessment of allostatic load in male mantled howler monkeys
Pedro Américo D. Dias, Alejandro Coyohua-Fuentes, David Roberto Chavira-Ramírez, Domingo Canales-Espinosa, Ariadna Rangel-Negrín
bioRxiv 2021.02.22.432350; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.22.432350
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo LinkedIn logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
A preliminary assessment of allostatic load in male mantled howler monkeys
Pedro Américo D. Dias, Alejandro Coyohua-Fuentes, David Roberto Chavira-Ramírez, Domingo Canales-Espinosa, Ariadna Rangel-Negrín
bioRxiv 2021.02.22.432350; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.22.432350

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Zoology
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (4384)
  • Biochemistry (9602)
  • Bioengineering (7100)
  • Bioinformatics (24887)
  • Biophysics (12625)
  • Cancer Biology (9968)
  • Cell Biology (14365)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (7966)
  • Ecology (12116)
  • Epidemiology (2067)
  • Evolutionary Biology (15997)
  • Genetics (10932)
  • Genomics (14746)
  • Immunology (9875)
  • Microbiology (23683)
  • Molecular Biology (9486)
  • Neuroscience (50909)
  • Paleontology (370)
  • Pathology (1540)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (2684)
  • Physiology (4022)
  • Plant Biology (8669)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (1510)
  • Synthetic Biology (2397)
  • Systems Biology (6442)
  • Zoology (1346)