Abstract
Human culture is considered to differ from animal culture due to its interactive nature built on shared intentionality and cognitive flexibility. Here, we investigated whether chimpanzees use communication to engage in cultural practices by analyzing grooming handclasp (GHC) interactions – a socio-cultural behavior requiring coordination. Previous accounts attributed GHC initiations to behavioral shaping whereby the initiator physically molds the partner’s arm into the GHC posture. Using frame-by-frame analysis and matched-control methodology, we find that chimpanzees use gestural communication to initiate GHC, which requires an active and synchronized response from the partner. This showcases a behavioral expression of joint commitment to engage in this shared cultural practice. Moreover, we show that GHC initiators used various initiation strategies, attesting to situation-contingent interactional flexibility. We conclude that chimpanzees can be jointly committed to a cultural practice, which suggests that culture predicated on shared intentionality and flexible communication may not be unique to the human species.
Introduction
Culture – the inheritance of behavioral phenotypes through social learning (Laland and Janik, 2006) – lies at the heart of the biological success of the human species (Henrich, 2016). It is thought to qualitatively differ from culture in non-human animals (henceforth ‘animals’) by its communicative nature reflecting both shared intentionality and cognitive flexibility (Enfield and Levinson, 2006; Tomasello, 2019). Shared intentionality is present in collaborative interactions in which participants share psychological states with one another (Tomasello and Carpenter, 2007). It facilitates mutual engagement and commitment from the collaborators by synchronizing their attention and attuning their actions into adequate and complementary action-response sequences. In human culture, two individuals may thus enter a state of shared commitment to engage in a cultural ritual together (e.g., shaking hands when meeting each other (Oxlund, 2020)), which allows for the coordination of the required actions to pursue and complete the shared ritual (Bratman 1992; Enfield and Levinson 2006).
According to an influential view in developmental psychology, animals lack the prerequisites for shared intentionality (Tomasello, 2019; Tomasello et al., 2012; Tomasello and Carpenter, 2007; Tomasello and Moll, 2010). Series of experiments showed that chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are equivalent to humans when it comes to cognitive skills for dealing with the physical world (e.g., space, causal reasoning), but lag behind substantially in the social realm (e.g., high-fidelity imitative learning, cooperative communication; Herrmann et al. 2007; Tomasello and Moll 2010). In turn, it has been suggested that these reduced socio-cultural capacities preclude chimpanzees from understanding the roles of joint activities and sharing joint goals, the prerequisites for shared intentionality (see (Tomasello and Moll, 2010)).
Recent work, however, has challenged this view and proposed to seek for evolutionary precursors of human shared intentionality not with experimental designs but within naturally occurring contexts of interacting animals (Genty et al., 2020; Heesen et al., 2020, 2017). From an interactional perspective, correlates of shared intentionality could manifest in the establishment of joint attention, joint commitment and joint actions, given their requisite functions for sharing psychological states (Enfield and Levinson, 2006; Genty et al., 2020). Bonobos, for example, have been observed to use communication to facilitate re-engagement of the partner during social games (Pika and Zuberbühler, 2008), and naturally occurring interactions (Heesen et al., 2020), which attests to their commitment to joint activities (Genty et al., 2020). Given the interactional nature of human culture (Enfield and Levinson, 2006; Tomasello, 2019) and the quest for what makes human culture unique (Boyd et al., 2011; Ramsey, 2013; Richerson and Boyd, 2005), an important outstanding question is whether great apes similarly display (correlates of) shared intentionality during the execution of their joint cultural practices.
Here, we investigate whether chimpanzees communicate their desire to engage partners in a joint cultural practice requiring the coordination of actions – the grooming handclasp (McGrew and Tutin, 1978). The grooming handclasp (henceforth “GHC”) is a cultural social-grooming variant defined as a symmetrical postural configuration in which two partners simultaneously extend one of their arms overhead and clasp each other’s extended hand at the palm, wrist or forearm, while grooming each other with the other arm (McGrew and Tutin, 1978; Nakamura and Uehara, 2004; van Leeuwen et al., 2012). While the cultural nature of GHC has been firmly established (McGrew et al., 2001; Nakamura, 2002; van Leeuwen et al., 2017, 2012; Wrangham et al., 2016), little is known about the ways in which chimpanzees (or bonobos: (Fruth et al., 2006)) coordinate the execution of the GHC, other than one individual (i.e., the initiator) physically shaping the body of the envisioned partner into the GHC posture (De Waal and Seres, 1997). To learn more about this coordination process, we studied the onset of GHCs with frame-by-frame analysis and compared the observed behaviors with matched-control windows (i.e., social grooming events of the same partners without GHC) (sensu De Waal and Yoshihara 1983). Furthermore, we examined whether chimpanzees initiated GHCs flexibly or in ritualized sequences to shed light on their interactional versatility. Lastly, we inspected whether chimpanzees showed signs of determination/goal-directedness to perform this joint action together with their allocated partner, by identifying the use of persistence and/or elaboration of initial initiation strategies when the desired response (i.e., the partner’s commitment to GHC) remained absent. In conjunction, these investigations may illuminate if great apes have the capacity and motivation to engage in socio-cultural practices by means of (correlates of) shared intentionality.
Results
GHC initiations
We examined GHC initiations in a group of 52 semi-wild chimpanzees (Table S1). To determine the mechanisms underlying GHC initiation, we investigated the occurrences of ten selected chimpanzee behaviors (Table S2 and Table S3) in a comparison between i) GHC initiations and initiations of regular grooming bouts, and ii) pre-handclasp (PH) periods and their matched-control (MC) windows (see Methods and Figure 1). Four behaviors (“head touch”, “nosewipe”, “self-scratch” and “torso”) occurred both during GHC initiations and initiations of regular grooming bouts (>15% of the bouts; see Table S3), and were thus not considered unique to GHC initiations. Six behaviors were observed infrequently during initiations of regular grooming bouts (<15%) and more frequently in the PH compared to the MC context (Wilcoxon signed-rank: all p<0.04, Holm-corrected; nph-mc=94; nind=33; see Table S4). These behaviors were thus considered specific to GHC initiations. Two of these behaviors (“elbow hold” and “hand grab”) corresponded to the practice of shaping (De Waal and Seres 1997; e.g., Video S11), while the remaining behaviors (“elbow touch”, “hand touch”, “head move”, and “hold”) did not involve any/prolonged physical contact with the partner. These latter behaviors were considered to be potentially communicative, i.e., in the form of gestures (Liebal and Call 2012; e.g., Video S12).
GHC bout including the identified pre-handclasp (PH) and matched-control period (MC). The PHs and MCs were chosen to exactly match in terms of individuals, bodily positioning and activities (grooming) in order to identify mechanisms specific to the initiation of GHC.
Gestures are defined as bodily actions that are mechanically ineffective and recipient-directed, and result in a voluntary response from the recipient (Pika, 2008; Liebal and Call 2012). The four potentially communicative GHC-initiation behaviors followed this definition – they were mechanically ineffective bodily actions resulting in voluntary GHC responses. Only for “head move” we could envision partly non-intentional co-variation with other behaviors like changing grooming posture or redirecting attention. “Elbow touch” and “hand touch” involved targeted physical contact from actor to recipient and were thus recipient-directed, and during “hold” and “head move” signalers faced their recipient in 100% of observed instances (n=31 and n=18, respectively). These gestures were produced flexibly, with 14 of 15 individuals with more than one GHC initiation showing variation in the start behavior (only counting the 6 GHC-specific behaviors determined above) of their initiation sequences (Binomial test: p<0.001; also see Figure 2 and associated R-code). The gestures were also produced in a goal-directed way, as indicated by the occurrence of elaboration (i.e., the use of additional behavior: Leavens et al., 2005) in 31% of the cases where an initial gesture failed to initiate a GHC (n=10 out of the 32 instances where gestures were used as an initiation strategy, see Figure 2 and details below). Elaboration occurred after an average response waiting time of 0.5s and took the form of another gesture (n=4), a shaping behavior (n=2), or a combination of both another gesture and a shaping behavior (n=4) before the GHC finally commenced. Taken together, these observations show that chimpanzees are flexibly capable and determined to (re-)transmit their motivation to engage in GHC when needed.
Behavioral sequences by the initiator leading to GHC (n=110). Starting behaviors are depicted in the inner colored circle, with the grey outer circle being the endpoint of the sequence (i.e., GHC). In order to consider the full flexibility of all types of GHC initiations, we included the four “synchrony” behaviors that are also common to the initiation of regular grooming bouts (“head touch”, “nosewipe”, “self-scratch”, and “torso”). * Interactive version available as Figure S13.
In general, of the 94 PH/MC comparison bouts, 21 (22%) contained either or both shaping behaviors (“elbow hold”, “hand grab”), 32 (34%) contained one or more of the four communicative gestures (“elbow touch”, “hand touch”, “hold”, “head move”) and no shaping, and 41 (44%) contained neither shaping nor potentially communicative behaviors. We labeled the third type of GHC initiation as “synchronous”, as the individuals appeared to commit to the GHC near-simultaneously. Moreover, if any behavior was scored during the PH window in the synchronous GHCs aside from the raising of the arms (which by definition occurs before every GHC), these were behaviors common to the initiation of regular grooming bouts (“head touch”, “nosewipe”, “self-scratch”, “torso”, see Table S3).
Discussion
Our findings show that chimpanzees communicate to engage in one of their most enigmatic socio-cultural practices, the grooming handclasp (McGrew and Tutin, 1978). To date, the mechanism underlying the emergence and stability of the GHC culture in chimpanzees has been described in terms of physical shaping (De Waal and Seres, 1997). By applying matched-control methodologies (De Waal and Yoshihara, 1983) and following established criteria in the field of communication (Leavens et al., 2005; Liebal and Call, 2012; Pika, 2008), we identify flexible and goal-directed gestural communication as an additional mechanism by which chimpanzees initiate and coordinate their grooming handclasps.
The grooming handclasp is a cooperative activity that requires coordination for successful execution. Chimpanzees cooperate (Mitani, 2009), but not much is known about the ways in which they coordinate their joint efforts. In experimental settings, some chimpanzees used location enhancing behaviors (e.g., bodily positioning, touching, peering) (Melis and Tomasello, 2019), or generic gestures (e.g., arm fling, clapping, banging on panels; Voinov et al. 2020) to entice their conspecific partners into a joint action. In natural contexts, chimpanzees and bonobos communicate to coordinate joint actions like joint travel (Fröhlich et al., 2016) and social play (Heesen et al., 2017; Hobaiter and Byrne, 2014), yet “⃛ the degree to which these actions are joint in terms of whether or not partners aim to achieve shared goals together, or whether partners have shared intentions, remains unknown.” (Genty et al., 2020). Moreover, none of these examples pertain to cultural practices, while joint and shared intentionality (Koreň, 2016; Tomasello and Carpenter, 2007) conducive to joint action are so central to human accounts of culture (Enfield and Levinson, 2006; Tomasello, 2019).
Our findings identify chimpanzees to use gestural communication in a natural context to overcome a coordination challenge in the socio-cultural domain (McGrew and Tutin, 1978; Nakamura and Uehara, 2004; van Leeuwen et al., 2012). The socio-cultural interaction was not just shaped by one invested individual (De Waal and Seres, 1997), but when the initiator communicated its desire to engage in the GHC (e.g., by holding out its flexed arm at face level in front of the desired partner), an active commitment in the form of a complementary action by the partner was required to accomplish the interaction. As such, when communicated, the cultural GHC practice appeared to ensue with a degree of joint commitment regulating the coordination of the required actions, similar to the handshake example in humans. We conclude that chimpanzees synchronize their attention to a joint cultural action to which they are both committed, which suggests that chimpanzee social culture, like human culture, may be founded on (correlates of) shared intentionality (cf. (Tomasello et al., 2012; Tomasello and Carpenter, 2007; Tomasello and Moll, 2010)).
Reflecting on these findings, we posit that also for animals the socio-cultural context i) facilitates and shapes mechanisms conducive to coordinating joint actions (also see (Genty et al., 2020)), and ii) provides the adequate breeding ground for the transformation of individualistic to collaborative predispositions and capacities – a transformation thought to be unique to the evolution of the human species (Tomasello and Carpenter, 2007). Regarding i), the socio-cultural property of the GHC (McGrew et al., 2001; Nakamura, 2002; van Leeuwen et al., 2012) entails a group-level engagement which spurs in individuals both the motivation to instigate and be susceptible to requests for interaction and, in the case of initiating GHC, solve a coordination challenge. Whereas at first this context may produce overzealous individuals who physically shape their naïve partners into GHC (De Waal and Seres, 1997), in a later stage, the shared readiness to coordinate this joint cultural practice may catalyze the transformation of one-sided shaping (only one actor) into two-sided communication (two committed actors). This hypothesis would a) explain why communication for coordination purposes has not been readily observed in chimpanzees (i.e., cultural contexts have been overlooked) (Duguid et al., 2020a, 2020b), and b) predict more communication and seamless coordination in more experienced dyads as focus for future research. Regarding ii), it has been posited that humans are unique in their cooperative engagement owing to their capacities for shared intentionality (Tomasello, 2019). Humans are thought to transform individualistic activities into joint, collaborative activities during ontogeny and especially throughout socio-cultural immersion – a process presumably absent in chimpanzees (Tomasello and Carpenter, 2007). It may thus be no coincidence that exactly in this socio-cultural GHC-context we observed capacities for joint attention and commitment in chimpanzees, similar to the ones ascribed to young children engaging in joint activities (Tomasello, 2019). The ontogeny of chimpanzees’ cultural engagement with a specific focus on elements of shared intentionality may provide exciting insights for theories on the evolution of human thinking (Heyes, 2018; Tomasello, 2019).
Methods
Subjects
Subject were 52 chimpanzees (Table S1) at the Chimfunshi Wildlife Orphanage, Zambia. GHC has been a customary behavior in the group for over 15 years, and 39 individuals (including all 32 adults) were observed to GHC at least once. The study was approved by the Chimfunshi Research Advisory Board (permit: CWOT_ 2019C039), and conformed to the nationwide legal requirements. Chimfunshi is accredited by PASA and adheres to the rules and regulations with respect to animal care and management as stipulated by the Zambia Wildlife Authority.
Collection & Coding
Data were collected by ZG from 23-03-2019 to 04-06-2019 between 8am-4pm with handheld digital video cameras (Panasonic HDC-HS100). To capture GHC initiations, filming commenced as soon as two individuals approached one another. Filming continued if the individuals started social grooming and lasted until they (a) had stopped grooming for over 30 seconds, (b) started grooming another individual, or (c) physically separated. A grooming bout was defined as running from the start of grooming until the moment one of the aforementioned ending conditions was met. A bout was considered a GHC-bout if it contained one or multiple GHCs, and a regular grooming bout if no GHCs occurred.
GHC-bouts had either a side or back view and were analyzed for initiation behaviors if a 10 sec pre-handclasp (PH) social grooming window was available before the first GHC in the bout. We only analyzed the initiation of the first GHC in GHC-bouts, because previous GHCs could possibly function as signals for subsequent GHCs. The start of a GHC was defined as the instance of handclasp above face level; the end as the instance that physical contact of the palms/wrists was broken. Moreover, a Matched-Control (MC) period was recorded to enable comparison of individual initiation behaviors across conditions (De Waal and Yoshihara, 1983). The MC-period was defined as a 10sec-window minimally 10sec after the last GHC occurrence in the GHC bout, in which the individuals had to be positioned in the same relative positions as during the GHC, while still engaging in social grooming (Figure 1). Additionally, initiations of regular, non-GHC-bouts were opportunistically recorded (nside and back=23, Table S3) to identify behaviors used in the initiation of regular grooming bouts.
Videos were scored in ELAN (Wittenburg et al., 2006), behaviors were coded based on preliminary screening of the videos and established chimpanzee ethograms (Nishida et al., 1999) (see Table S2 and SI videos). An individual was considered the initiator of a GHC bout when they were either a) the first one to produce a GHC-specific initiation behavior or b) in the absence of these behaviors, the first to raise their arm for the GHC. A subset of 20% of the data was coded by two further observers to establish IRR. Mean dyadic agreement was 0.833 for coding behaviors (range 0.81-0.89), and 0.973 for identifying the initiating individual (range 0.89-1; see SI for details).
Analyses
Analyses were done in R 3.6.1. Non-parametric statistics were applied, including Bonferroni-Holm corrections for multiple testing (Holm, 1979). For the PH-MC comparison, we analyzed those behaviors that i) occurred ≥5 times in GHC-bouts, and ii) did not also frequently (>15% of bouts) occur during initiations of regular grooming bouts (see Table S3). Given that social grooming occurred in both PH and MC windows by definition (see Figure 1 and “Collection & Coding”), we did not consider the grooming behaviours (see Table S3) as possible signals for GHC initiation. We only used GHC-bouts with a side-view (optimal vantage point) and a Matched Control period (n=94). Auxiliary analysis including also back-view observations (ntotal=133) supported our main analyses (all 6 behaviors p<0.02, Holm corrected; see Tables S3 & S4). When assessing flexibility and elaboration in GHC initiations (Figure 2), we used all side-view bouts regardless of the presence of MCs (n=110).
Data availability statement
The code and data used for analyses are available via http://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4616274.
Competing interests
The authors declare to have no competing interests.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
The authors declare no conflict of interests to exist. The research on chimpanzees was strictly non-invasive/observational and adhered to the ethical guidelines provided by the Chimfunshi Research Advisory Board. All applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed.
Funding
EJCvL was funded by a Postdoctoral Fellowship (12W5318N) awarded by the Research Foundation Flanders (FWO). ZG was supported by the ABS Student Grant from the Animal Behavior Society. The authors declare no competing interests.
Supplementary Information captions
SI_1. Supplementary information and tables, including subject demographics, the behavioral ethogram, frequency table of behavior, outcomes of the Wilcoxon-signed rank tests, and details on the inter-rater reliability calculation.
Video S1. Elbow hold.
Video S2. Elbow touch.
Video S3. Hand grab.
Video S4. Hand touch.
Video S5. Head move.
Video S6. Head touch.
Video S7. Hold.
Video S8. Nosewipe.
Video S9. Self-scratch.
Video S10. Torso.
Video S11. Video example of a grooming handclasp initiated with shaping behavior.
Video S12. Video example of a grooming handclasp initiated with gestural communication.
Interactive Figure S13. Interactive version of Figure 2.
Supplementary Information
Subject demography of Group 2 at Chimfunshi Wildlife Orphanage on 01-07-2019.
Ethogram of behavior, with reference to supplementary videos of behaviors.
Occurrence of all ethogram behaviors in pre-handclasp (PH) and matched-control (MC) periods, as well as initiation of regular grooming bouts (IRG). Reported is the total number of instances and between brackets the number of instances divided by the number of bouts.
Results of the paired Wilcoxon signed rank test of behavior frequency between the PH and MC periods.
Inter-rater reliability assessment
To establish inter-rater reliability, two additional coders (JB and AM) analyzed the PHs and MCs of 20% (N = 19) of the GHC bouts. Additionally, ZG coded this subset for a second time to compare to her first coding one year prior.
Due to the continuous nature of the behavioral scoring and the importance of observers not only agreeing on the presence of (often rare) behaviors but also the order in which they occurred, the usual Cohen’s Kappa approach was not feasible. We calculated inter-rater reliability as follows (see also attached data for each rater’s coding and an overview):
If raters both scored the same behavior in the same order at the same time (within 1 second) it was noted as an “agree”, if either of the raters scored a behavior the other one did not score it was a “disagree”. As an informed estimation for the total amount of behaviors per video we used the sum of the instances of agree and disagree. To correct for the probability of two raters agreeing on a behavior by chance, we multiplied the probability of scoring a particular behavior (1/11, the amount of different behaviors) by the probability of scoring it for a certain individual (0.5).
The final calculation of the reliability score combined this probability of agreement based on chance (POA) with the relative observer agreement (ROA, defined as agreed behaviors/total amount of behaviors), in the form of . For instance, 50 agrees out of 70 total behaviors leads to a reliability score of
. For this study, all dyads showed inter-rater reliability scores above 0.80 (ZG_2-ZG_1 = 0.89; JB-ZG_1 = 0.81; AM-ZG_1 = 0.82; JB-AM = 0.81). Furthermore, each rater also coded who they thought initiated the bout, and inter-rater reliability on initiator was calculated in a similar manner (with POA being 0.5) and was above 0.90 for all raters (JB-ZG_1 = 0.89; all other dyads = 1).
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to the Chimfunshi Wildlife Orphanage Trust for their ongoing support of our research endeavors. In particular we wish to thank the staff of Chimfunshi, in particular manager Innocent Mulenga, as well as Jake Brooker for his assistance during data collection and inter-rater-reliability.
Footnotes
(ZG: zgoldsborough{at}outlook.com; AMS: A.M.Schel{at}uu.nl)