Abstract
Sexual selection is considered the major driver for the evolution of manifold sex differences. However, the eco-evolutionary dynamics of sexual selection and their role for a population’s adaptive potential to respond to environmental change have only recently been explored. Theory predicts that sexual selection promotes adaptation at a low demographic cost only if net selection is stronger on males compared to females. We used a comparative approach to show that net selection is indeed stronger in males in species prone to intense sexual selection. Given that both sexes share the vast majority of their genes, our findings corroborate the notion that the genome is often confronted with a more stressful environment when expressed in males. Collectively, our study supports a long-standing key assumption required for sexual selection to bolster adaptation, and intense sexual selection may therefore enable some species to track environmental change more efficiently.
One sentence summary Comparative study finds support for stronger net selection in males.
Introduction
For almost a century researchers have gathered compelling evidence that sexual selection (i.e. selection arising from competition for mating partners and/or their gametes) constitutes the ultimate evolutionary force generating sexual dimorphism in a multitude of reproductive characters and life-history traits (Andersson 1994; Clutton-Brock 2007). Despite this outstanding progress, we are just beginning to understand the eco-evolutionary dynamics of sexual selection in terms of its impact on demography and adaptive potential of a population (Svensson 2019). Traditionally, sexual selection has often been considered to impair population growth and constrain the adaptation to changing environments as a consequence of inter- and intra-locus sexual conflict (Arnqvist & Rowe 2005). More recently, however, both theoretical and empirical work suggest that sexual selection can facilitate how populations cope with environmental change (Lorch et al. 2003; Candolin & Heuschele 2008; Holman & Kokko 2013; Martínez-Ruiz & Knell 2017; Martinossi-Allibert et al. 2019). This latter school of thought is based on two main assumptions. First, sexual and natural selection need to be aligned, meaning that sexual selection favors alleles that also improve fecundity and survival − a process mediated by condition-dependence of sexually selected traits (Rowe & Houle 1996). Such an alignment is expected to manifest in a positive genetic correlation between male and female fitness components, which has already been documented across animal taxa (Poissant et al. 2010), though there is also evidence for negative cross-sex genetic correlations indicating the presence of intra-locus sexual conflict in some species (Chippindale et al. 2001). Moreover, there is also solid comparative and meta-analytic evidence supporting that the expression of pre− and post−copulatory sexual traits depends on the overall condition of the male (Cotton et al. 2004; Macartney et al. 2019) implying that sexual selection may not only favor the evolution of prominent secondary sexual traits but also traits that confer health and vigor (Jennions et al. 2001), and therefore eventually purges deleterious alleles.
In contrast to the solid support for an alignment of sexual and natural selection in many species, we still know very little on whether the second key assumption for sexual selection to empower evolutionary adaptation is generally fulfilled across a broad range of taxa. That is that sexual selection enforces natural selection only if it gives rise to stronger net selection (defined as the sum of genome-wide selection against deleterious alleles) on males compared to females. In a landmark synthesis paper, Whitlock and Agrawal (2009) explore the effect of sex-specific selection on the population’s mutation load, i.e. the overall reduction of absolute fitness due to deleterious alleles in a population. They expanded the fundamental work of Haldane (1937) by relaxing the assumption of random mating to demonstrate that the mutation load L is expected to be where μ is the mutation rate from good to bad alleles and s is the average selection coefficient against deleterious alleles of females (sf) and males. Hence, the mutation load is reduced whenever sf < s, which arises if net selection is stronger on males compared to females. Given that the population’s productivity is typically governed by female fecundity, a population with stronger net selection on males can purge its mutation load and adapt faster to a new environment with a lowered demographic cost and thereby reducing its extinction risk. In other words, females benefit from being part of a gene pool that is purified primarily through stronger selection on males (Whitlock & Agrawal 2009).
There is ample empirical evidence that typically (though not always) males undergo stronger sexual selection whereas females are primarily exposed to fecundity selection (Janicke et al. 2016) as predicted by Bateman’s principle (Bateman 1948). However, our knowledge on whether such stronger sexual selection on males eventually translates into stronger net selection relative to females is still limited and equivocal (Whitlock & Agrawal 2009; Hendry et al. 2018). This lack of evidence stems at least partially from difficulties in quantifying the strength of net selection in a framework that also allows comparisons among sexes and species. Potentially the most promising metric to contrast the strength of net selection across contexts is the mean-standardized variance in fitness, which is often expressed as the coefficient of variation (CV). Previous comparative studies provided evidence that the phenotypic variance in fitness (‘opportunity for selection’ (Crow 1958)) is typically larger in males compared to females, suggesting that the upper opportunity for net selection is stronger in males (Janicke et al. 2016). However, it has been questioned whether the phenotypic variance in fitness provides a good proxy for the strength of net selection because environmental variation can substantially inflate this variance, which limits its explanatory power with respect to evolutionary responses and complicates the comparison across contexts and studies (Whitlock & Agrawal 2009). To overcome this problem, researchers have advocated to use the genetic rather than the phenotypic variance in fitness as a proxy for the strength of net selection (Jones 2009; Hendry et al. 2018). This is also because the genetic variance in relative fitness corresponds to the rate of increase in mean fitness that results from selection on allele frequencies (Fisher 1930). Therefore, the mean-standardized genetic variance (CVG) in fitness provides a highly diagnostic metric to quantify the strength of net selection, but it has to our knowledge not yet been subjected to a systematic and global test for an overall sex difference.
Here we addressed this key aspect of sexual selection theory by using a comparative approach to test whether net selection is generally stronger on males across a broad taxonomic range. Specifically, we ran a systematic literature search and compiled 101 pairwise estimates of male and female genetic variances for two main fitness components – reproductive success and lifespan – from a total of 26 species. Applying phylogenetically-informed comparative analyses we tested (i) whether phenotypic variances are aligned to genetic variances as assumed by the phenotypic gambit (Grafen 1991) and (ii) whether genetic variances show consistent sex differences with the prediction that males show larger genetic variance in reproductive success but not lifespan. According to the so-called genic capture hypothesis, virtually every deleterious allele impairs the condition of an organism, which is defined as the pool of acquired resources that can be allocated into different fitness components such as survival and reproductive success (Rowe & Houle 1996). As a consequence, all fitness components are expected to be condition-dependent, i.e. to positively covary with condition because it captures the overall genetic quality of a given organism. Importantly, if sexual selection operates on males, reproductive success in males does not only depends on gamete production and post-zygotic investment (as predicted to constitute the primary determinants in females), but also on the outcome of pre- and post-copulatory mate choice and mate competition. Therefore, a deleterious allele with its negative effect on condition is predicted to have a disproportional higher impact on reproductive success in males compared to females. This heightened condition dependence of male performance (Rowe & Houle 1996) is expected to translate ultimately into a higher genetic variance in reproductive success in males (Whitlock & Agrawal 2009; Hendry et al. 2018). By contrast, lifespan is considered to be unaffected by the outcome of sexual selection except in the relatively rare event of mortal combats for access to mates, mate harassment or indirectly by shaping life-history strategies (Bonduriansky et al. 2008). If correct, a deleterious allele is therefore expected to have a similar effect on lifespan in males and females so that we predict if anything, a much weaker male bias for genetic variance in lifespan. In this context, we further explored the role of sexual selection for generating the hypothesized sex differences in net selection. For this purpose we contrasted socially monogamous and polygamous species, with the prediction that a male bias in genetic variance for reproductive success is primarily prevalent in polygamous species where sexual selection is most likely to be stronger compared to monogamous species (Shuster & Wade 2003).
Material and Methods
Literature Search
We ran a systematic literature search in order to obtain an unbiased sample of coefficients of genetic variation for two major fitness components: reproductive success and lifespan. Specifically, we screened the ISI Web of Science Core Collection database (Clarivate Analytics) on 2nd of August 2019 (for search terms see Supplementary Material). This search yielded 3793 records. Moreover, we screened previous synthesis articles on related questions (Poissant et al. 2010; Hendry et al. 2018; Connallon & Matthews 2019) for other primary studies by which we could identify one additional record (Wheelwright et al. 2014). Furthermore, we posted a request on ‘evoldir’ mailing list (http://life.mcmaster.ca/evoldir.html) and ResearchGate platform (https://www.researchgate.net/) for unpublished data, which resulted in one additional study (Abbott and Norden, in prep.). Finally, we added two published studies indicated by colleagues (Gay et al. 2011; Pélissié et al. 2012) and two of our own unpublished studies (Janicke et al., in prep.; Moiron et al., in prep.). After exclusion of duplicates and screening of abstracts, we checked a total of 203 records for eligibility based on three selection criteria. First, studies must report or include information to compute coefficients of genetic variation of reproductive success and/or lifespan. Second, studies must report genetic parameters for males and females both quantified under same conditions (i.e. same field populations or same experimental laboratory conditions). Third, we only included studies on animals simply because of the scarcity of data on genetic variances of male and female fitness components in plants. The final dataset included data from 55 primary studies (see Supporting Information for PRISMA diagram (Fig. S1) and reference list of all primary studies).
We note that primary studies differed in terms of how genetic variances were estimated including full-sib breeding designs (N = 3), half-sib breeding designs (N = 12), inbred lines (N = 17), pedigrees (N = 21) and twin studies (N = 2). While this may have induced potential biases in absolute values, we do not expect that the different experimental approaches led to a systematic bias in the sex difference of genetic variance. Therefore, we pooled all data obtained from different approaches in all analyses.
Data acquisition
For all primary studies we extracted four parameters for both sexes: (i) sample size, (ii) arithmetic mean, (iii) phenotypic variance, and (iv) genetic variance of reproductive success and/or lifespan. For 11 studies at least one of these parameters was not reported in the article. In these cases, we received the parameter estimates from the authors upon request or reanalyzed the raw data (either published together with the article or provided by the authors). We computed the coefficients of phenotypic and genetic variation (CVP and CVG, respectively) as the square root of the variance (i.e. the standard deviation) divided by the arithmetic mean, which makes this metric comparable across contexts and species. Note that CV of a given trait is often denoted as ‘evolvability’ (Houle 1992) and equals the square root of the opportunity for selection I, which is also frequently used to quantify the upper limit of the strength of selection (Jones 2009; Hendry et al. 2018).
In total we obtained 101 paired estimates of CVP and CVG for males and females, including 62 estimates for reproductive success and 39 estimates for lifespan. In all primary studies, reproductive success was measured as the number of offspring except for one study in which it was estimated as the number of grandchildren (Bolund et al. 2013). Lifespan was primarily measured as adult survival (i.e. excluding mortality until reaching maturity; 33 out of 39 estimates) and in the few remaining cases as the age of last reproduction, reproductive lifespan (calculated as time between first and last reproduction), or total lifespan (including juvenile mortality).
Phylogeny and Mating System Classification
The 55 primary studies encompass a total of 26 animal species with an overrepresentation of insects (N = 12) and birds (N = 7). In order to account for any source of phylogenetic non-independence we reconstructed the phylogeny of all sampled species (Fig. S2). Firstly, we retrieved pairwise estimates of divergence times from the TimeTree database (http://www.timetree.org/; (Kumar et al. 2017)). Secondly, we aged undated nodes on the basis of divergence times of neighboring nodes applying the branch length adjuster (BLADJ) algorithm (Webb et al. 2008). Finally, we used the resulting distance matrix to compute a phylogeny, using the unweighted pair group method with arithmetic mean (UPGMA) algorithm implemented in MEGA (https://www.megasoftware.net/; (Kumar et al. 2018)) and transformed it into the Newick format for further analysis.
To explore the role of sexual selection in generating sex differences in phenotypic and genetic variances, we used published information in the scientific literature on the social mating system as a proxy for the strength of sexual selection. Specifically, we distinguished between socially monogamous (N = 6) and polygamous species (N = 20; including polygynous and polygynandrous species). In strictly monogamous species the variance in reproductive success is expected to be identical for males and females. However strict monogamy is rather rare in animals (Lukas & Clutton-Brock 2013) and most of our sampled monogamous species are birds, which are described as socially monogamous rather than genetically monogamous because all of them show at least some degree of extra-pair paternity (Brouwer & Griffith 2019). Moreover, the occurrence of partial polygyny in some sampled bird species rendered their classification problematic. In these problematic cases, we searched the literature for estimates of the proportion of polygynous males in the studied population and only considered those species with less than 10 % of polygynous males as socially monogamous (Table S1). Hence, even though sexual selection is likely to operate also in most socially monogamous species, we assume that the strength of pre- and post-copulatory sexual selection is generally higher in polygamous compared to monogamous species (Shuster & Wade 2003). We examined the sensitivity of our classification criteria to different thresholds that have been previously used by other authors and found that the identity of monogamous and polygamous species remained when applying a 5% (Moller 1986) or a 15% threshold (Dunn et al. 2001). Nevertheless, we acknowledge that our classification is an oversimplification of a continuum in the strength of sexual selection across species in nature and we stress that all provided tests for correlations with the social mating system are exploratory, especially given the underrepresentation of monogamous species and their taxonomically uneven distribution in our dataset.
Statistical Analyses
Statistical analyses were carried out in two steps. First, we examined the key assumption of the ‘phenotypic gambit’ by testing whether estimates of phenotypic variance predict the estimated genetic variance. For this we computed the Pearson correlation coefficient r, testing the relationship between CVP and CVG for both sexes and the two fitness components separately. In addition, we tested whether the sex bias in CVP translates into a sex bias in CVG by correlating the coefficient of variation ratio lnCVR (Nakagawa et al. 2015), which refers to the ln-transformed ratio of male CV to female CV, with positive values indicating a male bias. The analyses on the phenotypic gambit were motivated from a methodological perspective and we did not expect that inter-specific variation in the difference between CVP and CVG can be explained by a shared phylogenetic history. However, for completeness, we also ran correlations on phylogenetic independent contrasts (PICs; computed using the ape R-package (version 5.4.1) in R (Paradis & Schliep 2019)) to test whether our findings were robust when accounting for potential phylogenetic non-independence. We report Pearson’s correlation coefficients r for normally distributed data and Spearman’s ρ if assumptions of normality were violated.
Second, we tested the hypothesis that net selection is stronger on males by testing for a male bias in CVP and CVG. Specifically, we ran Phylogenetic General Linear Mixed-Effects Models (PGLMMs) with CVP or CVG as the response variable, and sex as a fixed effect. To account for the paired data structure, we added an observation identifier as a random effect. Moreover, all models included a study identifier and the phylogeny (transformed into a correlation matrix) as random effects to account for statistical non-independence arising from shared study design or phylogenetic history, respectively. Note that the latter also accounts for the non-independence of estimates obtained from the same species as some studies estimated genetic variances from distinct field populations (Fox et al. 2004) or different experimental treatments under laboratory conditions such as food stress (Holman & Jacomb 2017) and temperature stress (Berger et al. 2014). In an additional series of PGLMMs we tested whether our proxy of sexual selection explained inter-specific variation in the sex-differences of CVP or CVG by adding mating system and its interaction with sex as fixed effects to the models. Finally, given that primary studies varied in the empirical approach used to quantify CVP and CVG, we also used PGLMMs to test whether study type (23 field studies versus 32 laboratory studies) represented a methodological determinant of the observed sex-differences in CVP and CVG.
All PGLMMs were ran with the MCMCglmm R-package (version 2.2.9) (Hadfield 2010), using uninformative priors (V = 1, nu = 0.002) and an effective sample size of 20000 (number of iterations = 11000000, burn-in = 1000000, thinning interval = 500). We computed the proportion of variance explained by fixed factors (‘marginal R2’) (Nakagawa & Schielzeth 2013). In addition, we quantified the phylogenetic signal as the phylogenetic heritability H2 (i.e., proportional variance in CVP or CVG explained by species identity), which is equivalent to Pagel’s ((de Villemereuil & Nakagawa 2014).
In a previous study testing for sex-specific phenotypic variances in reproductive success (Janicke et al. 2016), we ran formal meta-analyses using lnCVR as the tested effect size (Nakagawa et al. 2015). This is potentially a more powerful approach for comparing phenotypic variances but rendered unsuitable when comparing genetic variances. This is because the computation of the sampling variance of lnCVR is a function of the sample size of the sampled population and the point estimate of lnCVR (Nakagawa et al. 2015). However, genetic variances are estimates from statistical models and notorious for being estimated with low precision (i.e. have large confidence intervals). Therefore, using a meta-analytic approach for genetic variances using lnCVR as an effect size leads to overconfident estimation of the global effect size and is therefore likely to result in type-II-errors. However, to allow comparison with the previous meta-analysis, we report the outcome of phylogenetic meta-analyses on phenotypic variances using lnCVR in the Supplementary Material (Table S2), which largely reflects the results on the point estimates of CVP from PGLMMs.
Results
We found that the phenotypic coefficient of variation (CVP) of reproductive success does not predict the genetic coefficient of variation (CVG) in either males (raw estimate: r = 0.18, N = 62, P = 0.160; Phylogenetic Independent Contrasts (PICs): r = 0.20, N = 61, P = 0.126) or females (raw estimate: r = 0.08, N = 62, P = 0.557; PICs: r = 0.06, N = 61, P = 0.623; Fig. S3). In contrast, we detected a significant positive correlation between CVP and CVG for lifespan in males (raw estimate: r = 0.45, N = 39, P = 0.004; PICs: ρ = 0.60, N = 38, P < 0.001) and females (raw estimate: r = 0.45, N = 39, P = 0.005; PICs: ρ = 0.40, N = 38, P = 0.014). Despite these distinct findings for both fitness components, we found that the sex bias in CVP and CVG quantified as lnCVR was positively correlated for reproductive success (raw estimate: r = 0.41, N = 62, P < 0.001; PICs: ρ = 0.47, N = 61, P < 0.001) and lifespan (raw estimate: r = 0.37, N = 39, P = 0.020; PICs: ρ = 0.34, N = 38, P = 0.039).
Most importantly, CVP of reproductive success was generally larger in males compared to females, which translated into a male bias in CVG, with sex explaining 6 % and 4 % of the observed variance, respectively (Table 1; Fig. 1A-B and Fig. 2). Remarkably, this sex difference could be detected in polygamous but not monogamous species, which manifested in a significant sex by mating system interaction (Table 1; Fig. 1 and Fig. 2). Contrary to the results for reproductive success, we did not observe consistent sex differences in CVP and CVG for lifespan (Table 1; Fig. 1C-D2). Finally, study type did not predict the sex difference in CVP and CVG neither for reproductive success nor lifespan (Table S3).
Discussion
Males and females share the vast majority of their genome but are often subject to fundamentally different selection pressures, which is predicted to impact the demography and the adaptive potential of a population when facing environmental change (Whitlock & Agrawal 2009; Holman & Kokko 2013; Svensson 2019). In line with sexual selection theory, our study provides first comparative evidence that genome-wide selection is generally stronger on males compared to females. More specifically, our results have two major implications. First, phenotypic variance of lifespan, but not reproductive success, is aligned to genetic variance, suggesting that the phenotypic gambit does not hold for the latter. Therefore, the opportunity for selection measured as the phenotypic variance in reproductive success is a poor predictor for the strength of net selection. Despite this, the sex difference in phenotypic variance is positively correlated with the sex difference in genetic variance in both fitness components. As a consequence, our second major finding is that the previously observed male bias in the phenotypic opportunity for selection (Janicke et al. 2016) is also reflected in an overall higher male genetic variance in reproductive success. Importantly, this male bias can only be detected in polygamous species, in which sexual selection is likely to be stronger compared to monogamous species (Shuster & Wade 2003). Hence, pre- and post-copulatory competition and/or mate choice seem to magnify the material that selection acts on in a sex-specific manner. In contrast, phenotypic and genetic variances in lifespan do not show sex differences in either polygamous or monogamous species. The sample size for lifespan (NEstimates = 39; NSpecies = 16) was smaller compared to reproductive success (NEstimates = 62; NSpecies = 21) meaning that we had less statistical power to detect a sex difference in phenotypic and genetic variances for lifespan. However, we did not observe any consistent trend for lifespan and our data suggest that even if there was a sex difference in genetic variance in lifespan, its effect size would be considerably smaller compared to reproductive success. Thus, we conclude that while sexual selection may promote sex differences in mean lifespan at an evolutionary scale (Lemaitre et al. 2020), our results indicate that it does not generate sex-specific genetic variances in this fitness component. Hence, the strength of selection on survival appears to be similar among males and females.
Despite the strong sex difference in genetic variation of reproductive success that we demonstrated, our results on the effect of the social mating system need to be considered with caution. This is because of the underrepresentation of monogamous species in our dataset and because of only two independent evolutionary changes from polygamy to monogamy in our phylogeny. Moreover, our binary classification of the mating system into socially monogamous and polygamous species fails to capture the continuum in the strength of sexual selection across the sampled taxa, likely limiting its explanatory power as a predictor variable. Ideally, one would use a standardized continuous estimate for the strength of sexual selection allowing inter- and intra-specific comparisons such as Bateman metrics (Arnold 1994; Jones 2009). Unfortunately, such data are currently only accessible for a small fraction of the sampled species (i.e. 19% based on a recent database by (Janicke & Morrow 2018)), which renders those measures as additional predictors for the strength of sexual selection unavailable at the current state of knowledge. Therefore, we argue that mating system is currently the most reliable predictor for the potential of sexual selection of the sampled species.
In essence, our findings provide support for the prediction that sexual selection promotes stronger net selection on males compared to females. We conclude that the key assumption required for sexual selection to assist natural selection and thereby to accelerate the adaptation to changing environments is often fulfilled in nature. Stronger net selection on males implies that populations may purge deleterious alleles across the genome primarily at the expense of males and thus at a low demographic cost (Agrawal 2001; Siller 2001). This has important eco-evolutionary consequences because stronger net selection on males will not only bolster local adaptation but will also reduce extinction risk when populations are coping with challenging environmental conditions (Lumley et al. 2015). Therefore, our findings support the idea that sexual selection can play a pivotal role in evolutionary rescue (Candolin & Heuschele 2008; Holman & Kokko 2013; Svensson 2019) and are in line with a recent meta-analysis providing compelling evidence that sexual selection increases non-sexual fitness (Cally et al. 2019). Interestingly, environmental stress has repeatedly been found to elevate the effect of deleterious mutations and thereby increase genetic variation in fitness-related traits (Rowinski & Rogell 2017). Thus we extend the sexes-as-environments analogy (Rice & Chippindale 2001) to say that an almost identical genome is expressed in a more stressful male environment versus a relatively more benign female environment.
Whether stronger net selection on males eventually promotes adaptation to a new environment, or even contributes to the evolution and maintenance of sexual over asexual reproduction (Agrawal 2001; Siller 2001), will also depend on another important aspect of the genetic architecture of male and female fitness components: the cross-sex genetic covariance. Specifically, only if fitness in both sexes is condition-dependent (i.e. positively affected by the amount of acquired resources) and therefore largely governed by a similar set of genes, will sexual selection on males purge deleterious alleles in females and thereby facilitate adaptation (Whitlock & Agrawal 2009). Empirical tests for an inter-specific genetic correlation of fitness revealed mixed results including examples of highly negative correlations indicating intense intra-locus sexual conflict (Foerster et al. 2007; Poissant et al. 2010). Only a small fraction of the primary studies included in our analysis reported cross-sex genetic correlations, but exploratory analyses of this subset (Supplementary Information) support an earlier finding of highly positive genetic correlations for lifespan with no consistent pattern for reproductive success (Hendry et al. 2018). Further work on the genetic covariance between male and female fitness components is needed to evaluate the overall potential of sexual selection to facilitate or constrain the adaptation to changing environments. Specifically, how genetic variances and covariances of male and female fitness components change with ecological conditions is largely unknown (Delcourt et al. 2009; Berger et al. 2014) though such knowledge is crucial to predict evolutionary trajectories when populations face environmental change. Moreover, for some taxa, sexual selection has been found to increase extinction risk (Doherty et al. 2003; Le Galliard et al. 2005) potentially as a consequence of intense sexual conflict but the quantitative genetics of male and female fitness of those species remain mostly unexplored.
Despite the detected sex differences and the effect of the social mating system, a large fraction of the intra- and inter-specific variance in CVG remained unexplained (Table 1). This is potentially, at least in part, because genetic variances are often estimated with low precision, which may have introduced substantial noise into our analyses. Besides that, we suspect that another part of the unexplained variation stems from environmental effects influencing genetic (co)variances (i.e., genotype by environmental interactions) (Rowinski & Rogell 2017), which limits comparisons of studies conducted under different experimental conditions. Moreover, there are also methodological differences between primary studies, which may have contributed to the unexplained variation in CVG. This includes the application of different breeding designs used to quantify genetic variances (i.e. pedigrees, half-sib/full-sib breeding, parent-offspring regressions, inbred lines), differences in the analytical approaches (latent-scale versus data-scale estimates of genetic variances), and disparity in the measurement of reproductive success (annual versus lifetime reproductive success; see Material and Methods). While all these sources of uncontrolled variation are likely to have introduced noise into our dataset, we are not aware of any systematic biases that they might have created. Finally, our study covers a broad taxonomic range spanning flatworms, mollusks, arthropods, and vertebrates but is based on relatively few species. This is admittedly a limitation of our study but at the same time illustrates the clear need for more quantitative genetic studies measuring genetic variation of fitness components in both sexes.
Collectively, our analysis highlights the role that sexual selection has for generating sex differences in the strength of net selection. However, we have just started to understand the eco-evolutionary consequences of sexual selection in terms of its impact on demography and the adaptive potential of populations to cope with changing environments.
Funding
LW and TJ were funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG grant number: JA 2653/2-1). TJ received funds from the Centre national de la recherche scientifique (CNRS). MM was funded by a Marie Curie Individual Fellowship (PLASTIC TERN; grant agreement number: 793550). EHM was funded by a Royal Society University Research Fellowship and the Swedish Research Council (grant number: 2019-03567).
Supplementary Information
Supplementary Analysis
Cross-sex genetic correlations
Theory predicting a positive effect of sexual selection on adaptation is based on the additional assumption that alleles favored in males must also be beneficial in females, which, if true, may manifest in a positive genetic correlation between male and female fitness. Estimates of such cross-sex genetic correlations are scarce and a previous comparative study included only few correlation of fitness components relative to morphological, physiological and behavioral traits (Poissant et al. 2010).
In an explorative meta-analysis, we tested for an overall positive genetic cross-sex correlation in reproductive success and lifespan based on estimates provided in the primary studies obtained from our systematic literature search (Fig. S1). Only few primary studies reported cross-sex correlations, which led to small samples sizes for reproductive success (N = 25) and lifespan (N = 19), and is also reflected in low numbers of sampled species (reproductive success: NSpecies = 10; lifespan: NSpecies = 5).
We used phylogenetically informed meta-analyses by running PGLMMs with the MCMCglmm R package (using same specifications as for models reported in the main text), in which Pearson correlation coefficients (r) were defined as response variable weighted by the inverse of its variance. Models included study identifier and phylogeny as random effect terms. We found support for positive cross-sex genetic correlation for lifespan (PGLMM: global r = 0.548, 95% CI = 0.185 – 0.918, P = 0.020) but not for reproductive success (PGLMM: global r = 0.024, 95% CI = −0.242 – 0.244, P = 0.816). In conclusion, our limited data does not support a positive cross-sex genetic correlation for reproductive success.
Supplementary Data
Mating system classification
References – Mating system classification
References
Supplementary Data
Search terms and list of primary studies
Systematic literature search was carried out in the Web of Science Core Collection (Clarivate) using the following search terms:
TS=((sex* OR (male AND female) OR (man AND woman) OR “sex diff*” OR “gender diff*” OR sex-specific OR intersex* OR inter-sex* OR cross-sex* OR “across sex*” OR “between sex*” OR “between-sex*” OR “sex-limited”) AND (fitness OR “reproductive success” OR survival OR longevity OR lifespan OR “life span”) AND (“intra-locus sexual conflict” OR “intralocus sexual conflict” OR “sexually antagonistic genetic” OR “genetic co*” OR heritability OR “genetic varia*” OR “quantitative genetics” OR “genetic architecture” OR evolvability))
The list below encompasses all 52 published primary studies included in the comparative analyses. It does not comprise three unpublished studies that have also been included (Abbott, J., and A. Norden. in prep.; Janicke, T., E. Chapuis, S. Meconcelli, N. Bonel, and P. David. in prep.; Moiron M, Charmantier A, Bouwhuis S. in prep.).
References
Acknowledgements
We are very grateful to all authors of the primary studies and in particular those providing additional information and/or data including Jessica Abbott, Mats Björklund, Sandra Bouwhuis, Ryan G. Calsbeek, Julie Collet, David Hosken, Zenobia Lewis, Jacob Moorad, Tom Tregenza and Felix Zajitschek. Moreover, we thank Patrice David, Shinichi Nakagawa, Klaus Reinhardt, Holger Schielzeth, Céline Teplitsky and Pierre de Villemereuil for discussions and statistical advice.
Footnotes
Statement of authorship: TJ conceived the study. LW, MM, EHM, and TJ designed the study. LW and TJ collected the data and performed the statistical analyses. LW and TJ wrote the paper with the help of MM and EHM.
Data accessibility statement: After acceptance of the article, all data will be uploaded on the Dryad digital repository (https://v1.datadryad.org/).
Competing interests: All authors declare no competing interests.