Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

Repetition effects in action selection reflect effector- but not hemisphere-specific coding

View ORCID ProfileChristian Seegelke, Carolin Schonard, Tobias Heed
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.07.16.450393
Christian Seegelke
1Biopsychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Sport Sciences, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
2Center for Cognitive Interaction Technology (CITEC), Bielefeld, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Christian Seegelke
  • For correspondence: christian.seegelke@uni-bielefeld.de
Carolin Schonard
1Biopsychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Sport Sciences, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Tobias Heed
1Biopsychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Faculty of Psychology and Sport Sciences, Bielefeld University, Bielefeld, Germany
2Center for Cognitive Interaction Technology (CITEC), Bielefeld, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Data/Code
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

Action choices are influenced by future and recent past action states. For example, when performing two actions in succession, response times (RT) to initiate the second action are reduced when the same hand is used. These findings suggest the existence of effector-specific processing for action selection. However, given that each hand is primarily controlled by the contralateral hemisphere, the RT benefit might actually reflect body side or hemisphere-specific rather than effector-specific repetition effects. Here, participants performed two consecutive movements, each with a hand or a foot, in one of two directions. Direction was specified in an egocentric reference frame (inward, outward) or in an allocentric reference frame (left, right). Successive actions were initiated faster when the same limb (e.g., left hand - left hand), but not when the other limb of the same body side (e.g., left foot - left hand) executed the second action. The same-limb advantage was evident even when the two movements involved different directions, whether specified egocentrically or allocentrically. Corroborating evidence from computational modeling lends support to the claim that repetition effects in action selection reflect persistent changes in baseline activity within neural populations that encode effector-specific action plans.

Introduction

Interacting with our environment continually requires us to decide between multiple potential actions. Consider the simple decision of choosing an effector to open a door. Intuitively, one might choose between the left and right hand. However, when our hands are occupied, for example because we are carrying a stack of heavy folders, we can flexibly come up with another effector option and open the door with an elbow or knee.

Neurophysiologically inspired models such as the Affordance Competition Hypothesis regard the process of action selection as a competitive decision process between multiple, simultaneously prepared actions (Cisek, 2007; Cisek & Kalaska, 2010). In this view, a distributed network of posterior parietal and premotor cortical regions continuously specifies all currently possible actions in parallel (Andersen & Buneo, 2002; Cisek & Kalaska, 2005; Cui & Andersen, 2007; Hoshi & Tanji, 2007). Once the neural activity for one of the potential actions passes a threshold, the selected response is initiated.

Several factors can bias this decision process: In the context of choosing between different limbs (i.e. left vs. right hand), hand choice is influenced by handedness (Bryden et al., 2000), future task demands (Coelho et al., 2014), biomechanical costs (Oliveira et al., 2010; Schweighofer et al., 2015), spatial reference frames (Bakker et al., 2018) and the recent motor history, also termed motor hysteresis (Rostoft et al., 2002; Schütz & Schack, 2020; Schweighofer et al., 2015; Valyear et al., 2019). Motor history effects manifest as a bias of hand choice for a new action towards the effector that was used for the previous action, especially when the biomechanical costs of responding for alternative effectors, such as the two hands, is similar. The prevailing explanation for the existence of motor hysteresis has been that it is computationally more efficient to reuse and slightly modify a previously used action plan than to create a new plan for each single action (Rosenbaum et al., 2007; Schütz & Schack, 2013).

Recent behavioral and neuroimaging data have provided support for this ‘planning efficiency account’ (Valyear & Frey, 2014, 2015). In these studies, participants performed two actions in succession, referred to as prime and probe actions, respectively. These actions involved grasping a tool with one hand and rotating it in one of two directions – either inward or outward with respect to the body midline – each with a distinct grasp. The study design comprised four conditions that reflected the relationship between prime and probe actions: Participants repeated the same action (identical repeat, IR); they used the same hand but executed different rotations and grasps (hand repeat, HR); they repeated the same rotation and grasp, but used different hands in the two consecutive actions (grasp repeat, GR), or used both a different hand and rotation/grasp (no repeat, NR). Response time (RT) to initiate probe action was shorter when the same hand was repeated even when different grasps were required (i.e., in condition HR). In contrast, no RT advantage was observed when participants repeated the same grasp with the opposite hand. The repetition effect was accompanied by reduced fMRI activity in bilateral posterior parietal cortex (PPC), specifically in the intraparietal and superior parietal cortices. A reduction of neural activity is frequently observed when stimulus or task features are repeated and are referred to as repetition suppression effects (Desimone, 1996; Grill-Spector & Malach, 2001; Heed et al., 2016). The authors concluded that action plans are computed independently for each hand and that, thus, PPC processes action plans separately for each effector.

These findings potentially support effector-based accounts of PPC organization which have suggested that the PPC is subdivided into lower-dimensional, effector-specific subspaces that encode motor goals in relation to a specific effector (e.g., Andersen & Cui, 2009; Vesia & Crawford, 2012). However, given the primarily contralateral organization of PPC (Chang et al., 2008; Filimon, 2010; Rice et al., 2007; e.g., Valyear & Frey, 2015), differential fMRI activation for the left and right hands might reflect effects related to body and brain symmetry rather than effector specificity (Medendorp & Heed, 2019). Indeed, fMRI activity patterns were strikingly similar in many parts of PPC when participants prepared to point to visual targets, whether the hand or foot of one body side had to execute the pointing movement (Heed et al., 2011; Leoné et al., 2014). In these studies, effector-specific hand and foot activity was evident only in anterior parts of PPC.

Moreover, repetition suppression was evident across hand and foot in caudal parts of PPC, suggesting shared neural resources for the two limbs in the context of motor planning (Heed et al., 2016). In sum, these findings suggested that PPC comprises both effector-specific and effector-unspecific action processing and have led to the proposal that PPC may better be viewed as organized according to functional than anatomical criteria (Medendorp & Heed, 2019). In this framework, repetition effects – both behavioral and neural (Valyear & Frey, 2014, 2015) cannot be conclusively attributed strictly to effector-specificity, but may instead be at least partly due to executing consecutive actions with limbs of one body side or hemisphere.

To test this alternative interpretation, we modified the paradigm of Valyear and Frey (2014) to include action sequences in which the second of two consecutive actions involved repeating movements with a different effector of the same body side. Participants executed actions with hands and feet rather than just the hands, as had been done in previous studies. Following previous work, we will term the first action “ prime” and the second “ probe” from here on.

The movements in our tasks were hand and foot movements of both body sides in one of two directions: towards the body midline (inward) and away from the body midline (outward). Each action was cued by means of different shapes that were visually displayed on a computer screen (see Figure 1A; Valyear & Frey, 2014). The probe comprised the same or different effector type (hand vs. foot), the same or different body side (left vs. right), and the same or different movement direction (inward vs. outward). Thus, our task comprised a 2⨯ 2 ⨯ 2 factorial design that allowed dissociating effector-specific from body side/ hemispheric-specific repetition effects (Figure 2): Effector Type (repeat, no repeat) x Body Side (repeat, no repeat) x Movement Direction (repeat, no repeat).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 1.

Experimental task and procedure. Participants performed two successive movements (a prime and a probe) separated by a 0.5 sec delay interval. Movements were performed in one of two directions (inward, outward) with either (i.e., left, right) hand or foot. A Example shape-action assignment. Actions were defined through arbitrary rules by means of different shapes visually displayed on a computer screen. Each shape corresponded to one specific action and shape-action associations were randomized across participants. Movement direction was instructed in an egocentric reference frame (i.e., in- vs. outward movement) in Experiment 1 and in an allocentric reference frame (i.e., left- vs. rightward movement) in Experiment 2. White arrows indicate movement direction. B Example trial sequence with event timing.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 2.

Experimental conditions of Experiment 1 (egocentric instruction). The task comprised a 2 Effector Type (Repeat, No repeat) x 2 Body Side (Repeat, No Repeat) x 2 Movement Direction (Repeat, No Repeat) design that yielded eight conditions. The left and right columns illustrate conditions in which the same (e.g., hand – hand) or different effector type (e.g., hand – foot) was used for prime and probe action, respectively. The upper and lower two rows show conditions in which the same (e.g., left – left) or different body side (e.g., left – right) was used, respectively. Conditions with same-colored (blue – blue) and different-colored arrows (blue – yellow) indicate conditions in which movements were performed in the same (e.g., outward – outward) or different (e.g., outward – inward) direction. Conditions are illustrated for a left hand outward movement as prime action but apply analogously for all other actions.

When effector type, body side, and movement direction were all repeated (identical repeat, IR), the visual cue symbol of prime and probe was also identical. For all other comparisons, prime and probe involved different symbols, because they involved a change in at least one factor and, thus, were cued by an individual shape. The IR visual cue repetition makes it difficult to interpret comparisons with the IR condition, because we cannot exclude that effects stem from visual processing of the cue and/or associative rule retrieval. In fact, the IR condition has yielded by far the fastest RT in previous studies (Valyear & Frey, 2014). The visual confound, therefore, impedes interpretation of main effects and interactions of our factorial design. For this reason, we used as baseline the condition in which neither effector type, body side, nor movement direction were repeated (No repeat, NR); such prime-probe sequences always comprised different symbol cues. To assess repetition effects, we compared RT in the NR condition with all other conditions in dedicated contrasts.

If previously reported behavioral repetition effects reflected effector-specific representations, then our adapted design should evoke faster RT only for probe events that involved repeating the same effector type on the same body side (repeat effector type & repeat body side). Conversely, if previous repetition results were actually due more generally to hemisphere or body side-specific processing and not to reusing the same effector, RT should also be faster for probe events that involved repeating the same body side without repeating the same effector type (body side & movement repeat; body side repeat). Finally, our experimental design allows testing whether repeating only movement direction (movement repeat) or effector type (hand vs. foot) affects motor planning (effector type & movement repeat; effector type repeat). Previous studies that involved only the hands were necessarily agnostic about this question, as it requires testing movements with different effector types, here, hand and foot.

Experiment 1: Egocentric task instruction

Methods

Participants

A previous study that involved only the two hands reported a significant repetition effect with N = 16 (Valyear & Frey, 2014). Because we suspected that repetition effects may be smaller when instructed movements implicate the same body side but not the same effector, we defined a larger target sample size of 20 participants.

We collected data from 20 participants (1 left hander; 15 female, 5 male; mean age = 23.40 years, SD = 6.39 years, range = 19 – 49 years; mean handedness score right handers = 100.00, handedness score left hander = -50; Dragovic, 2004). All participants were physically and neurologically healthy and reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision. They received course credit for their participation. The Bielefeld Ethics committee approved all experiments (Ethical Application Ref: 2017-114) and all participants provided written informed consent prior to participation.

Apparatus and stimuli

Participants sat on a chair in front of a 24” computer monitor (Dell UltraSharp U 2412M; 1920×1200 pixels; 60Hz refresh rate; height adjusted to eye level) at a distance of about 75 cm. Four square PVC blocks (width ⨯ length ⨯ height: 10 × 10 × 3 cm) with centrally embedded round pushbuttons of 7 cm diameter served as start locations for the hands and feet. The buttons were positioned such that the participants could comfortably place their hands and feet on them. The foot buttons were placed on the floor about 40 cm in front of the monitor and about 40 cm apart (center-to-center distance). The hand buttons were positioned 80 cm directly above the foot buttons on a small board. Thus, hand and foot buttons were positioned with identical distances with respect to the body and the vertical body midline.

Visual shape cues instructed the movements participants were to execute (see Figure 1A). They were colored white and sized 413 ⨯ 413 pixels. They were presented centrally on a black background (Figure 1A). Each shape defined one of eight response actions, as defined by the 3-factor design of the experiment: an inward or outward movement with respect to the body midline performed with the left or right hand or with the left or right foot. Stimulus-response assignment was randomized across participants. Stimulus presentation and response recording was controlled with Matlab (The MathWorks Version R2015; Natick, MA, USA) using the Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997). An optical motion capture system (Visualeyez II VZ4000v, Phoenix Technologies Inc, Vancouver, BC, Canada) recorded hand and foot kinematics at 250 Hz sampling rate. We placed infrared markers on the dorsal side of each hand and foot (distal end of the third metacarpal and third metatarsal, respectively).

Procedure

The trial sequence is illustrated in Figure 1B. At the start of each trial, participants depressed the four response buttons with their hands and feet. After an interval of 500 ms, the prime stimulus (one of the eight shapes) appeared on the screen for 2500 ms. Participants then performed the action instructed by the stimulus. After a fixed interval of 500 ms, the probe stimulus (again one of the eight shapes) appeared on the screen for 2500 ms and participants then performed the respective probe action. After an inter-trial interval of 2000 ms, participants could initiate the next trial by depressing the four buttons. Participants received written feedback on the screen if they did not complete the respective action within the 2500 ms stimulus display time (“ Too slow”) and if they released the wrong button, that is, a button of an effector that did not belong to the displayed cue (“ Wrong effector”). Participants were instructed to perform inward or outward movements with the respective effector at a comfortable speed. All 64 possible prime-probe combinations were presented once within a block in a pseudorandomized order, and participants completed 10 blocks for a total of 640 trials. Due to a technical error with motion data recording, four participants performed 11 rather than 12 blocks.

Participants performed 2-3 practice blocks prior to the experimental blocks until they reported to be sufficiently familiar with the stimulus-response associations. The trial structure was identical to the experimental trials except that only one stimulus was shown and participants performed only one action per trial. If participants selected the wrong effector, an error message indicating the correct response was displayed. Each stimulus was repeated 8 times within a block, yielding 64 trials per block. The entire experiment took about 3 hours to complete.

Data processing and analysis

We processed kinematic data using custom-written Matlab scripts. We interpolated missing data points using the spring metaphor method in the inpaint_nans function (D’Errico, 2021) and smoothed the data using a second-order Butterworth filter with a cut-off frequency of 6 Hz. We determined movement onset as the time point at which the vectorial velocity of a marker exceeded 50 mm/s and response time (RT) as the time between probe stimulus onset and movement onset. We excluded trials in which no data was recorded and trials in which trajectories could not be reconstructed due to too many missing data points (2.3%). We removed trials in which participants performed the wrong prime or probe action (8.8%) and trials in which RT was faster than 200ms or slower than 1500ms (1.2%). We also considered analyzing repetition effects on errors in action choice. However, trials in which participants committed an error during the probe phase were very rare (Wrong effector: 1.1%, Wrong Body Side: 1.3%, Wrong movement direction: 0%), and hence conclusions about this aspect of our data are problematic.

Statistical approach

We analyzed our data in R (R Development Core Team, 2020) using Bayesian regression models created in Stan (http://mc-stan.org/) and accessed with the package brms version 2.13.0 (Bürkner, 2017). We fitted a model for our dependent variable (RT) using the within-subject categorical variables Effector Type (repeat, no repeat), Body Side (repeat, no repeat), and Movement Direction (repeat, no repeat) as independent predictors. We set orthogonal contrasts using the set_sum_contrasts() command in afex version 0.28-0 (Singmann et al., 2020). The model was specified as RT ∼ Target Orientation * Target Direction * Target Distance + (1 | participant), thus capturing all main effects and interactions of our design. We included only random intercepts, because models did not reliably converge when random slopes were included. We used a shifted log-normal distribution to estimate RTs. To improve convergence and guard against overfitting, we specified mildly informative, conservative priors for population-level (i.e., fixed) effects. The estimation of parameters’ posterior distributions were obtained by Hamiltonian Monte-Carlo sampling with 4 chains, 1,000 sample warmup, and 11,000 iterations, and checked visually for convergence (high ESS and Rhat ≈ 1). To analyze and describe the posterior distributions, we report the median as a point-estimate of centrality and the 95% credible interval (CI) computed based on the highest-density interval (HDI) to characterize the uncertainty related to the estimation. As an index of the credibility of a given effect, we tested whether the HDI excluded a region of practical equivalence (ROPE) of +/-0.1 effect sizes around 0 (Kruschke, 2018; Makowski et al., 2019). If the HDI is completely outside the ROPE, the null hypothesis for this effect is rejected. If the ROPE completely covers the HDI (i.e., all most credible values of a parameter are inside the ROPE), the null hypothesis is accepted. This approach is conservative because it requires not only that the HDI exclude 0, but in addition that no overlap exists between the HDI and the ROPE interval.

Results

RT for probe actions was fastest when effector type, body side, and movement direction were all repeated (identical repeat, IR; median = 525 ms, 95% HDI [503, 547]) and slowest when none of the three tested criteria was repeated (no repeat, NR; median = 790 ms, 95% HDI [750, 831]). Compared with NR, RT was faster when effector type and body side were repeated (median = 716 ms, 95% HDI [681, 752]); recall that this factor combination reflects true effector-specificity, because the combination of repeated effector type and body side implies re-use of a given effector. In contrast, RT was similar when only body side but not effector was repeated (body side repeat & movement direction repeat: median = 780 ms, 95% HDI [741, 821]; body side repeat: median = 778 ms, 95% HDI [740, 819]; Figure 3).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 3.

Response times (ms) for probe actions as a function of effector type, body side, and movement repeat in Experiment 1. Areas show the posterior distributions of the estimated marginal means, black dots represent the medians and error bars the 95% highest density interval (HDI).

To distinguish between effector- and hemispheric-specific repetition effects, we calculated contrasts between the posterior distributions of NR and conditions that involved compared to NR (NR – IR: median difference = 265 ms, 95% HDI [244, 286]; 0% of HDI in ROPE). However, as mentioned previously, these differences cannot be separated from visual processing and associative rule retrieval. RT was also faster than NR when effector and body side were repeated (NR – EBSR, median difference = 74 ms, 95% HDI [60, 89]; 0% of HDI in ROPE). In contrast, comparisons of conditions that involved repetition of the same body side but not effector type did not yield any credible RT differences against NR (NR – BSMR: median difference = 9 ms, 95% HDI [-5, 24]; NR – BSR: median difference = 11 ms, 95% HDI [-3, 26]; Figure 4, rows 3 and 4) and 88% and 81% of the HDIs overlapped with the ROPE, respectively. In addition, comparison of conditions that did vs. did not include an effector-specific repeat were credibly different, suggesting a critical influence of repeating a repetition of the same effector type and/ or body side (i.e., IR, EBSR, BSMR, BSR; Figure 4 upper four rows). Repeating the action (IR) resulted in a substantial decrease of RT specific effector rather than just any limb of the given body side (BSMR – EBSR: median difference = 65 ms, 95% HDI [51, 79]; 0% of HDI in ROPE; BSR – EBSR: median difference = 63 ms, 95% HDI [49, 77]; 0% of HDI in ROPE; Figure 4 bottom two rows). These findings lend support to the view that repetition effects in effector choice reflect effector-specific rather than hemisphere-specific coding of action representation. Lastly, we did not find evidence for RT improvement when only the effector type was repeated (NR – EMR: median difference = 30 ms, 95% HDI [15, 44]; 17% of HDI in ROPE; NR – ER: median difference = 18 ms, 95% HDI [4, 33]; 57% of HDI in ROPE; Figure 4, rows 5 and 6) or when only movement direction was repeated (NR – MR: median difference = 16 ms, 95% HDI [2, 31]; 64% of HDI in ROPE; Figure 4 row seven).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 4.

Contrasts of posterior distributions in Experiment 1. Black dots represent the medians and error bars the 95% highest density interval (HDI). The grey shaded area indicates the region of practical equivalence (ROPE) of +/-0.1 effect sizes around 0 [-20, 20]. Portions of the distributions outside and inside the ROPE are shown in light blue and dark blue, respectively. NR = No repeat, IR = identical repeat, EBSR = effector type repeat and body side repeat, BSMR = body side repeat and movement direction repeat, BSR = body side repeat, EMR = effector type repeat and movement direction repeat, ER = effector type repeat, MR = movement direction repeat.

Experiment 2: Allocentric task instruction

In Experiment 1, repeating using the same effector repeatedly resulted in a substantial RT benefit even when the performed actions were different (e.g., left hand inward movement – left hand outward movement). In contrast, movements of two different effectors of one body side (e.g., left foot – left hand) did not yield a credible decrease in RT. These findings suggest that repetition effects in effector selection reflect effector-specific rather than hemispheric-specific action representations.

Movements of homologous limb pairs, such as the two hands, are more stable and accurate when they are symmetrical with respect to the sagittal body midline than when the two limbs have to coordinate non-symmetrically. This symmetry advantage is known as the “ egocentric constraint” . In contrast, movements of non-homologous limb pairs (e.g., one hand and one foot are more stable and accurate when the two limbs move in the same direction in extrinsic space, known as the “ allocentric constraint” (cf. Swinnen, 2002). In Experiment 1, participants were instructed to perform inward and outward movements, that is, instructions were formulated egocentrically. This choice of instructions may have penalized movements performed with non-homologous and, hence, prevented the expression of body side-specific repetition effects. To test this conjecture, we instructed participants allocentrically in Experiment 2: they were now asked to perform left- and rightward-directed movements in response to the visual stimuli.

Methods

Participants

20 individuals from Bielefeld University (1 left hander; 8 female, 12 male; mean age = 22.90 years, SD = 1.94 years, range = 20 – 28 years; mean handedness score right handers = 97.53, SD = 7.63, handedness score left hander = -43; Dragovic, 2004) participated in Experiment 2. None of them had participated in Experiment 1. All participants were physically and neurologically healthy, reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and received course credit in exchange for their participation.

Apparatus, stimuli, and procedure

The apparatus, stimuli, and procedure were identical to Experiment 1 except that we instructed and trained participants to perform leftward and rightward movements (allocentric response coding) as opposed to inward or outward movements (egocentric response coding) in response to the symbols.

Data processing and analyses

Data processing and analysis were analogous to Experiment 1. We excluded trials in which no data was recorded and trials in which trajectories could not be reconstructed due to too many missing data points (1.7%). We removed trials in which participants performed the wrong prime or probe action (9%) and trials in which RT was faster than 200ms or slower than 1500ms (1.6%). Trials in which participants committed an error during the probe phase were similarly rare as in Experiment 1 (Wrong effector: 1.0%, Wrong Body Side: 1.7%, Wrong movement direction: 1.0%), hence we also omitted analyzing repetition effects on action choice in Experiment 2.

Results

Results were remarkably similar to those of Experiment 1. RT for probe actions was fastest when effector type, body side, and movement direction were all repeated (identical repeat, IR; median = 525 ms, 95% HDI [497, 553]). Compared to NR (median = 757 ms, 95% HDI [708, 809]), RT as again faster when effector type and body side were repeated (median = 697 ms, 95% HDI [654, 743]). In contrast, RT was similar when only body side but not effector was repeated (body side repeat & movement direction repeat: median = 754 ms, 95% HDI [705, 805]; body side repeat: median = 754 ms, 95% HDI [704, 804]; Figure 5).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 5.

Response times (ms) for probe actions as a function of effector type, body side, and movement repeat in Experiment 2. Areas show the posterior distributions of the estimated marginal means, black dots represent the medians and error bars the 95% highest density interval (HDI).

Again, we calculated contrasts between the posterior distributions of NR and conditions that involved repetition of the same effector type and/ or body side to distinguish between effector- and hemispheric-specific repetition effects (i.e., IR, EBSR, BSMR, BSR; Figure 6 upper four rows). RT was substantially faster in IR (median difference = 232 ms, 95% HDI [209, 257]; 0% of HDI in ROPE), but as in Experiment 1, this contrast may be strongly affected by factors that are not of interest in our study. RT was also faster when a specific effector was repeated (EBSR, median difference = 60 ms, 95% HDI [46, 74]; 0% of HDI in ROPE). In contrast, there were no credible RT differences for repetition of body side (NR – BSMR: median difference = 3 ms, 95% HDI [-11, 16]; NR – BSR: median difference = 3 ms, 95% HDI [-11, 16]), and the 95% HDI of the respective credible difference distributions were fully inside the ROPE. In addition, comparison of specific effector repetition (i.e., repeating effector and body side) and repeating only body side confirmed credible differences between these conditions (BSMR – EBSR: median difference = 56 ms, 95% HDI [43, 71]; 0% of HDI in ROPE; BSR – EBSR: median difference = 56 ms, 95% HDI [44, 71]; 0% of HDI in ROPE; Figure 6, bottom two rows). Finally, there were no credible effects for repeating only effector type but not body side (NR – EMR: median difference = 14 ms, 95% HDI [0, 27]; 71% of HDI in ROPE; NR – ER: median difference = 17 ms, 95% HDI [4, 30]; 60% of HDI in ROPE; Figure 6 rows five and six) and when repeating only movement direction (NR – MR: median difference = -12 ms, 95% HDI [-26, 1]; 76% of HDI in ROPE; Figure 6 row seven). In sum, these results confirm the findings of Experiment 1, and the two experiments together reinforce the view that repetition effects in action selection originate from effector-specific processes.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 6.

Contrasts of posterior distributions in Experiment 2. Black dots represent the medians and error bars the 95% highest density interval (HDI). The grey shaded area indicates the region of practical equivalence (ROPE) of +/-0.1 effect sizes around 0 [-20, 20]. Portions of the distributions outside and inside the ROPE are shown in light blue and dark blue, respectively. NR = No repeat, IR = identical repeat, EBSR = effector type repeat and body side repeat, BSMR = body side repeat and movement direction repeat, BSR = body side repeat, EMR = effector type repeat and movement direction repeat, ER = effector type repeat, MR = movement direction repeat.

Modeling

We employed computational modeling to examine the source of the (effector-specific) repetition effects in action selection. We analyzed reaction times and accuracy data of both experiments (n = 40) within the framework of drift-diffusion models (DDM; (Ratcliff & McKoon, 2008). According to DDM, action selection in binary decision tasks can be regarded as an accumulation-to-threshold mechanism in which activity related to the different response options builds up as a function of the evidence for or against those choices until one of two opposing decision bounds is reached. These models can explain both accuracy patterns and response latencies across a variety of different tasks (Brown & Heathcote, 2008; Forstmann et al., 2016; Gold & Shadlen, 2007; Ratcliff & McKoon, 2008; Smith & Ratcliff, 2004; Usher & McClelland, 2001). Moreover, neural activity in the posterior parietal cortex seems to reflect the accumulated evidence in perceptual decision-making tasks (Gold & Shadlen, 2007). One explanation is that repetition effects are a consequence of persistent changes in baseline activity within neurons encoding effector-specific action plans. Accordingly, sustained elevated neural activity for recently selected effectors would result in faster action selection (Valyear et al., 2019; Valyear & Frey, 2015). Within the DDM framework, this can be modeled as a shift of the starting point of the evidence accumulation process towards the repeated action in repeat trials as compared to non-repeat trials (starting point model). The shift reduces the distance from the starting point to one of the decision bounds in effector-repeat trials, so that less evidence accumulation is required until the bound is reached and, accordingly, less time will pass until the associated movement is initiated.

Repetition effects may also emerge through a higher drift rate in repeat over non-repeat trials (drift rate model). The drift rate is a measure of the speed of information processing and higher values indicate a faster rate of evidence accumulation such that the bound is reached faster. Changes in either parameter can explain sequential effects (cf. Ratcliff et al., 2016).

In addition to these two model variants, we fitted a model in which both starting point and drift rate varied per repeat and non-repeat condition (full model). Finally, we considered a model in which starting point and drift rate were fixed across all conditions to assess whether any of the two parameters of interest is necessary to explain behavior (baseline model).

We fitted each of the four model variants with the maximum-likelihood approach implemented in the software fast-dm (Voss et al., 2015; Voss & Voss, 2007). All model variants included the parameters drift rate v, starting point zr, accumulation threshold a, and non-decision time t0. The four model variants differed in terms of which parameters were allowed to vary across Effector Type, Body Side, and Movement Direction conditions: In the full model, drift rate and starting point were allowed to vary simultaneously. In the drift rate model, only the drift rate, and in the starting point model, only the starting point could vary between conditions. In the baseline model, no parameter was allowed to differ between conditions. In all model variants, inter-trial variabilities of drift rate and starting point were fixed at zero, because simulations show weak recovery for these parameters, especially in smaller data sets (van Ravenzwaaij & Oberauer, 2009). However, we included variability of the starting point st0, as this can counteract data outliers (Lerche & Voss, 2016). We fitted each model to individual participants’ RT data for the probe stimuli. We only included trials with correct and incorrect movement responses to the probe stimulus that followed a correct response to the preceding prime stimulus. Otherwise, outlier criteria remained the same as in the main analysis. The models’ upper response thresholds were associated with a correct response and the lower threshold with any possible incorrect response option.

We compared the BIC/AIC weights of the individual model fits by converting the Bayesian information criterion (BIC) and Akaike information criterion (AIC) for each model (Wagenmakers & Farrell, 2004) to determine the best model in terms of goodness of fit and parameter parsimony. The starting point model provided the best fit, followed by the drift rate and full model, while the baseline model provided the worst fit (Table 1). Although BIC and AIC ordered drift rate and full models differently, the two criteria were consistent concerning the starting point and baseline model.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1.

BIC and AIC calculated for different DDM. Estimated mean values and 95% HDI using Bayesian regression (BIC/ AIC ∼ model variant + (1|participant)).

We further scrutinized the relevance of the drift rate parameter by comparing the model parameters of the full model. Individual drift rates were higher for the identical repeat (IR) condition compared to all other conditions (Figure 7A). Contrasts between the posterior distributions showed credible differences between NR and IR (mean difference = -1.34, 95% HDI [-1.61, -1.06]; 0% of HDI in ROPE; Figure 7C). However, this high drift rate is likely due to facilitated processing of perceptual features, given the previously discussed visual symbol repetition (Voss et al., 2004). Importantly, drift rate was similar for the effector repeat condition (EBSR) and the NR condition (mean difference = 0.16, 95% HDI [-0.15, 0.44]; 36% of HDI in ROPE; Figure 7C), indicating that repetition effects do not reflect a change in drift rate. In contrast, starting points were shifted towards the decision bound for the IR and the effector repeat (EBSR) condition (Figure 7B). Contrasts between the posterior distributions showed credible differences between NR and IR (mean difference = -0.35, 95% HDI [-0.38, -0.32]; 0% of HDI in ROPE) and between NR and EBSR (mean difference = -0.13, 95% HDI [-0.16, -0.10]; 0% of HDI in ROPE; Figure 7D). Thus, even if we allow free fitting of drift rate for each condition, this parameter does not account for repetition effects in motor performance. These results are in line with the superior fit of the starting point model and indicate that repetition effects likely arise due to a change in baseline activity that is exclusive to a specific effector.

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 7.

DDM parameter estimates. Posterior distributions of the estimated marginal means of drift rates (A) and starting points (B) for probe actions as a function of effector type, body side, and movement repeat calculated from the DDM (full model). (C,D) Contrasts of posterior distributions between the no repeat and the other conditions for drift rates (C) and starting points (D). Black dots represent the means and error bars the 95% highest density interval (HDI).The grey shaded area indicates the region of practical equivalence (ROPE) of +/-0.1 effect sizes around 0. Portions of the distributions outside and inside the ROPE are shown in light blue and dark blue, respectively.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to test whether repetition effects in action selection reflect effector- or hemisphere-specific action representations. Participants performed movements with hands and feet in one of two directions. Successive actions were initiated faster when the same effector was moved repeatedly (e.g., left hand – left hand), even when those actions involved different movement directions. Conversely, moving two effectors of the same body side (e.g., left foot – left hand), that is, a body side repetition, did not yield any significant RT benefit. This result pattern was evident irrespective of whether movement direction was instructed egocentrically (inward vs. outward) or allocentrically (left vs. right). These findings are consistent with the view that current limb choice is influenced by limb choice in the recent past, and that successive actions can be initiated faster when an effector is employed repeatedly (Valyear et al., 2019; Valyear & Frey, 2014), possibly through persistent changes in baseline activity of the neural assembly responsible for the given effector, as suggested by our computational modeling approach. Importantly, our findings confirm that repetition effects arise from effector-specific processing and do not support the alternative that repetition effects are based on common processing of effectors of the same body side, which are largely controlled by one hemisphere.

Multiple studies have demonstrated that previously performed actions can bias current actions. Such motor hysteresis effects have been observed across a variety of different tasks. Besides hand choice (Rostoft et al., 2002; Schütz & Schack, 2020; Schweighofer et al., 2015; Valyear et al., 2019), motor hysteresis is also known to influence grasp height (Cohen & Rosenbaum, 2004; Seegelke et al., 2013; Seegelke, 2015), grasp orientation (Dixon et al., 2012; Kent et al., 2009; Schütz et al., 2011), and reach trajectories (Jax & Rosenbaum, 2007, 2009) such that current actions tend to be more similar to previously executed actions.

The presence of such history effects is typically explained in terms of computational gains underlying action selection and specification. It is commonly assumed that it is computationally more efficient for the sensorimotor system to recycle motor parameters of previously specified action plans than to create them anew for every single movement (Mawase et al., 2018; Rosenbaum et al., 2007; Schütz & Schack, 2013). Consistent with this view, previous work has shown that actions are initiated faster when an effector is used repeatedly (Valyear et al., 2019; Valyear & Frey, 2014), and this RT benefit was accompanied by reduced fMRI activity in bilateral posterior parietal cortex (Valyear & Frey, 2015).

However, previous studies have consistently used homologous effectors, usually the hands, in their experiments. Thus, a non-excluded possibility so far had been that repetition effects do not originate from repeatedly using one specific effector but, instead, from a hemisphere-wide, or body-side-wide advantage, which would, of course, also benefit action repetitions of an identical effector.

Our present results support the previously insufficiently backed-up conclusion that repetition effects in limb choice paradigms reflect processing advantages at the level of specific effectors. In turn, our results do not provide any evidence for a role of processes that encompass an entire hemisphere or body side. This is because a reaction time benefit was only present when the same effector was moved twice, but not two different effectors of one body side were moved consecutively.

Our study, thus, suggests that repetition effects result from effector-specific processing, presumably in PPC (Andersen & Cui, 2009; Colby & Goldberg, 1999; Matelli & Luppino, 2001; Valyear & Frey, 2015; Vesia & Crawford, 2012). The concept of effector-specific PPC organization has mainly originated from neurophysiological studies in macaque monkeys that reported differential neuronal activity for hand vs. eye movements. For instance, the macaque lateral intraparietal area (LIP) is more active during saccade than reach planning, and the parietal reach region (PRR) shows the opposite activity pattern (Cui & Andersen, 2007; Quian Quiroga et al., 2006; Snyder et al., 1997). Several fMRI studies have revealed considerable overlap for saccade and reach planning in human PPC (Beurze et al., 2007; Levy et al., 2007), and these results have challenged strict effector-related accounts of PPC. Still, effector-specific activity for saccades vs. reaches could be decoded with multivoxel pattern analysis in several human PPC regions such as the superior parietal-occipital cortex (SPOC) and the posterior intraparietal sulcus (Gallivan et al., 2011).

Nevertheless, the reliance of effector-specificity accounts on the dissociation of hand and eye movements has been challenged (cf. Medendorp & Heed, 2019), based on the argument that eye and hand movement serve different, namely perceptual-exploratory vs. manipulatory, purposes. Studies that contrasted planning-related activity of eye, hand, and foot movements have produced a more differentiated picture of PPC organization (Heed et al., 2011; Heed et al., 2016; Leoné et al., 2014). Specifically, these studies have revealed a caudo-rostral gradient for eye vs. limb movements and a lateral-to-medial gradient for hand vs. eye vs. foot movements in anterior PPC, combined with mostly common activation patterns and voxel patterns for multiple effectors effectors in posterior, and differentiated activity in anterior PPC. Moreover, some anterior PPC regions appeared to code one effector against the others. Bases on these dichotomous representations, the authors proposed that these anterior regions probably play a role in effector selection, rather than representing one specific effector. The fact that the RS effects accompanying the RT benefit in effector repeat trials described by Valyear and Frey (2015) were also located in more anterior parts of PPC corroborates this interpretation.

According to the Affordance Competition Hypothesis (Cisek, 2007; Cisek & Kalaska, 2010; Pezzulo & Cisek, 2016), the brain simultaneously specifies multiple actions that are presently available in the environment and decides between them through continuous competition of the neural activity related to each action within a distributed fronto-parietal network. Once the neural activity for one of the potential actions reaches a threshold, its activity is further increased while the activity of competing actions is inhibited. This perspective is in line with computational models that conceptualize decision making as an accumulation-to-threshold mechanism in which activity related to different response options builds up as a function of the evidence for each choice until a threshold is reached (Brown & Heathcote, 2008; Forstmann et al., 2016; Gold & Shadlen, 2007; Ratcliff & McKoon, 2008; Smith & Ratcliff, 2004; Usher & McClelland, 2001). Our results from computational modeling further reinforce such an interpretation. Specifically, we found that the starting point parameter was shifted towards the decision bound specifically for the condition involving effector repetition (i.e., IR, EBSR). While the shift in starting point in combination with the higher drift rate in the IR condition might largely reflect facilitated perceptual processing due to symbol repetition, it cannot readily explain the starting point shift in the EBSR condition, which does not involve symbol repetition. Rather we contend that this finding fits the idea that effector-specific RT advantages would result from changes in activity within neural populations in anterior PPC that are distinctly dedicated to a given effector. Once an effector-specific population has been activated, neural activity that remains within this particular circuit would support the building of new activity, while alternative effectors would still be suppressed by the activity left-over from the previous movement. Accordingly, evidence for the recently selected effector would be closer to the threshold, yielding shorter RTs in effector repetition trials. Yet, it is obvious that this rather crude conceptualization lacks specifics as to why different movement specifications can easily arise within the pre-activated circuit, and why, for instance, lingering activity does not appear to favor repeating a specifically defined movement, such as a particular grasp or movement direction.

In this context, it is noteworthy that other interpretations of motor repetition effects have been put forward. For example, Dixon et al. (2012) showed that repetition effects in grasping were linked to object identity and persisted across intervening trials that involved different grasps, suggesting that repetition effects reflect memory retrieval processes (Weigelt et al., 2009). There is also evidence that hysteresis effects can transfer from one hand to the other (Dixon et al., 2012; van der Wel et al., 2007), indicative of an involvement of effector-independent representations in repetition effects (Schütz & Schack, 2015). Why such cross-hand advantages arise in some, but not other studies (such as in our present one), is currently unresolved. Finally, the time course of hysteresis effects appears to depend on the task specifics and can range from below one second (Jax & Rosenbaum, 2009) to several seconds (Dixon et al., 2012; Schütz & Schack, 2013; Valyear & Frey, 2014, 2015). Thus, it is possible that the repetition effects observed in different tasks reflect different underlying processes (see alsoMarinovic et al., 2017). It is conceivable, then, that the exclusiveness of effector-specific repetition effects identified in our present experiments may not generalize to all motor contexts.

An important aspect of our current results is the independence of the repetition effects we observed from the reference frames that specified the instructed movements in the two experiments. Although egocentric coding exhibits higher activity in parietofrontal cortex, and allocentric coding exhibits higher activity in early visual cortex, neural activity for allocentric and egocentric mechanisms for reach target coding also shows considerable overlap (Chen et al., 2014). Moreover, the repetition effects in the present study and previous work (Valyear & Frey, 2014, 2015) likely reflect effects related to effector choice and may, thus, not be directly linked to processes that involve coordinate transformations – a hypothesis that will require further exploration but has received some support from limb choice tasks in the tactile domain (Maij et al., 2020).

To summarize, we demonstrate that the successive re-use of an identical effector, but not of effectors that belong to the same body side, results in a substantial decrease in the time required for movement initiation. These findings are consistent with the view that current limb choice is influenced by choices in the recent past. They lend support to the widely held view that repetition effects in limb choice reflect effector-specific rather than hemisphere-specific coding.

Disclosures

No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors.

Author contributions

C.Se. and T.H. conceived and designed research; C.Se. performed experiments; C.Se and C.Sc. analyzed data; C.S., C.Sc., and T.H. interpreted results of experiments; C.Se. prepared figures; C.Se. drafted manuscript; C.Se., C. Sc., and T.H. edited and revised manuscript; C.S., C.Sc., and T.H. approved final version of manuscript.

Acknowledgments

We thank Tobias Oehler and Florian Grünendahl for help with data acquisition and Conrad Alting for help with programming of the experiments. Data and code for the present paper are available at the Open Science Framework website https://osf.io/h5zrb.

Footnotes

  • https://osf.io/h5zrb/

References

  1. ↵
    Andersen, R. A., & Buneo, C. A. (2002). Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 25, 189–220. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142922
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  2. ↵
    Andersen, R. A., & Cui, H. (2009). Intention, action planning, and decision making in parietal-frontal circuits. Neuron, 63(5), 568–583. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.028
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  3. ↵
    Bakker, R. S., Selen, L. P. J., & Medendorp, W. P [W. Pieter] (2018). Reference frames in the decisions of hand choice. Journal of Neurophysiology, 119(5), 1809–1817. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00738.2017
    OpenUrl
  4. ↵
    Beurze, S. M [S. M.], Lange, F. P. de, Toni, I [I.], & Medendorp, W. P [W. P.] (2007). Integration of target and effector information in the human brain during reach planning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97(1), 188–199. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00456.2006
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  5. ↵
    Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  6. ↵
    Brown, S. D., & Heathcote, A. (2008). The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation. Cognitive Psychology, 57(3), 153–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.12.002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  7. ↵
    Bryden, P. J., Pryde, K. M., & Roy, E. A. (2000). A performance measure of the degree of hand preference. Brain and Cognition, 44(3), 402–414. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1999.1201
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  8. ↵
    Bürkner, P.-C. (2017). brms : An R Package for Bayesian Multilevel Models Using Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 80(1). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v080.i01
  9. ↵
    Chang, S. W. C., Dickinson, A. R., & Snyder, L. H. (2008). Limb-specific representation for reaching in the posterior parietal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 28(24), 6128–6140. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1442-08.2008
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    Chen, Y., Monaco, S., Byrne, P., Yan, X., Henriques, D. Y. P., & Crawford, J. D. (2014). Allocentric versus egocentric representation of remembered reach targets in human cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 34(37), 12515–12526. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1445-14.2014
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    Cisek, P. (2007). Cortical mechanisms of action selection: The affordance competition hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 362(1485), 1585–1599. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2054
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    Cisek, P., & Kalaska, J. F. (2005). Neural correlates of reaching decisions in dorsal premotor cortex: Specification of multiple direction choices and final selection of action. Neuron, 45(5), 801–814. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.027
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  13. ↵
    Cisek, P., & Kalaska, J. F. (2010). Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33, 269–298. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.13 5409
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  14. ↵
    Coelho, C. J., Studenka, B. E., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2014). End-state comfort trumps handedness in object manipulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 40(2), 718–730. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034990
    OpenUrl
  15. ↵
    Cohen, R. G., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2004). Where grasps are made reveals how grasps are planned: Generation and recall of motor plans. Experimental Brain Research, 157(4), 486–495. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-1862-9
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  16. ↵
    Colby, C. L., & Goldberg, M. E. (1999). Space and attention in parietal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 319–349.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  17. ↵
    Cui, H., & Andersen, R. A. (2007). Posterior parietal cortex encodes autonomously selected motor plans. Neuron, 56(3), 552–559. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.031
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  18. ↵
    D’Errico, J. (2021). inpaint_nans [Computer software]. https://www.mathworks.com/matlabcentral/fileexchange/4551-inpaint_nans
  19. ↵
    Desimone, R. (1996). Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93, 13494–13499.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    Dixon, P., McAnsh, S., & Read, L. (2012). Repetition effects in grasping. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology = Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale, 66(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026192
    OpenUrl
  21. ↵
    Dragovic, M. (2004). Towards an improved measure of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory: A one-factor congeneric measurement model using confirmatory factor analysis. Laterality, 9(4), 411–419. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500342000248
    OpenUrlPubMed
  22. ↵
    Filimon, F. (2010). Human cortical control of hand movements: Parietofrontal networks for reaching, grasping, and pointing. The Neuroscientist : A Review Journal Bringing Neurobiology, Neurology and Psychiatry, 16(4), 388–407. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858410375468
    OpenUrl
  23. ↵
    Forstmann, B. U., Ratcliff, R [R.], & Wagenmakers, E.-J [E-J] (2016). Sequential Sampling Models in Cognitive Neuroscience: Advantages, Applications, and Extensions. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 641–666. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033645
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    Gallivan, J. P., McLean, D. A., Smith, F. W., & Culham, J. C. (2011). Decoding effector-dependent and effector-independent movement intentions from human parieto-frontal brain activity. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 31(47), 17149–17168. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1058-11.2011
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    Gold, J. I., & Shadlen, M. N. (2007). The neural basis of decision making. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 535–574. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  26. ↵
    Grill-Spector, K., & Malach, R. (2001). fMR-adaptation: a tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. Acta Psychologica, 107(1-3), 293–321. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(01)00019-1
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  27. ↵
    Heed, T., Beurze, S. M [Sabine M.], Toni, I [Ivan], Röder, B., & Medendorp, W. P [W. Pieter] (2011). Functional rather than effector-specific organization of human posterior parietal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 31(8), 3066–3076. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4370-10.2011
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    Heed, T., Leone, F. T. M., Toni, I [Ivan], & Medendorp, W. P [W. Pieter] (2016). Functional versus effector-specific organization of the human posterior parietal cortex: Revisited. Journal of Neurophysiology, 116(4), 1885–1899. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00312.2014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    Hoshi, E., & Tanji, J. (2007). Distinctions between dorsal and ventral premotor areas: Anatomical connectivity and functional properties. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17(2), 234–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2007.02.003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  30. ↵
    Jax, S. A., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2007). Hand path priming in manual obstacle avoidance: Evidence that the dorsal stream does not only control visually guided actions in real time. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 33(2), 425–441. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.425
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  31. ↵
    Jax, S. A., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2009). Hand path priming in manual obstacle avoidance: Rapid decay of dorsal stream information. Neuropsychologia, 47(6), 1573–1577. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008. 05.019
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Kent, S. W., Wilson, A. D., Plumb, M. S., Williams, J. H. G., & Mon-Williams, M. (2009). Immediate movement history influences reach-to-grasp action selection in children and adults. Journal of Motor Behavior, 41(1), 10–15.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    Kruschke, J. K. (2018). Rejecting or accepting parameter values in Bayesian estimation. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(2), 270–280. doi:10.1177/2515245918771304
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  34. ↵
    Leoné, F. T. M., Heed, T., Toni, I [Ivan], & Medendorp, W. P [W. Pieter] (2014). Understanding effector selectivity in human posterior parietal cortex by combining information patterns and activation measures. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 34(21), 7102–7112. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5242-13.2014
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    Lerche, V., & Voss, A. (2016). Model Complexity in Diffusion Modeling: Benefits of Making the Model More Parsimonious. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1324. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01324
    OpenUrl
  36. ↵
    Levy, I., Schluppeck, D., Heeger, D. J., & Glimcher, P. W. (2007). Specificity of human cortical areas for reaches and saccades. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 27(17), 4687–4696. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0459-07.2007
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    Maij, F., Seegelke, C., Medendorp, W. P [W. Pieter], & Heed, T. (2020). External location of touch is constructed post-hoc based on limb choice. ELife, 9. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.57804
  38. ↵
    Makowski, D., Ben-Shachar, M. S., Chen, S. H. A., & Lüdecke, D. (2019). Indices of Effect Existence and Significance in the Bayesian Framework. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2767. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02767
    OpenUrl
  39. ↵
    Marinovic, W., Poh, E., Rugy, A. de, & Carroll, T. J. (2017). Action history influences subsequent movement via two distinct processes. ELife, 6, e26713. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.26713.001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Matelli, M., & Luppino, G. (2001). Parietofrontal circuits for action and space perception in the macaque monkey. NeuroImage, 14(1 Pt 2), S27–32. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0835
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  41. ↵
    Mawase, F., Lopez, D., Celnik, P. A., & Haith, A. M. (2018). Movement Repetition Facilitates Response Preparation. Cell Reports, 24(4), 801–808. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2018.06.097
    OpenUrl
  42. ↵
    Medendorp, W. P [W. Pieter], & Heed, T. (2019). State estimation in posterior parietal cortex: Distinct poles of environmental and bodily states. Progress in Neurobiology, 183, 101691. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2019.101691
    OpenUrl
  43. ↵
    Oliveira, F. T. P., Diedrichsen, J., Verstynen, T., Duque, J., & Ivry, R. B. (2010). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of posterior parietal cortex affects decisions of hand choice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(41), 17751–17756. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1006223107
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    Pezzulo, G., & Cisek, P. (2016). Navigating the Affordance Landscape: Feedback Control as a Process Model of Behavior and Cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(6), 414–424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.013
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Quian Quiroga, R., Snyder, L. H., Batista, A. P [Aaron P.], Cui, H., & Andersen, R. A. (2006). Movement intention is better predicted than attention in the posterior parietal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 26(13), 3615–3620. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3468-05.2006
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    R Development Core Team. (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (Version 3.6.3) [Computer software]. Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/
  47. ↵
    Ratcliff, R [Roger], & McKoon, G. (2008). The diffusion decision model: Theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. Neural Computation, 20(4), 873–922. https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.2008.12-06-420
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  48. ↵
    Ratcliff, R [Roger], Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D., & McKoon, G. (2016). Diffusion Decision Model: Current Issues and History. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(4), 260–281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.01.007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    Rice, N. J., Tunik, E., Cross, E. S., & Grafton, S. T. (2007). On-line grasp control is mediated by the contralateral hemisphere. Brain Research, 1175, 76–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.08.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  50. ↵
    Rosenbaum, D. A., Cohen, R. G., Jax, S. A., Weiss, D. J., & van der Wel, R. (2007). The problem of serial order in behavior: Lashley’s legacy. Human Movement Science, 26(4), 525–554. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2007.04.001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  51. ↵
    Rostoft, M., Sigmundsson, H., Whiting, H., & Ingvaldsen, R. (2002). Dynamics of hand preference in 4 year-old children. Behavioural Brain Research, 132(1), 59–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00415-6
    OpenUrlPubMed
  52. ↵
    Schütz, C., & Schack, T. (2013). Influence of mechanical load on sequential effects. Experimental Brain Research, 228(4), 445–455. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3576-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    Schütz, C., & Schack, T. (2015). Movement plans for posture selection do not transfer across hands. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1358. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01358
    OpenUrl
  54. ↵
    Schütz, C., & Schack, T. (2020). Shifts of the point-of-change can be attributed to a lower mechanical cost of motor execution. Experimental Brain Research, 238(5), 1097–1105. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-020-05781-3
    OpenUrl
  55. ↵
    Schütz, C., Weigelt, M., Odekerken, D., Klein-Soetebier, T., & Schack, T. (2011). Motor control strategies in a continuous task space. Motor Control, 15(3), 321–341. https://doi.org/10.1123/mcj.15.3.321
    OpenUrlPubMed
  56. ↵
    Schweighofer, N., Xiao, Y., Kim, S., Yoshioka, T., Gordon, J., & Osu, R. (2015). Effort, success, and nonuse determine arm choice. Journal of Neurophysiology, 114(1), 551–559. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00593.2014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    Seegelke, C. (2015). Observing end-state comfort favorable actions does not modulate action plan recall. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 45. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00045
    OpenUrl
  58. ↵
    Seegelke, C., Hughes, C. M. L., & Schack, T. (2013). Simulating my own or others action plans?--Motor representations, not visual representations are recalled in motor memory. PloS One, 8(12), e84662. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084662
    OpenUrl
  59. ↵
    Singmann, H., Bolker, B., Wesfall, J., & Aust Frederik. (2020). afex: Analysis of Factorial Experiments (Version R package version 0.16-1) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=afex
  60. ↵
    Smith, P. L., & Ratcliff, R [Roger] (2004). Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions. Trends in Neurosciences, 27(3), 161–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2004.01.006
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  61. ↵
    Snyder, L. H., Batista, A. P [A. P.], & Andersen, R. A. (1997). Coding of intention in the posterior parietal cortex. Nature, 386, 167–170.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  62. ↵
    Swinnen, S. P. (2002). Intermanual coordination: From behavioural principles to neural-network interactions. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 3(5), 348–359. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn807
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    Usher, M., & McClelland, J. L. (2001). The time course of perceptual choice: The leaky, competing accumulator model. Psychological Review, 108(3), 550–592. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.108.3.550
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  64. ↵
    Valyear, K. F., Fitzpatrick, A. M., & Dundon, N. M. (2019). Now and then: Hand choice is influenced by recent action history. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(1), 305–314. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1510-1
    OpenUrl
  65. ↵
    Valyear, K. F., & Frey, S. H. (2014). Hand selection for object grasping is influenced by recent motor history. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(2), 566–573. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0504-2
    OpenUrl
  66. ↵
    Valyear, K. F., & Frey, S. H. (2015). Human posterior parietal cortex mediates hand-specific planning. NeuroImage, 114, 226–238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.05 8
    OpenUrl
  67. ↵
    van der Wel, R. P. R. D., Fleckenstein, R. M., Jax, S. A., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2007). Hand path priming in manual obstacle avoidance: Evidence for abstract spatiotemporal forms in human motor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1117–1126. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.5.1117
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    van Ravenzwaaij, D., & Oberauer, K. (2009). How to use the diffusion model: Parameter recovery of three methods: EZ, fast-dm, and DMAT. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 53(6), 463–473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2009.09.004
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  69. ↵
    Vesia, M., & Crawford, J. D. (2012). Specialization of reach function in human posterior parietal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 221(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3158-9
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  70. ↵
    Voss, A., Rothermund, K., & Voss, J. (2004). Interpreting the parameters of the diffusion model: An empirical validation. Memory & Cognition, 32(7), 1206–1220. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196893
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  71. ↵
    Voss, A., & Voss, J. (2007). Fast-dm: A free program for efficient diffusion model analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 39(4), 767–775. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03192967
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  72. ↵
    Voss, A., Voss, J., & Lerche, V. (2015). Assessing cognitive processes with diffusion model analyses: A tutorial based on fast-dm-30. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 336. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00336
    OpenUrl
  73. ↵
    Wagenmakers, E.-J [Eric-Jan], & Farrell, S. (2004). Aic model selection using Akaike weights. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 192–196. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03206482
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  74. ↵
    Weigelt, M., Rosenbaum, D. A., Huelshorst, S., & Schack, T. (2009). Moving and memorizing: Motor planning modulates the recency effect in serial and free recall. Acta Psychologica, 132(1), 68–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.06.005
    OpenUrlPubMed
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted July 16, 2021.
Download PDF
Data/Code
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Repetition effects in action selection reflect effector- but not hemisphere-specific coding
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Repetition effects in action selection reflect effector- but not hemisphere-specific coding
Christian Seegelke, Carolin Schonard, Tobias Heed
bioRxiv 2021.07.16.450393; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.07.16.450393
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo LinkedIn logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Repetition effects in action selection reflect effector- but not hemisphere-specific coding
Christian Seegelke, Carolin Schonard, Tobias Heed
bioRxiv 2021.07.16.450393; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.07.16.450393

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Neuroscience
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (3686)
  • Biochemistry (7781)
  • Bioengineering (5672)
  • Bioinformatics (21253)
  • Biophysics (10565)
  • Cancer Biology (8165)
  • Cell Biology (11916)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (6744)
  • Ecology (10391)
  • Epidemiology (2065)
  • Evolutionary Biology (13847)
  • Genetics (9698)
  • Genomics (13060)
  • Immunology (8131)
  • Microbiology (19973)
  • Molecular Biology (7839)
  • Neuroscience (42997)
  • Paleontology (318)
  • Pathology (1276)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (2257)
  • Physiology (3350)
  • Plant Biology (7217)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (1309)
  • Synthetic Biology (2000)
  • Systems Biology (5529)
  • Zoology (1126)