Summary
Dopaminergic inputs to basal amygdala (BA) instruct learning of motivational salience. Here, we investigated the dynamics of dopamine release and downstream signaling during multiple salient events occurring within tens of seconds. We established in vitro and in vivo real-time tracking and manipulation of cAMP – a key intracellular plasticity signal downstream of dopamine receptor activation. Optogenetically-evoked release of dopamine drove proportional increases in cAMP in almost all BA glutamatergic neurons, suggesting widespread actions of dopamine across neurons preferring positive or negative valence. These cAMP responses decayed more slowly than dopamine release, potentially extending the window of plasticity. cAMP levels accumulated following direct photostimulation of cAMP but not repeated stimulation of dopamine axons, due to potent depression of dopamine release. cAMP and protein kinase A (PKA) responses to repeated appetitive or aversive stimuli also exhibited pronounced depression. Thus, history-dependent dynamics of dopamine and cAMP may regulate learning of temporally clustered, salient stimuli.
Introduction
The basolateral amygdala is critical for learning the valence of initially neutral sensory cues and guiding decisions to approach or avoid such cues (O’Neill et al., 2018; Zhang and Li, 2018). The neural plasticity that occurs during this associative learning involves dopamine, norepinephrine and other neuromodulators that bind to receptors on target cells to regulate cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP) levels and synaptic plasticity (Bissière et al., 2003; Johansen et al., 2014; Tronson et al., 2006). During associative plasticity, the precise timing of dopamine-dependent cAMP signals is important (Handler et al., 2019; Steinberg et al., 2013; Yagishita et al., 2014). However, we understand much less about the dynamics of dopamine-evoked cAMP signals, which also depend on regulatory mechanisms that vary across states and across neuron types.
An increase in release of dopamine from VTA neurons projecting to the basal amygdala (BA) occurs during salient events of both positive and negative valence (Lutas et al., 2019). Despite receiving this common dopamine signal, individual BA glutamatergic neurons preferentially respond to either appetitive or aversive events (Lutas et al., 2019; O’Neill et al., 2018; Zhang and Li, 2018). These observations led us to hypothesize that dopamine release broadly facilitates plasticity by increasing cAMP levels in both appetitive- and aversive-preferring BA neurons during salient outcomes. Meanwhile, learning of the valence of the outcome associated with a cue may be achieved via other mechanisms involving calcium-dependent processes driven by distinct pathways relaying information about sensory cues and about positive and negative outcomes (Correia and Goosens, 2016).
We developed an approach to optogenetically control dopamine release from VTA axons in BA (VTADA→BA) while simultaneously visualizing calcium or cAMP dynamics in BA glutamatergic neurons. To first test the sufficiency and timing-dependence of dopamine-evoked cAMP in instructing stimulus salience, we performed longitudinal imaging of calcium responses in BA neurons in awake mice across sessions in which one of two visual stimuli is paired with optogenetic stimulation of VTADA→BA axons. We found that optogenetic stimulation of dopamine axons was sufficient to drive the acquisition of stimulus responses in a subset of BA neurons across days, similar to the acquisition of responses to cues paired with natural appetitive and aversive outcomes (Lutas et al., 2019). We then investigated the immediate consequences of dopamine receptor activation on BA glutamatergic neurons by tracking intracellular cAMP production using a genetically-encoded fluorescent biosensor (Tewson et al., 2016). We found that exogenous dopamine and transient photostimulation of VTADA→BA axons elevated cAMP in most BA glutamatergic neurons. The proportion of BA neurons with dopamine-evoked increases in cAMP scaled with dopamine concentration, and all BA neurons showed elevated cAMP in the presence of high levels of exogenous dopamine. These findings suggest that while dopamine release does not determine the encoded valence of the conditioned stimulus, it may determine its salience by controlling the number of stimulus-responsive BA neurons.
We also found that transient dopamine release triggered cAMP increases lasting for over 30 seconds. This duration was primarily determined by cell-autonomous processes rather than by prolonged elevations in extracellular dopamine, since we could replicate the cAMP decay kinetics by circumventing dopamine receptors using a photoactivatable adenylate cyclase. Further, we found that presynaptic depression at VTADA→BA terminals limits the duration of elevated cAMP when dopamine release events are spaced tens (but not hundreds) of seconds apart. This depression in cAMP signaling was observed for repeated optogenetically-evoked dopamine release as well as for repeated delivery of unexpected palatable food or aversive tail shock. We also observed potent depression at the next stage in the signaling pathway, protein kinase A (PKA) activity, in vivo in response to repeated salient events. Thus, the learned salience associated with novel stimuli could be regulated by the magnitude of dopamine-evoked cAMP within the BA. Together, our results suggest that dopamine-evoked cAMP initiates a widespread permissive plasticity window that is correlated with stimulus salience and engages other intracellular signals in BA to control the acquisition of sensory responses to cues surrounding motivationally salient outcomes.
Results
Pairing visual cues with optogenetic stimulation of VTADA→BA axons results in cue-evoked calcium responses in BA neurons
Neurons in the basolateral amygdala acquire responses to previously meaningless visual cues when these cues are paired with salient appetitive or aversive outcomes (Lutas et al., 2019; O’Neill et al., 2018; Schoenbaum et al., 1999). To determine whether pairing dopamine release with an arbitrary visual stimulus was sufficient for BA neurons to acquire responses to this stimulus, we combined two-photon calcium imaging of the same individual BA neurons across several sessions with photostimulation of VTADA→BA axon terminals (Supplementary Figure 1A,B). We used two simple visual stimuli differing only in the orientation of a drifting grating. One of the stimuli (“Cue A”) was paired with photostimulation of VTADA→BA axon terminals, which occurred 200 ms after the visual stimulus offset, and the second (“Cue B”) was not paired with any outcome (Supplementary Figure 1C). An inter-trial interval (ITI) of 6 seconds was used to mirror our prior study that paired these visual stimuli with salient appetitive or aversive outcomes (Lutas et al., 2019). We first imaged BA neuron responses to the presentation of these visual stimuli prior to any manipulations. Only a small fraction of BA neurons was significantly responsive (1%; n = 14/1283 neurons from 7 mice) to the presentation of these arbitrary visual stimuli (Supplementary Figure 1D), consistent with prior work (Lutas et al., 2019). We then paired “Cue A” with brief photostimulation of VTADA→BA axon terminals across several daily sessions while tracking calcium responses in the same BA neurons. After three days of pairing, a larger fraction of BA neurons had become responsive to the paired visual stimulus (4%; n = 48/1199 neurons from 6 mice; Supplementary Figure 1D). Despite the randomized nature of stimulus presentations and the six-second ITI between trials, BA neurons that acquired responses to “Cue A” also acquired responses to the unpaired “Cue B” (Supplementary Figure 1E,F). As discussed below, such generalization may relate to sustained effects of transient VTADA→BA axon photostimulation. Nevertheless, responses to “Cue B” were weaker than those to “Cue A”, and neurons preferentially responded to “Cue A” rather than “Cue B” (Supplementary Figure 1G,H). Therefore, photostimulating VTADA→BA axons immediately following an arbitrary visual cue leads to the acquisition of cue-evoked responses in a subset of BA glutamatergic neurons.
A cAMP sensor reveals spatially broad dopamine signaling across all BA glutamatergic neurons
To better understand the immediate effects of photostimulating VTADA→BA axons on postsynaptic neurons, we expressed a Cre-dependent, compact fluorescent biosensor for cAMP, cADDis (Tewson et al., 2016), in BA glutamatergic neurons via AAV injections into EMX1-Cre transgenic mice (Figure 1A,B). We tested the sensor functionality by applying the adenylate cyclase (AC) activator forskolin (Figure 1C) or dopamine (Figure 1D) to brain slices to stimulate intracellular cAMP production. Both forskolin and dopamine generated reliable decreases in mean fluorescence intensity, indicating increasing cAMP concentration (Tewson et al., 2016) (note that for visualization purposes, y-axes have been inverted so that a rise in cADDis signal reflects an increase in cAMP; Figure 1C).
We next used cADDis to estimate the fraction of BA glutamatergic neurons that could respond to dopamine. Since almost all BA glutamatergic neurons express Type 1 dopamine receptors (D1) (Lutas et al., 2019; Namburi et al., 2015; O’leary et al., 2020), while only few express Type 2 dopamine receptors (D2), dopamine should elevate cAMP in BA neurons. To directly determine the fraction of BA glutamatergic neurons that showed increases in cAMP in response to dopamine, we extracted fluorescence traces from the somatic regions of individual neurons (Figure 1E; see Methods). We first analyzed responses to application of forskolin as a measure of dopamine receptor-independent cAMP production. Forskolin resulted in cAMP increases in all BA glutamatergic neurons. We then found that application of a high concentration of dopamine (20 μM) potently elevated cAMP in all BA glutamatergic neurons, similar to forskolin, while lower concentrations drove weaker yet detectable increases in BA neurons (Figure 1F). This finding demonstrates a near-universal potential for dopamine signaling in BA glutamatergic neurons, and suggests that the diversity of valence preferences of nearby BA glutamatergic neurons (Lutas et al., 2019; O’Neill et al., 2018) may not to be due to differences in their ability to increase (or decrease) their cAMP levels in response to dopamine.
Photostimulation of VTADA→BA axons transiently elevates cAMP in almost all BA glutamatergic neurons
While exogenous application of dopamine revealed a widespread increase in cAMP levels in BA glutamatergic neurons, physiological release of dopamine directly from axon terminals may be more spatially restricted. To evoke endogenous dopamine release, we targeted expression of a red-shifted channelrhodopsin (Chrimson) in VTA dopamine neurons using DAT-Cre transgenic mice (Bäckman et al., 2006; Klapoetke et al., 2014). To confirm that photostimulating VTADA→BA axons in brain slices reliably evoked dopamine release, we combined widefield photostimulation of Chrimson with two-photon imaging of a green fluorescent biosensor for dopamine, dLight1.1, that was expressed in BA glutamatergic neurons (Patriarchi et al., 2018) (Supplementary Figure 2A). Brief trains of optical stimulation (20 Hz; 5 s duration) generated transient elevations in dopamine that lasted for several seconds (Supplementary Figure 2B). Importantly, fluorescence changes in this D1-based dopamine sensor could be completely blocked by application of a D1 antagonist (SCH23390, 300 nM, Supplementary Figure 2C), confirming that the signals were mediated by dopamine release and not by artifacts related to optical stimulation. By comparing endogenous release of dopamine following optical stimulation of VTADA→BA axons to that following application of a near-saturating concentration of exogenous dopamine (cf. Figure 1), we found that the bulk endogenously released dopamine concentration was 5 times lower than near-saturating concentrations of dopamine (Supplementary Figure 2D), although the effective concentration at synaptic clefts may be higher.
We next tested whether we could visualize changes in cAMP levels in BA glutamatergic neurons in response to photostimulated dopamine release (Figure 1G). We crossed DAT-Cre and EMX1-Cre transgenic mice, which allowed for viral targeting of Chrimson to dopamine-releasing neurons in the VTA and cADDis to glutamatergic neurons in the BA. Photostimulation of VTADA→BA axon terminals combined with two-photon imaging of cADDis revealed transient increases in cAMP that peaked ~5 seconds later than dopamine, consistent with a delay between the rise in extracellular dopamine and the generation of intracellular cAMP in target neurons (Supplementary Figure 2E). The cAMP elevation remained above baseline for tens of seconds following optical stimulation of VTADA→BA axons – much longer than the duration of elevated extracellular dopamine (Figure 1H). The slower decay of cAMP was not due to prolonged activation of target dopamine receptors, as similar decay kinetics of evoked increases in cAMP were observed following direct photostimulation of intracellular cAMP using a novel blue-light activated adenylate cyclase (biPAC; Supplementary Figure 3A-C) (Zhang et al., 2021b). Thus, the release of dopamine onto BA neurons is transformed into a transient elevation in intracellular cAMP that extends in time by approximately five-fold compared to the elevation in extracellular dopamine (note that this is likely an underestimate of the duration of cAMP elevations given the low affinity of cADDis for cAMP, Kd ~ 10 μM). This extended elevation in cAMP across tens of seconds may reflect an extended window of plasticity that could result in generalization of plasticity to unpaired cues occurring within that time window (e.g. Supplementary Figure S1).
We next examined endogenous dopamine-evoked cAMP signals from individual BA neuron somata (Figure 1I,J; see Methods). Similar to cellular responses to exogenous dopamine application (Figure 1F), we detected widespread cAMP responses in over 70% of all BA glutamatergic neurons as well as in the surrounding neuropil largely composed of dendrites of these neurons (Figure 1K,L). While the magnitudes of the cAMP increases were small (Figure 1M), they were larger than those observed following 1 μM exogenous dopamine, a concentration that also drove cAMP increases across all BA neurons (Figure 1F). We observed similar magnitude responses in somatic and neuropil compartments, consistent with the finding that dopaminergic synaptic terminals contact dendrites of BA glutamatergic neurons (Muller et al., 2009) (Figure 1M). These findings indicate that most BA glutamatergic neurons express functional D1 receptors and can respond to axonal release of dopamine.
Synaptic depression of dopamine release temporally restricts cAMP signals in BA neurons
We noticed that brief trains of optical stimulation (20 Hz; 2, 5, or 10 s duration) generated transient elevations in cAMP that increased sublinearly with pulse train duration (Figure 2A-C). This sublinearity may reflect presynaptic depression of dopamine, as has been observed for dopaminergic inputs to the striatum (Liu and Kaeser, 2019). Indeed, we observed that photostimulating VTADA→BA axons evoked dopamine release that scaled sublinearly with increasing duration of the 15 Hz pulse train (Figure 2D-F). We also observed a diminished magnitude of dopamine release when inter-trial intervals (ITIs) were 20 seconds long, but not when they were two minutes long (Figure 2G-I). We found that 20 second ITIs resulted in a 25% depression in dopamine release whereas 120 second ITIs did not result in depression (Figure 2H,I). This long-lasting depression of dopamine release is consistent with the depression of VTADA→BA axon-evoked glutamate co-released with dopamine in the BA (Lutas et al., 2019), and with evidence that VTA projections to the striatum also exhibit synaptic depression when ITIs are shorter than two minutes (Adrover et al., 2014).
In vivo cAMP dynamics in BA neurons following repeated photostimulation of VTADA→BA axons
We next asked whether cAMP dynamics in BA glutamatergic neurons exhibit similar characteristics in vivo as in our brain slice experiments. We used fiber photometry to track cADDis signals in BA glutamatergic neurons in response to photostimulation of Chrimson in VTADA→BA axon terminals (Figure 3A). We previously used this method of stimulation and recording to measure optical stimulation-evoked dopamine release of a similar magnitude as natural (shock-evoked) dopamine release (Lutas et al., 2019). We reliably detected cAMP production in BA neurons in response to optically induced dopamine release in vivo (Figure 3B). Similar to our slice measurements, we observed elevated cAMP levels lasting tens of seconds following brief optical stimulation of dopamine release (Figure 3B). Our protocol for in vivo photostimulation of VTADA→BA axons was well below saturating levels, as it drove weaker changes in cAMP than intraperitoneal injection of a high concentration of a dopamine Type 1 receptor agonist (SKF81297; 20 mg/kg; Supplementary Figure 4A). When we repeatedly photostimulated VTADA→BA axons every 30 s, we found that cAMP transients attenuated over time, such that within three minutes, they were profoundly reduced in magnitude (Figure 3C,D). This result led us to consider the possibility that the diminished responses were caused by presynaptic depression of dopamine release, as described in Figure 2.
We explored this possibility by modeling cAMP dynamics in response to dopamine released at variable ITIs (30 s or 10 s; Figure 3E; Supplementary Figure 4B). We started with the assumption that the dynamics of intracellular cAMP in BA neurons following photostimulation of dopamine axons could be understood as a simple convolution of the dynamics of evoked dopamine concentration and of the decay in intracellular cAMP. We separately fit monoexponential decay functions to the post-peak response time courses from slice recordings for axon-evoked dopamine release and for cAMP evoked by direct, transient photostimulation of intracellular cAMP (dopamine: τ = 4 s; cAMP: τ = 20 s; see Methods). We then modeled the expected exponential attenuation in dopamine release with repeated stimulation based on our measurements of paired-pulse depression (Figure 2C; paired pulse ratio: 75%). Indeed, our simulation revealed that depression in dopamine release over trials is largely sufficient to result in a return of evoked cAMP levels to baseline within three minutes (Figure 3E; Supplementary Figure 4B).
We compared this simulation to in vivo fiber photometry of cADDis signals during the same protocol of photostimulation of VTADA→BA axons with 30- or 10-second ITIs. We found that endogenous cAMP dynamics mirrored those in our simulation (Figure 3F; Supplementary Figure 4C). To further assess whether a presynaptic rather than postsynaptic mechanism mediated the depression in evoked cAMP responses, we used biPAC to bypass the dopamine receptor and directly stimulate cAMP production (Figure 3G). When we photostimulated biPAC repeatedly every 30 seconds, cAMP levels remained persistently elevated through the duration of the recording (Figure 3H). Together, these results demonstrate that transient cAMP elevations such as those that likely occur during a rapid sequence of salient events do not accumulate across minutes, likely because of presynaptic depression of dopamine release.
Repeated salient events evoke decreasing cAMP responses in BA neurons
Dopamine is naturally released in the BA during motivationally salient appetitive and aversive events (Lutas et al., 2019). We tested whether we could detect cAMP changes during unexpected delivery of appetitive palatable food in hungry mice or aversive unexpected tail shocks in sated mice (Figure 4A), both of which drive robust dopamine release in the BA (Lutas et al., 2019). On the first trial, we were able to detect significant changes in response to the consumption of unexpected food delivery or to the delivery of an unexpected tail shock (Figure 4B,C), albeit with a lower amplitude than that observed during photostimulation. Similar to our finding with repeated photostimulation of VTADA→BA axons, we found that repeated food delivery or tail shock resulted in attenuated cAMP transients, such that within 5 minutes, we were unable to detect significant evoked cAMP increases (Figure 4D,E). The depression of cAMP responses in vivo in response to tail shocks or food delivery that was repeated every 30 or 10 seconds was similar to expectations from our simulated dopamine-evoked cAMP signal (Figure 4F,G; Supplementary Figure 4D,E).
Finally, we addressed whether depression of cAMP signals also resulted in depression rather than accumulation of protein kinase A (PKA) activity - a key downstream regulator of synaptic plasticity (Figure 4H). We use fiber photometry and a fluorescent sensor of PKA activity, exrai-AKAR2 (Zhang et al., 2021a), to detect in vivo responses to tail shocks (Figure 4H). We found that unexpected tail shock delivery elicited responses in the exrai-AKAR2 photometry signals that were more pronounced than in our cADDis recordings (Figure 4I). We then tested if repeated delivery of tail shocks every 30 s produced a depressing exrai-AKAR2 response. Indeed, consistent with the depression in the evoked cAMP response magnitude, we found that exrai-AKAR2 responses depressed with repeated unexpected tail shocks (Figure 4I). While exrai-AKAR2 reports the ratio of phosphorylation by PKA and dephosphorylation by protein phosphatases, our earlier findings of depressing cAMP responses to repeated dopamine axon photostimulation suggests a weakening of cAMP-mediated PKA activation. However, we cannot fully rule out the possibility that increases in protein phosphatase activity also contribute to the depression in exrai-AKAR2 signal. Together, our characterization of ex vivo and in vivo cAMP and PKA sensor responses following endogenous dopamine release reveals temporal constraints on sustained cAMP elevation and PKA phosphorylation of targets in BA neurons. These temporal constraints likely affect the potency of associative plasticity of novel vs. rapidly repeated salient events.
Discussion
The release of neuromodulators including dopamine in basal amygdala is critical for cue-outcome association learning (Johansen et al., 2014; Tang et al., 2020). Dopamine receptor activation, via cAMP dynamics, may strengthen synaptic inputs to enhance the salience of stimuli paired with either appetitive or aversive outcomes (Lutas et al., 2019). Here, we establish an imaging approach to investigate this process in BA glutamatergic neurons using optical tools to monitor cAMP dynamics and to evoke dopamine release or drive direct cAMP production. We demonstrate that exogenous dopamine as well as dopamine release from VTADA→BA axon terminals can increase cAMP in all BA glutamatergic neurons. Given that BA glutamatergic neurons segregate into appetitive- or aversive-preferring neurons, this widespread dopamine-evoked cAMP signal ignores the boundaries imposed by valence-specific teaching signals.
We found that during stimulation protocols that mimic temporally clustered, salient events, VTADA→BA axons initially drive cAMP in BA neurons in vivo. However, synaptic depression of these VTADA→BA axons limits the temporal window of dopamine-related accumulation of cAMP. In contrast, direct, repeated photostimulation of cAMP production that bypasses the dopamine receptor does not show this depression, and instead drives a persistent elevation in cAMP levels. These and other findings confirm that depression of dopaminergic input and actions of phosphodiesterases (Supplementary Figure 3C) control the accumulation of cAMP in BA neurons, thereby potentially regulating windows of plasticity in this region. More generally, our findings regarding the dynamics of dopamine release and cAMP accumulation in BA in vitro and in vivo provide a platform for linking the dynamics of intracellular biochemical signals with the dynamics of within-trial, across-trial, and across-day plasticity and learning.
Synaptic depression of dopamine release in BA restricts the window of elevated cAMP when release events are clustered in time
We found a potent and long-lasting depression of dopamine-evoked cAMP transients. We showed that synaptic depression of dopamine release occurs at VTADA→BA terminals and that modeling this synaptic depression fully captures the weakened cAMP signals we recorded in vivo in response to photostimulated dopamine release. While to our knowledge this property had not been measured previously at the VTADA→BA synapse, similar characteristics have been observed in dopamine neuron projections to the dorsal and ventral striatum (Adrover et al., 2014). Synaptic depression at these mesostriatal dopaminergic synapses is mediated by several factors including the activation of presynaptic Type 2 dopamine receptors following release of dopamine, and by depletion of the synaptic vesicle pool (Liu and Kaeser, 2019). We suspect that similar mechanisms mediate the minutes-long depression we observed in VTADA→BA projections, both in slices and in vivo. We demonstrated that this depression greatly impairs the ability of dopamine axon activation to maintain elevated postsynaptic cAMP levels. Thus, depression of dopamine release may impose temporal constraints on synaptic plasticity when salient events are clustered in time. It will be interesting to test whether clustering together or spacing apart salient events using the temporal information obtained from cAMP measurements influences learning rates or memory consolidation in amygdala-dependent tasks, as has been observed in Drosophila (Jacob and Waddell, 2020). Moreover, as certain medications (e.g. methylphenidate) as well as drugs of abuse (e.g. cocaine) can enhance dopamine synaptic depression (Adrover et al., 2014), future studies can investigate how these drugs influence the temporal properties of amygdala plasticity.
Is a permissive plasticity signal from VTADA→BA axons broadcast widely or only to a subset of BA neurons?
A major goal of our efforts to image cAMP in BA neurons was to understand whether dopamine, which is released from individual VTADA→BA axons following both appetitive and aversive outcomes (Lutas et al., 2019), drives increases in cAMP in all BA glutamatergic neurons. This could allow for dopamine to encode the motivational salience of an event by scaling the proportion of BA neurons that undergo a rise in cAMP levels. If different BA neurons exhibit varying sensitivities to dopamine, greater dopamine release during events with higher motivational salience should result in a greater percentage of BA neurons with dopamine-evoked changes in downstream signaling. We found that exogenous dopamine could elevate cAMP in all BA glutamatergic neurons, and photostimulation of endogenous release from dopamine axons drove detectable cAMP responses in most BA glutamatergic neurons in slices. On the one hand, this is likely to be a conservative estimate of affected neurons, given the limited sensitivity of our cAMP sensor (see below). On the other hand, we found that endogenous release of dopamine in response to palatable food and unexpected tail shocks were weaker than the photostimulated release, consistent with the interpretation that fewer BA neurons may have elevated cAMP following natural release of dopamine. The depression in dopamine-evoked cAMP we observed could further limit the magnitude of cAMP in some BA neurons. Thus, if different BA neurons have varying magnitudes of dopamine-evoked cAMP, the size of the BA neuronal population that exhibits plasticity could be limited to those neurons with the largest cAMP response. These results suggest that dopamine may determine the percentage of BA neurons that are plastic at a given time, thereby scaling learning rates with the motivational salience of expected appetitive and aversive outcomes.
Potential limitations imposed by the sensitivity of the cAMP sensor
The affinity of the cAMP sensor we used is in the low micromolar range (Tewson et al., 2016), which means that changes in cAMP concentration in the nanomolar range would be outside of this sensitivity range. Especially in the case of our in vivo fiber photometry measurements, we may have missed changes in cAMP in response to unexpected delivery of palatable food or tail shocks. We overcame this limitation via additional experiments using a fluorescent biosensor of PKA activity (Zhang et al., 2021a), which had improved signal-to-noise in response to unexpected tail shocks and revealed clear depression with repeated salient events. An additional limitation is that our sensor measurements were not targeted specifically to dendritic compartments where cAMP increases may be concentrated, and thus our bulk measurements across somatic and dendritic compartments may have limited our sensitivity further. Nevertheless, presynaptic depression of dopamine release likely limits cAMP and PKA signals both in dendritic and somatic compartments. Future studies using higher affinity cAMP sensors (Klarenbeek et al., 2015) as well as sensors targeted to dendritic spines (e.g. by fusing with PSD-95) should allow for further real-time interrogation of these signals in response to endogenous salient events.
Conclusions and future directions
We have established an imaging platform using reporters and actuators of intracellular cAMP that allowed for direct interrogation of dopamine-dependent cAMP signals in BA glutamatergic neurons in vitro and in vivo. Future studies can employ this approach to investigate cAMP dynamics in BA neurons in pre-clinical settings, such as following acute and chronic exposure to addictive substances, or to stressful, traumatic events. In addition, many other critical neuromodulatory signals in the BA (e.g., norepinephrine, PACAP, and serotonin) act via influences on cAMP. A similar imaging approach could be used to investigate how these additional neuromodulators affect cAMP levels alone and in conjunction with dopamine signaling. Another key future direction will involve examination of cAMP dynamics in non-glutamatergic targets of VTA dopamine neurons in BA, such as parvalbumin-positive interneurons (Chu et al., 2012; Pinard et al., 2008). In conclusion, we have revealed important temporal and spatial characteristics of dopamine actions via cAMP signaling in BA, which have informed hypotheses about the role of dopamine during associative learning. Importantly, while most in vivo studies of cellular plasticity have focused on changes in electrical and calcium activity, continued optimization of tools to detect molecular signaling cascades will expand our understanding of the underlying biochemical signals that control neural circuit plasticity.
Author contributions
A.L. and M.L.A. conceived the project and wrote the manuscript. A.L. designed and performed brain slice and two-photon imaging. A.L., K.F. and A.S. conducted fiber photometry recordings. A.L., K.F., A.S. and S.X.Z. performed surgical procedures. A.L. and S.X.Z. designed experiments for photostimulation concurrent with two-photon imaging of acute brain slices. A.L. analyzed all data.
Lead contact
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact, Mark Andermann (manderma [at] bidmc.harvard.edu).
Materials availability
For inquiries regarding cADDis plasmid and virus generated during this study, please contact Montana Molecular.
mKate2-biPAC plasmid DNA generated during this study and another study (Zhang et al., 2021) will be deposited to Addgene [catalog # 169127].
Code and data availability
Analysis scripts were written in Matlab and used built-in functions and previously published code. No novel code was generated for the purpose of this study. Data is available upon request.
EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS
Animals
All animal care and experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center. Animals were housed in a 12-hour-light/12-hour-darkness environment with standard mouse chow and water provided ad libitum, unless specified otherwise. For both in vitro and in vivo experiments, adult male and female mice between the ages of 9 to 20 weeks were used for this study.
METHOD DETAILS
Stereotaxic surgeries
Viral injections, fiber implantations, and GRIN lens implantations were performed as described in (Lutas et al., 2019) with the following specifications and modifications.
For targeting expression into BA glutamatergic neurons, mice (8 – 12 weeks of age) expressing Cre recombinase driven by the Emx1 promoter (EMX1-Cre) were injected at (relative to Bregma): anteroposterior (AP): − 1.6 mm; dorsoventral (DV): − 4.8 mm; mediolateral (ML): ± 3.3 mm. For targeting expression into VTA dopamine neurons, DAT-Cre mice were injected at AP: − 3.2 mm; DV: − 4.5 mm; ML: ± 0.4 mm.
For in vivo experiments using the cAMP sensor cADDis, we unilaterally injected AAV1-hSyn-DIO-GreenDownward-cADDis (Children’s Hospital Vector Core; 300 nL). For cADDis or dLight1.1 (AAV1-hSyn-dLight1.1; Children’s Hospital Vector Core; 300 nL) brain slice imaging experiments, injections were targeted bilaterally to increase the number of useable slices. For in vivo calcium imaging experiments, we unilaterally injected AAV1-hSyn-FLEX-GCaMP6s (Addgene; 300 nL).
For fiber photometry experiments, optic fibers (400 μm diameter core; multimode; NA 0.48; 5.0 mm length; Doric Fibers) were implanted over BA at (relative to Bregma): AP: −1.6 mm; DV: −4.7 mm; ML: 3.3 mm.
For in vivo BA cell body imaging of GCaMP6s, mice were implanted with a singlet gradient index (GRIN) lens (GRINtech, NEM-100-25-10-860-S-1.0p; 1.0 mm diameter; 9 mm length; 250 μm focal distance on brain side at 860 nm, (NA 0.5); 100 μm focal distance on air side (NA 0.5); non-coated). GRIN lens implantation coordinates for cell body imaging of BA neurons in EMX1-Cre transgenic mice (relative to Bregma): AP: −1.6 mm, ML: 3.2 mm, DV: −4.8 mm. In order to ensure a snug fit for the lens to reduce brain motion and to increase surgical survival rate, we pre-set the insertion tracks by lowering a syringe needle with a slightly narrower diameter (20-gauge, 0.9 mm) to a depth of 0.1 mm above the final depth of the lens.
Widefield epifluorescence imaging of acute brain slices
Following 3 – 5 weeks of expression, acute slices were prepared as described in Lutas et al., 2019, and widefield fluorescence imaging was performed on an upright microscope (Axioskop 2 plus; Zeiss) equipped with an sCMOS II camera (Prime, Photometrics). Fluorescence excitation for imaging was achieved using a 470 nm LED (Thorlabs). Image acquisition was performed using ImageJ Micro-manager (Edelstein et al., 2014). Image acquisition frame rate was 2 Hz for cADDis fluorescence imaging. A 10x (Olympus) or 20x (Zeiss) objective was used for all imaging experiments. During imaging, slices were continuously superfused (flow rate: 2-5 ml/min) with oxygenated (95% O2 and 5% CO2) artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) at room temperature. To prevent oxidation of dopamine, 50 μM Na-metabisulfite was included in all ACSF solutions during dopamine application experiments.
General two-photon imaging methods
Two-photon imaging was performed using a two-photon resonant-galvo scanning microscope (NeuroLabWare) at 15.5 frames/second and 796 x 512 pixels/frame as described previously (Lutas et al., 2019). An InSight X3 laser (Spectra-Physics) was used to excite the fluorophores (910-1050 nm), and the emission light was filtered (green: 510/84 nm; red: 607/70 nm; Semrock) before collection with photomultiplier tubes (H10770B-40; Hamamatsu). The XY scanning was performed using resonant/galvo mirrors and the Z scanning was achieved with an electrically-tunable lens (Optotune).
Two-photon imaging of acute brain slices
For two-photon imaging of acute brain slices, slices were prepared as described in Lutas et al. and transferred to a recording chamber perfused with ACSF (oxygenated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2; flow rate: 2-5 mL/min) at either room temperature or 32° C as indicated. Imaging was performed with a 16x 0.8 NA water-immersion objective (Nikon). The excitation wavelength used was 920 nm.
In optogenetic experiments involving Chrimson, stimulation was triggered by the onset of a frames (15.5 Hz) for 2-, 5-, or 10-second duration. The gating property of the PMT was triggered at the onset of the frame to protect the PMT from optogenetic stimulation light and lasted for 10 ms. Thus, the top ~16% of each frame during the stimulation was blank, but much of the frame (~84%) provided near-simultaneous imaging of biosensor signals during the photostimulation. A 620 nm LED (1 mW/mm2, Luxeon Star LEDs) driven by an Arduino-controlled driver (Luxeon Star LEDs) was used for photostimulation.
In optogenetic experiments involving biPAC, each slice was imaged for 10.5 minutes (15.5 frames per second). At time points: 0.5 min, 2.5 min, 4.5 min, 6.5 min the PMT was powered off, a 470 nm LED (1 mW/mm2, Luxeon Star LEDs) driven by an Arduino-controlled driver (Luxeon Star LEDs) was then turned on for 2 seconds, and the PMT power was the restored to the original level. The PMT was turned off and on to protect it from the LED light.
To block phosphodiesterase activity, IBMX (100 μM in DMSO) was applied to the brain slice for at least 10 minutes. Recordings of cADDis following biPAC stimulation were performed before and after application of IBMX. To block dLight1.1 signals (dLight1.1 is engineered from the D1 receptor), we applied an antagonist of the D1 receptor (SCH23390; 300 nM) while recording dLight1.1 signals evoked by photostimulation of dopamine release from VTADA→BA axons in brain slices.
In vivo two-photon imaging via implanted lens
Two-photon imaging via implanted lenses was performed as previously described (Lutas et al., 2019) with the following adjustments. A 10x 0.5 NA air objective was used (ThorLabs). For optogenetic stimulation of Chrimson via the implanted lens, a 617 nm LED (ThorLabs) was used (5 - 10 mW at the objective face).
Fiber photometry recording
Fiber photometry recordings were performed as described in Lutas et al., 2019, using head-fixed mice that were free to run on a circular treadmill. Fiber optic cables (1 m long; 400 μm core; 0.48 NA; Doric Lenses) were coupled to implanted optic fibers with zirconia sleeves (Precision Fiber Products). Excitation and emission light was passed through a four-port fluorescence mini-cube (FMC4_E(460-490)_F(500-550)_O(580-650)_S, Doric Lenses), which allowed for collection of GFP fluorescence and excitation of red-shifted channelrhodopsins. For biosensor photometry recordings, the excitation light (~ 100 μW at the face of the patch cord) was provided by a 465 nm LED (Plexon LED and driver). For optogenetic stimulation, the excitation light (~ 5 mW at the face of the patch cord) was provided by a 620 nm LED (Plexon LED and driver) which was controlled by an Arduino Uno. Emission light was collected by a femtowatt photoreceiver (Newport 2151), demodulated using a lock-in amplifier (SR830; Stanford Instruments) and digitized at 1 kHz sample rate (PCIe-6321; National Instruments). Data acquisition was controlled using a custom script in MATLAB (MathWorks). In a subset of experiments the D1 receptor agonist (SKF81297; 20 mg/kg in saline; Tocris R&D systems), was injected intraperitoneally while recording cADDis photometry signals. In these experiments, endogenous dopamine release was achieved with Chrimson photostimulation of VTADA→BA terminals and we compared peak cAMP responses following photostimulation and injection of SKF81297. Delivery of appetitive (Ensure) or aversive (tail shock; 0.3 mA,50 ms) outcomes was performed as previously described (Lutas et al., 2019).
QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Statistics
The numbers of samples in each group were based on those in previously published studies. Experiments were conducted by an investigator with knowledge of the animal genotype and treatment. Custom-written MATLAB analysis scripts allowed for data analysis in an automated and unbiased manner. All virus expression, optic fiber implants, and GRIN lens placements were verified by post hoc histology. Mice in which either the virus expression or optic fiber was not appropriately located (< 10 % of the time) were excluded from analysis. All data presented as bar and line graphs indicate mean ± s.e.m. with individual data points also plotted. Statistical analyses were performed in MATLAB. Significance levels are indicated as follows unless otherwise specified: *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
Data analysis
All data analyses were performed using MATLAB (Mathworks) and ImageJ (NIH).
Fiber photometry analysis
Photometry signals were sampled at 1 kHz, low pass filtered below 100 Hz, and downsampled to 50 Hz. We calculated ΔF/F = (F − F0)/F0, where F0 the mean of baseline window prior to the event of interest. For analysis of the responses to an individual event, all trials containing presentations of that event during a run were averaged to obtain a mean timecourse, and then the peak response during the event window was obtained. We also did not use the sliding-window method for baseline normalized to avoid distorting the slow dynamics of intracellular cAMP signals.
Widefield fluorescence brain slice imaging analysis
Movies were initially corrected for x-y motion using identical methods (efficient subpixel registration to averaged reference image) as used for two-photon imaging analyses above. For cADDis cAMP sensor imaging, as cells were very bright at baseline (as evoked cAMP leads to a decrease in fluorescence of this sensor) and showed slower dynamic changes in fluorescence upon dopamine application, regions-of-interests were automatically segmented by using morphological filters to identify bright spherical regions (Liang et al., 2018). Briefly, a mean projection through the movie was first applied to obtain a single mean image. Local normalization was then applied by subtracting the local mean (Gaussian kernel with sigma = 8 pixels) and dividing by local variance across pixels (Gaussian kernel with sigma = 150 pixels). Basic MATLAB functions were then used to remove small unconnected structures and fill in gaps in larger structures. The image was then binarized and regions of interest were segmented by applying a Euclidian distance transform followed by a watershed transform (a common strategy used to segment spherical objects). We estimated neuropil signals by taking circular annuli around the region of interest, as described above for two-photon image analysis.
Two-photon brain slice and in vivo imaging analysis
Image registration for brain slice and in vivo two-photon calcium imaging of BA cell bodies was performed as previously described (Lutas et al., 2019). Briefly, to corrected for x-y motion, each frame from an imaging session was registered to a reference image (average of 1000 frames within a session) using efficient subpixel registration methods (Bonin et al., 2011). For extraction of signal from cell body regions of interest (ROIs) from volumetric brain slice imaging (15 depths; ~ 10 μm apart), we used CellPose (Stringer et al., 2020), which optimally identified ROIs from bright cADDis expressing cell bodies. For in vivo calcium imaging analysis, we used PCA/ICA to extract masks of pixels with correlated activity, corresponding to individual axons or cell bodies (Mukamel et al., 2009). Timecourses were extracted by averaging each of the pixels within each binarized mask. We calculated neuropil activity as the median value of an annulus surrounding each ROI (inner radius: 15 pixels; outer radius: 50 pixels; pixels belonging to any other ROI were excluded from these annulus masks). This timecourse of neuropil activity was then subtracted from the activity timecourse of the associated ROI to create a fluorescence timecourse, F(t), where t is time of each imaging frame. The change in fluorescence was calculated by subtracting a running estimate of baseline fluorescence (F0(t)) from F(t), then dividing by F0(t): ΔF/F(t) = (F(t) − F0(t))/ F0(t), where F0(t) is a running estimate of baseline fluorescence calculated as the 10th percentile of F(t) in the previous 32-second sliding window (Petreanu et al., 2012).
Criteria for determining responsivity to cues from in vivo calcium imaging
To determine if a cell was responsive to each cue, we used previously established, conservative criteria (Lutas et al., 2019), which are described here. We performed a Wilcoxon sign-rank test for each frame post-stimulus onset against the 1-s baseline period prior to stimulus onset, with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons across frames (p < 0.01). If three consecutive frames were significantly different than the baseline period, a cell was considered responsive to that cue. For all cells with significant responses to at least one cue, a cell preferred cue was determined as the cue evoking the largest response during the cue period. For estimation of a cell’s mean cue-evoked response magnitude, and for estimation of a cell’s response bias to a given cue, we averaged all trials containing presentations of that cue during the run to obtain a mean timecourse for that cell, and then the maximum response during the 2-s duration of the cue presentation was used as that cell’s response magnitude.
Modeling dopamine-evoked cAMP dynamics
To model the dynamics of dopamine and cAMP, we first fit monoexponential functions to averaged traces of either dLight1.1 or cADDis recordings. We used the decay rate from the dLight transient to first model dopamine dynamics in response to 2 s square inputs (30 s intervals between each input), which exponentially depressed such that the second input was 25% weaker than the first pulse (as observed in our measurements of dopamine synaptic depression). We convolved this input kernel with an exponential (4 s time constant; step size of 0.1 s). We then used this convolved waveform which models dopamine with synaptic depression as an input kernal to model the cAMP dynamics (20 s time constant, step size of 0.1 s). For qualitative comparison with in vivo recordings, we normalized the modeled cAMP dynamics to the peak of the first evoked response.
Acknowledgements
We thank members of the Andermann lab including K. Evans, R. Essner, N. Nguyen, K. McGuire, Dr. H. Kucukdereli, Dr. O. Amsalem, Dr. J.S. Alvarado for helpful feedback on the manuscript. We thank V. Flores-Maldonado for assistance with mouse colony care. We thank Dr. J. Madara for assistance with brain slice preparation. Boston Children’s Hospital Viral Core provided viral packaging services. We thank Drs. Ingie Hong and Richard Huganir for sharing exrai-AKAR2 virus. Authors were supported by an NIH F32 DK112589, a Davis Family Foundation Award, and a Boston Nutrition Obesity Research Center Pilot grant (A.L.), a Lefler Fellowship (S.X.Z.), NIH R01 DK109930, DP1 AT010971, DP1 AT010971-02S1, R01 MH12343, the McKnight Foundation, the Klarman Family Foundation, and the Harvard Brain Science Initiative Bipolar Disorder Seed Grant, supported by Kent and Liz Dauten (M.L.A).
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.