ABSTRACT
Electricity is widely utilized as environmental stimulus to sense the world by many animal species. Despite its importance, however, molecular and physiological mechanisms for responding to electrical stimulus have been far less understood compared to other sensory stimuli. Here we report novel behavioral responses to electrical stimulus of the nematode C. elegans. When the animals on food are stimulated by alternating current, their movement speed suddenly increases more than 2-fold, which persists for a few minutes even after the electrical stimulation is terminated. Genetic analyses reveal that voltage-gated channels are required for the response, possibly as the sensors, and neuropeptide signaling suppresses the persistent response. Additional behavioral analysis reveals that, in addition to the persistence, the animal’s response to electrical shock is scalable and has a negative valence, which are recently regarded as emotion primitives, suggesting that the response may reflect a primitive form of “fear” of animals.
INTRODUCTION
In order to survive and reproduce, animals respond to various environmental sensory stimuli by perceiving and processing these cues within the neural circuit, and convert them into behavioral outputs. In addition to the well-known stimuli cues, such as light, sound, chemical and temperature, animals also respond to other stimuli, such as magnetic field and electricity (Baker et al., 2013; Wiltschko & Wiltschko, 2005).
In neuroscience research, electricity is used as unconditioned stimulus with negative valence to cause associative learning in rodents and in flies (Aceves-Piña & Quinn, 1979; Blair et al., 2005; Tarpley et al., 2010; Tully et al., 1994). In nature, however, multiple animal species are known to respond to electricity for survival purposes, such as communication, navigation and/or prey detection (Clarke et al., 2013; Crampton, 2019; Pettigrew, 1999). For example, weakly electric African fish (Gnathonemus petersii) utilize their epidermal electroreceptors to receive self-produced electric signals, allowing the fish to identify, locate, and examine nearby objects (von der Emde et al., 1998). In addition, platypus (Ornithorhynchus anatinus), blind cave salamander (Proteus anguinus), and bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) are also known to sense electric signals for navigation and/or foraging (Istenič & Bulog, 1984; Roth & Schlegel, 1988; Scheich et al., 1986; Sutton et al., 2016). Such wide use of electrical signals in the animal kingdom suggests that the molecular mechanisms of electricity perception as well as neural circuits to utilize the perceived information have independently emerged or diverged during evolution. Despite their importance, the molecules required for responses to electrical signals have only been revealed in sharks and skates: Bellono et al. reported that electrosensory cells in little skate and chain catshark use L-type voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCC) (Bellono et al., 2017; 2018).
The nematode Caenorhabditis elegans has been widely used in neurobiological research because of the feasibility of molecular, physiological, and behavioral analyses of neural functions. The animals have been known to respond to direct current (DC), migrating along the electric field from the positive end to the negative end (Sukul & Croll, 1978), and a few classes of chemosensory neurons (ASH and ASJ) were found to be required for their ability to align themselves according to the DC field (Gabel et al., 2007). The animals are also reported to respond to strong alternating current (AC)—they exhibit a “convulsant” phenotype (paralysis and elongation) upon delivery of a brief electrical shock (200 Hz, 3.5 ms, 47 V) and recover rapidly after removal of the electrical shock (Risley et al., 2016).
However, other behavioral responses as well as molecular mechanisms for electrical signals have not been revealed.
In this study, we report that C. elegans responds to AC electric stimulus by immediately increasing their speed. The speed increase lasts for minutes even after an electric stimulus of seconds is terminated, suggesting that the response is caused not by direct stimulation of the motor system for rapid movement but possibly by persistent activity of a specific set of neurons to generate the behavioral response. Further behavioral analyses suggest that, in addition to the persistence, the behavioral response is associated with valence and scalability, thus exhibiting at least 3 out of the 4 features of “emotion primitives” (Anderson & Adolphs, 2014). A series of candidate genetic analyses reveal that the response is not mediated by well-known chemo- or mechano-sensory mechanisms. Instead, it requires both L-type VGCC, as in the shark and skate, and N-type VGCC, which have not previously been implicated in animal electrical responses. Furthermore, we find that neuropeptide signaling is required to suppress the persistence. These results indicate that the animals’ response to electric shock can be a suitable paradigm to reveal molecular and physiological mechanisms of electrosensation as well as a primitive form of emotion, such as fear.
RESULTS
Worms’ speed increases by AC stimulation
In order to study C. elegans’ response to electric shock, we established a setup (Figure 1), where several adult wild-type animals were placed onto 9 cm NGM agar plates seeded with a small bacterial food patch and subjected to AC stimulation. The complete trajectories produced by the animals were video-recorded, and their speed was calculated based on the x- y coordinates of worms’ centroids in each image frame.
We first studied the response to AC stimulation covering a range between 15 - 105 V at 60 Hz (the commercial power frequency in Japan), and found that the animals increased their average speed during electrical stimulation by varying amounts (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). We then conducted a series of systematic analyses with different voltages and frequencies at 30 – 75 V and 0.25–256 Hz (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). After the analysis, we noticed that an interesting characteristic of this behavioral phenotype is most apparent when using 4 Hz stimuli: When worms were stimulated with 30 V, their average speed of movement suddenly increased more than 2-fold, and this persisted during the electrical admission. We named this behavior the “ON response” (Figure 2A and C). During this running behavior, the worms engage in rapid body bends as well as rapid head movements (Figure 2—video 1). In the ON response, we did not detect a statistical bias in any direction (Figure 2—figure supplement 3). Moreover, when a stronger electric stimulus of 75 V was applied, it caused a significant increase in average speed not during but immediately after the stimulus, which we named the “OFF response” (Figure 2B). A fraction of the animals responded during the stimulus in the OFF response condition, while, in the majority of the animals, the speed was suppressed during the stimulus and then increased immediately after the removal (Figure 2—figure supplement 4 and video 2). During application of voltage, those worms that are immobilized appear to be convulsing, with jerky, unproductive muscle contractions occurring throughout the body. With other frequencies, ON and OFF responses were also observed, but were less cleare compared to those with 4 Hz (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). We considered the ON and OFF responses at 4 Hz to be interesting because only 2.5-fold differences in the voltage at the same frequency caused completely different behavioral responses, which does not happen generally with other stimuli, such as odor, to the animals (Bargmann et al., 1993).
We then analyzed whether this response depends on voltage or current by manipulating the salt concentration in the assay plate: The higher salt concentration should result in a larger current when the same strength of voltage is applied. As shown in Figure 3, 30 V and 75 V stimulus caused ON and OFF responses, respectively, regardless of the current value, indicating that the behavioral response depends on voltage.
Speed increase lasted for several minutes
Next, we examined how long the increased speed persists during and after the stimulus. When the duration of applied electric shock was 1-2 minutes, significant speed increases were maintained during the stimulus, lasted for ∼1 min after the stimulus, and went back to the baseline level (Figure 4A). Interestingly, when the animals were stimulated only for 5 sec, the speed increase still lasted for 1.5 min. When 4 min stimulus was applied, the increase was maintained during the stimulus but went back to the baseline level 30 sec after the stimulus. During 10 min stimulation, the significant speed increase was observed only for 5.5 min.
Thus, we concluded that the ON response caused by 30 V stimulation persists ∼5 min at most.
This result suggested the possibility that the speed increase persisted for ∼5 min because of fatigue in motor systems. However, animals stimulated intermittently 5 times for 30 seconds per stimulation maintained a speed increase for much longer time than those under the continuous stimulus (Figure 4B versus “10 minutes” in A). This result suggests that the decrease in speed during the long ON stimulation period is not caused by fatigue in the motor system, but possibly by sensory adaptation, which is widely known to adjust the animal’s sensory response to new environments (Webster, 2012).
We then tested the persistence of speed increase in the OFF response with 75 V. Interestingly, 5 and 30 sec stimuli caused longer persistent responses after the stimulus than 30 V did (Figure 4C). 45 sec stimulus caused >2 min persistent response, which is the longest among the responses to 30 and 75 V stimuli after the stimulus. However, when animals were stimulated for 1 min, no ON or OFF responses was observed, possibly due to physical damage to the animals. The fact that the larger stimulus (75 V) caused longer persistent responses than the smaller one (30 V) suggests that the response to electric shock is “scalable”, one of the critical “emotion primitives” together with persistence (Anderson & Adolphs, 2014).
We then tested the effect of food presence on the speed increase. C. elegans move slowly on the bacterial food lawn and faster out of the lawn (Sawin et al., 2000). As we used a small food lawn to localize the animal’s initial positions in the center of the plate (Figures 1 and 2C), it was possible that the electrical stimulus caused the animals to move away from the food lawn, which then caused increased speed due to the absence of food. If this is the case, the animal’s speed would be considerably lower with the electrical stimulus when the plates were fully covered with a bacterial lawn. To test the hypothesis, we compared the time- course of speed changes on plates with a small patch of food lawn and with a full food lawn. As shown in Figure 4D and E (compare Figure 4A “4 minutes” and B “ 30 seconds”, respectively), there was no substantial difference in the time course of speed change between the behavioral responses on the small food and the full food plates, demonstrating that the speed increase is not caused by the food absence but by the electrical stimulation itself.
To further confirm the result, we analyzed the animals’ speed on a stripe-like food pattern (Figure 4—figure supplement 1A). We did not observe a significant difference in the speeds when the animals moved into or out of the food area (Figure 4—figure supplement 1B). This result indicates that the animals’ migratory speed is not affected by the presence or absence of food, which is one the most influential environmental signals for the animals. It may further suggest that animals prioritize moving away from a harmful condition, such as strong electrical shock, to protect themselves.
Two types of voltage-gated calcium channels, but not chemo- or mechano-sensory molecules, are required for the sensation of electric shock
To identify gene(s) required for the response to electric shock, we analyzed a series of mutant strains of candidate genes. We tested the mutants of genes involved in the animals’ chemo- or mechano-sensation, the homologues of genes involved in electroreception in shark and skate, and genes involved in the biosynthesis of neuromodulators.
C. elegans’ chemo-sensation is largely mediated by the 12 pairs of amphid sensory neurons in the head, which are classified into the ones using TAX-2 and TAX-4 cyclic nucleotide-gated channel (CNGC) subunits or the others using OSM-9 and OCR-2 transient receptor potential (TRP) channel subunits for depolarization (Coburn & Bargmann, 1996; Colbert et al., 1997; Komatsu et al., 1996; Tobin et al., 2002). In addition to loss-of-function mutants for the above-mentioned genes, we tested mutants for che-2, a gene required for the proper formation and function of the ciliated sensory neurons (Fujiwara et al., 1999). For mechano- sensation, we analyzed loss- or reduction-of-function alleles of mec-4, mec-10, and trp-4. mec-4 and mec-10 genes encode DEG/ENaC proteins and form a mechanosensory ion channel complex for transduction of gentle touch (Chalfie & Sulston, 1981; Driscoll & Chalfie, 1991; Huang & Chalfie, 1994), while trp-4 encodes TRPN (NOMPC) for harsh touch response (Kang et al., 2010; Li et al., 2011). All the mutant strains exhibited wild-type- like responses in ON as well as OFF responses (panel A in Figures 5 and 6 for ON and OFF responses, respectively). Some mutants (osm-9;ocr-2, che-2, mec-10, and tph-1) exhibited statistical differences in the OFF response, suggesting the partial involvement of these genes, although the defects in speed increase (i.e. ΔSpeed) were not as severe as the ones of VGCC mutants (see below). The non-involvement of tax-4 also indicates that temperature increase caused by the electric stimulus or speed increase induced by high O2 due to the npr-1 mutation is not responsible for the response (Coates & de Bono, 2002) (see Discussion for details).
We then tested egl-19, the orthologue of the L-type VGCC alpha subunit, which functions in the sensory organ for environmental electric signals for shark and skate (Bellono et al., 2017, 2018). We found that two reduction-of-function alleles of egl-19 mutants exhibited strong defects in ON and OFF responses (Figures 5 and 6, panels B and F). This result suggests that the VGCC may be an evolutionarily conserved sensor for environmental electricity.
This finding further motivated us to test two other types of voltage-gated calcium channels, i.e. N-type voltage-gated calcium channel (UNC-2) and T-type voltage-gated calcium channel (CCA-1), although only L-type VGCC had been found to be involved in electrical responses in other animals. Unexpectedly, mutants for two alleles of unc-2 were defective in both ON and OFF responses, while cca-1(ad1650) mutants behaved similar to the wild-type controls (Figures 5 and 6, panels B and F). These results demonstrate that UNC-2, the N-type VGCC, is also required for the electric sensation, and also suggest that the worms may utilize similar but substantially different molecular mechanisms for electrical sensation than sharks and skates.
Lastly, we analyzed the genes required for the biosynthesis of neuromodulators, such as serotonin, dopamine, octopamine and tyramine. As shown in panel C in Figures 5 and 6, these mutants also exhibited wild-type-like responses, indicating that these neuromodulators are not involved either. Because dopamine and serotonin signaling are known to be required for the feeding status-dependent modulation of migratory speed, these results are also consistent with the fact that feeding status is not the causal reason for the speed increase (Figure 4D and E, and Figure 4—figure supplement 1).
We also tested the involvement of neuropeptides by using loss- or reduction-of-function mutations of egl-3, a gene required for maturation of pro-neuropeptides (Kass et al., 2001). Unexpectedly, mutations in both alleles of egl-3, n589 and ok979, caused much longer persistence of the speed increase after the electric shock (Figures 5 and 6, panels D-F), indicating that the persistent activity in the neural circuit for speed increase is down-regulated by neuropeptide signaling in the wild-type animals.
DISCUSSION
Response to AC stimulus and its molecular mechanism
Multiple vertebrate and invertebrate species are known to sense electric signals for navigation and/or foraging. For example, in addition to the electrical fish, platypus (Ornithorhynchus anatinus) detects electrical signals via their duck-like bills to locate and avoid objects when navigating in the water (Scheich et al., 1986). Blind cave salamander (Proteus anguinus) perceives a moving back-and-forth direct-current field and its polarity via ampullary organs to survive and navigate in their environment, which is in complete darkness as their eyes are undeveloped (Istenič & Bulog, 1984; Roth & Schlegel, 1988). And in invertebrates, bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) sense environmental electric fields via sensory hairs to make foraging decisions (Sutton et al., 2016). These results suggest that sensation of electrical signals are essential for survival and reproduction of animals in the wild.
In this study, we established an original experimental paradigm and found that C. elegans responds to AC electrical stimulus: The animals significantly increase their movement speed during and after the stimulus for minutes. Although the animals have also been reported to respond to DC (Gabel et al., 2007), we consider that the responses to AC and DC are different for the following reasons. (1) In the DC field, the animals moved at a certain angle (∼4° per 1 V/cm), which was not observed in our AC stimulus ( Figure 2—figure supplement 3). (2) Movement speed did not change with the DC stimulus.
In addition, five pairs of amphid sensory neurons were involved in the DC response (Gabel et al., 2007), while mutations is genes required for sensory signaling in the amphid sensory neurons (tax-4, osm-9, ocr-2, and che-2) did not affect the AC response in our study (Figures 5 and 6), indicating that DC and AC responses utilize different sensory mechanisms. Our result also rules out the possibility that the animals respond to increased agar temperature due to the AC stimulus, because the mutation in tax-4, the gene essential for temperature response (Komatsu et al., 1996) did not affect the response. In addition, the genes required for mechano-sensation (mec-4, mec-10, and trp-4) are not required for the AC response either.
We found that L-type as well as N-type VGCC, EGL-19 and UNC-2, respectively, are required for the AC response. L-type VGCC has been found to function in the electrosensory organs in the shark and skate, but not N-type, indicating that C. elegans utilizes similar but different molecular mechanisms. Since EGL-19 is expressed in most if not all the neurons (Lee et al., 1997), it will be interesting to identify the neurons in which the channel functions, whether they are the same or different from the neurons that utilize the N-type channels, and how they contribute to the increase in the movement speed. As mentioned above, various organs in different animal species are known to sense electrical stimuli. Therefore, it would be interesting to investigate whether L-type as well as N-type VGCCs also function in the organs of these animals to sense electrical signals.
Electric stimulus causes persistent behavioral response
Persistent neural activity, a sustained neural activity caused by a short-term stimulus, plays critical roles in brain function, such as controlling the oculo-motor system, working memory, and decision making, although its detailed mechanisms have not been sufficiently elucidated (Curtis & Lee, 2010; Major & Tank, 2004). Persistent behavioral state is likely caused by persistent neural activity, suggesting that genetic analysis of persistent behavioral state may reveal molecular mechanism(s) of persistent neural activity.
We unexpectedly found that C. elegans’ high speed response persists after electrical shock. In the animals, two other types of persistent behavioral responses have been reported. The first is that the animal’s movement speed is elevated at high O2 concentration in npr-1(lf) and in the Hawaiian wild isolate CB4856, which has the same amino acid variation in npr-1 (Cheung et al., 2005). In this behavioral response, the elevated speed returns rapidly to the basal speed when the high O2 is terminated, the animals still recognize and aggregate at the edge of a food lawn, and a mutation in the tax-4 CNGC homolog for sensory depolarization abolishes the response (Coates & de Bono, 2002). Another type of persistent behavioral response is roaming (Flavell et al., 2020; Manabi Fujiwara et al., 2002). Roaming is a behavioral state with high movement speed, although it is only exhibited when the animals are on food and requires serotonin signaling. Because the behavioral response to electrical shock persists more than 2 min after 30-45 sec stimulus with 75 V and more than 1.5 min after only 5 sec stimulus, is not affected by food stimulus, and does not require CNGC activity or serotonin signaling, the analysis of electrical shock response is likely different from the above-mentioned two behavioral responses and may provide a unique opportunity for genetic dissection of a persistent behavioral state.
Response to the electric stimulus may reflect a primitive form of emotion
Anderson and Adolphs defined emotion as an internal state triggered by specific stimuli likely rewarding or punishing and that persistence, scalability, valence, and generalization are key characteristics for primitive forms of emotion in animals (Anderson & Adolphs, 2014).
In addition to persistence, we consider that the electrical stimulus has negative valence. This is because the animals ignore food during the electrical shock response, despite the fact that food is one of the most influential signals for C. elegans, affecting many aspects of their behavior. For example, during the high speed state caused by high O2, animals still recognize and stay at the edge of a food lawn (Cheung et al., 2005; Coates & de Bono, 2002), suggesting that the electrical shock signal has a strong negative valence that overrides the strong positive valence of food. The third point is the scalability—stronger stimulus causes stronger behavioral response. Compared to the 30 V stimulus, the 75 V stimulus results in a larger number of immobile animals during the stimulus period (right panels in Figure 2A and B) as well as a longer high speed response after the stimulus (compare the panels for responses to 5 and 30 second stimulus in Figure 4A and C).
In summary, we found that C. elegans responds to electrical shock, which is regulated by VGCCs and neuropeptide signaling. Our findings may suggest the following model (Figure 7). When the animals sense 30 or 75V AC stimulus at 4 Hz, the stimulus is sensed with the L- and N-type VGCCs and their internal state transits from basal speed state to persistent high speed state. The persistent high speed state eventually returns to the basal speed state, which requires neuropeptide signaling. Thus, by studying the electrical responses of C. elegans, we will be able to investigate the mechanisms of animal electroception, persistent activity, and possibly a primitive form of emotion.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
C. elegans strains
C. elegans strains were maintained with standard procedures (Brenner, 1974). In brief, for regular cultivation, animals were grown on standard 6 cm nematode growth medium (NGM) agar plates which had been spread with E. coli strain OP50 and incubated at 19.0-19.5 °C. Strains used were the wild-type strain Bristol N2 and mutant strains PR678 tax-4(p678), CX4652 osm-9(ky10);ocr-2(ak47), CB1033 che-2(e1033), TU253 mec-4(u253), ZB2551 mec-10(tm1552), TQ296 trp-4(sy695), MT1212 egl-19(n582), DA995 egl-19(ad995), VC39 cca-1(ad1650), CB55 unc-2(e55), VC854 unc-2(gk366), KDK11 cat-2(tm2261), MT7988 bas-1(ad446), GR1321 tph-1(mg280), RB993 tdc-1(ok914), VC671 egl-3(ok979) and MT1219 egl-3(n589) .
C. elegans cultivation for electric shock behavioral assay
Before the behavioral assay, animals were cultivated as described previously (Kimura et al., 2010). In brief, four adult wild-type animals were placed onto NGM agar plates with OP50 and kept at 19.5°C for 7.5 hours before being removed. After removal, these plates were incubated at 19.5 °C for 3 days until the assay day. On the assay day, about 100 synchronized young adult animals were grown on each plate. As some mutant animals had slower growth or laid fewer eggs than wild-type animals did, the incubation temperature and number of these mutant animals were adjusted and increased accordingly in order to obtain a comparable developmental stage (i.e. young adult) with the wild-type animals. All behavioral assays were carried out with young adult hermaphrodites.
Experimental instruments for electric shock behavioral assay
The following electrical instruments (Figure 1) were utilized for the electric shock behavioral assay. A 50 MHz Arbitrary Waveform Generator (FGX-295, Texio Technology Corporation) was used to generate different types of electrical waveforms over a wide range of frequencies. However, this waveform generator has an output limit of 10 V. Thus, an AC Power Supply (PCR500MA, Kikusui Electronics Corp.) was used to amplify the voltage supply. We also used an Digital Storage Oscilloscope (DCS-1054B, Texio Technology Corporation) in parallel to measure the voltage and observe the electrical waveforms produced as well as a Digital Multimeter (PC720M, Sanwa Electric Instrument Co., Ltd.) to measure current. A USB camera (DMK72AUC02, The Imaging Source Co., Ltd.) with a lens (LM16JC5M2, Kowa) was used to record trajectories produced by the animals.
Electric shock behavioral assay with small OP50 bacterial food patch
Most of the behavioral assays were conducted on 9 cm NGM agar plates seeded with a small food patch unless indicated otherwise: For the food patch, the bacteria OP50 was grown in 100 mL of LB culture overnight at 37°C, spun down and resuspended in 10 volumes of NGM buffer, and 5 µL of the suspension was applied at the center of the plate with 3 × 10 mm in size on the assay day. This is to minimize the thickness of food patch as it prevents clear images of worms in the patch. Four animals per plate were placed in the food patch one hour before the assay to accustom the animals to the environment and to reduce their movement speed to the basal level. The assay plates were then inverted and placed onto a custom-made copper plate bridge, whose distance is 6 cm (Figure 1), the images were acquired 2 frames per s, and electric shock was delivered with the conditions described in each figure. Move- tr/2D software (Library Inc., Japan) was used to calculate the x-y coordinates of the animal centroids in each image frame, which were then analyzed using Excel (Microsoft) or R (The R Project). Baseline speed was calculated from the average speed over 30 s before the stimulation, and ΔSpeed was calculated by subtracting the baseline value from each animal’s speed during or after the stimulus.
Electric shock behavioral assay with full OP50 bacterial food lawn
For the assays conducted with full food lawn, the assay plates were seeded with OP50 and kept on the bench overnight until the assay began. Animals grown in regular cultivation plates were washed in two droplets of NGM buffer and then transferred to the center of the assay plate and left for 5 minutes. The rest of the procedures were the same as for assays conducted with small food patch.
Investigation of relationship between speed increase, current and voltage
Three different types of NGM agar plates were prepared with varying salt concentration and similar osmolarity: High-salt plates had 200 mM of sodium chloride; low-salt plates had 10 mM of sodium chloride and 380 mM of sucrose; control plates had 50 mM sodium chloride and 300 mM of sucrose. The purpose of adding sucrose into the plates was to adjust and balance the osmolarity. The final total osmolarity for sodium chloride (Na+ and Cl-) and sucrose, the osmo-regulator, for all the plates was 400 mOsm. The rest of the procedures were the same as for assays conducted with small food patch.
Data analysis and statistics
All the statistical analyses were performed in R (The R Project). Generally, data of 20 – 50 animals in total from 9 plates from 3 days of experiments for each condition were pooled and analyzed together. We chose this sample number based on a large scale behavioral analysis of C. elegans (Yemini et al., 2013). Data is presented as means ± SD unless otherwise specified.
FUNDING
This study was supported by Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (KAKENHI JP16H06545, 20H05700 and 21K19274 to K.D.K.), Grant-in-Aid for Research in Nagoya City University (48, 1912011, 1921102 and 2121101), the Joint Research by National Institutes of Natural Sciences (01112002), Toyoaki Scholarship Foundation, and RIKEN Center for Advanced Intelligence Project (to K.D.K).
COMPETING INTERESTS
The authors declare no competing interests.
SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURE LEGENDS
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT:
We thank Yuki Tanimoto and Yuka Tsuda for the initial phase of the electrical shock paradigm, Shinobu Aoyagi for setting up the system, and Young-Jay You, Aki Takahashi, and the Kimura lab members for their valuable advice, comments and technical assistance for the study. Nematode strains were provided by the Caenorhabditis Genetics Center (funded by the NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs P40 OD010440).