Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

Close encounters of three kinds: impacts of leg, wing, and body collisions on flight performance in carpenter bees

View ORCID ProfileNicholas P. Burnett, Stacey A. Combes
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.10.21.513269
Nicholas P. Burnett
1Department of Neurobiology, Physiology, and Behavior; University of California, Davis, CA 95616
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Nicholas P. Burnett
  • For correspondence: burnettnp@gmail.com
Stacey A. Combes
1Department of Neurobiology, Physiology, and Behavior; University of California, Davis, CA 95616
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Supplementary material
  • Data/Code
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

Flying insects often forage among cluttered vegetation that forms a series of obstacles in their flight path. Recent studies have focused on behaviors needed to navigate clutter while avoiding all physical contact, and as a result, we know little about flight behaviors that do involve encounters with obstacles. Here, we challenged carpenter bees (Xylocopa varipuncta) to fly through narrow gaps in an obstacle course to determine the kinds of obstacle encounters they experience, as well as the consequences for flight performance. We observed three kinds of encounters: leg, body, and wing collisions. Wing collisions occurred most frequently (in about 40% of flights, up to 25 times per flight) but these had little effect on flight speed or body orientation. In contrast, body and leg collisions, which each occurred in about 20% of flights (1-2 times per flight), resulted in decreased flight speeds and increased rates of body rotation (yaw). Wing and body collisions, but not leg collisions, were more likely to occur in wind versus still air. Thus, physical encounters with obstacles may be a frequent occurrence for insects flying in some environments, and the immediate effects of these encounters on flight performance depends on the body part involved.

Introduction

Flying animals frequently interact with cluttered vegetation in their habitats. Many birds, for instance, nest and perch in trees and pursue prey through dense foliage [1,2], and many insects forage for nectar and pollen among dense patches of flowers [3,4]. In each case, animals navigate around a series of vegetative structures that functionally serve as obstacles and constrain navigable paths [5,6]. Traversing obstacles while in flight requires coordinated detection of obstacles (e.g., visually) and rapid alteration of the flight path, for example by decelerating, accelerating, or changing body orientation [7–11]. However, most studies of obstacle traversal in flight focus on behaviors required to completely avoid obstacles, with little consideration of what happens when animals do make contact with obstacles (e.g., collisions). This contrasts with studies of terrestrial locomotion that consider obstacle encounters as an integral part of traversing terrestrial landscapes [12–14]. Thus, we know relatively little about the effects of physical encounters with obstacles on the performance of flying animals.

Encounters with obstacles can alter locomotory performance at the time of the collision and also lead to performance-altering injuries that may be immediate or cumulative. Intuitively, the effect of obstacle encounters seems likely to involve some ballistic component – i.e., an animal’s motion is redirected or slowed. However, the observed effect may deviate from intuition based on the animal’s mechanics and behavior, as well as details of the obstacle encounter, such as the initial motion of the animal and which body structures contact the obstacle [13]. Tolerance for collisions may vary between taxa – for instance, birds of prey can suffer bone fractures that eventually heal, whereas wing damage in insects is permanent [1,15]. Because wing damage can increase mortality in insects, the avoidance and consequences of wing collisions has been emphasized in many insect flight studies [9,10,16–18]. Furthermore, numerous insect species have wing morphologies that minimize damage by flexibly deforming during obstacle encounters, and these features have become the focus of studies aimed at extracting wing designs for bio-inspired flying vehicles [19–21]. As a result, our knowledge about obstacle encounters in flying insects is heavily focused on a specific anatomical structure, the wing, even though obstacle encounters and injuries can occur to other parts of the body [1]. We therefore know little about the full range and consequences of obstacle encounters that occur during insect flight.

Here, we use the Valley Carpenter Bee, Xylocopa varipuncta, to determine the types of obstacle encounters that can occur in flying insects and their consequences for performance. Among bees (family Apidae), carpenter bees in the genus Xylocopa are exceptionally large (wing span > 4 cm) and are important pollinators of crops and wild plants [22]. Thus, they commonly face the challenge of maneuvering a large body through dense foliage. Xylocopa spp. are also models for physiological and neurobiological studies due to their large flight muscles and visual acuity [22,23]. We used high-speed video cameras to film X. varipuncta flying through narrow gaps in an obstacle course with varying environmental conditions, including moving vs. stationary obstacles and wind vs. still air, in a laboratory flight tunnel. Using these data, we answered the following questions: (1) How frequent are different kinds of obstacle encounters?, What environmental factors affect the likelihood of encounters?, and (3) What are the performance consequences of each kind of encounter?

Methods

Female carpenter bees (Xylocopa varipuncta, n = 15) were collected from the University of California, Davis campus and used immediately in flight experiments. Individual bees were placed in a flight tunnel (20 × 19 × 115 cm; width x height x length) [10,24], which included a series of vertical obstacles that spanned the middle of the tunnel (obstacle diameter = 7 mm, space between obstacles = 34.44 ± 2.80 mm; mean ± SD). The bees’ wing spans (45.19 ± 2.11 mm, tip to tip) were larger than between obstacles; thus, bees needed to rotate their body (e.g., yaw) to pass between obstacles. Obstacles were attached to a mechanical arm that oscillated laterally (amplitude = 21 mm, frequency = 2 Hz) or remained stationary. Fans at each end of the tunnel could be turned on to produce a gentle breeze (mean velocity = 0.54 m/s) or off for still air. Wind direction was constant: bees flying in one direction experienced headwinds and in the other direction tailwinds. Up to 12 flights through the obstacles were elicited per bee, using full spectrum lights at each end of the tunnel [10,24]. Obstacle motion (stationary versus moving) was fixed for a given bee, but all bees experienced wind and still air, with wind condition switched after approximately six flights and the order of wind conditions alternated between bees. Thus, test conditions were still air with stationary obstacles (n = 40 flights) or moving obstacles (n = 34), and wind with stationary obstacles (n = 42) or moving obstacles (n = 29).

Flights were filmed at 1500 frames/s with two synchronized Phantom v611 cameras (Vision Research, Inc., Wayne, NJ, USA) positioned 30º from the vertical on opposite sides of the obstacles. Cameras were calibrated using a standard checkerboard calibration method and MATLAB functions [25,26]. In each video, the positions of the bee’s head (midpoint between antennae), thorax (approximating the body centroid), and wing tips were tracked with the machine-learning software DeepLabCut [27]. Tracked points were checked and manually corrected, and obstacle positions labeled using DLTdv6 in MATLAB [28]. Labeled positions were converted from two-dimensional coordinates in each camera view into three-dimensional space using MATLAB functions.

We classified and counted each obstacle encounter. The most common encounters were body collisions (head, thorax, or abdomen contacted obstacles), leg collisions (one or more forelegs contacted obstacles), and wing collisions (the distal half of one or more forewings contacted obstacles) (Fig. 1).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 1.

Types of obstacle encounters observed in carpenter bees. Top row: real examples of body, (b) leg, and (c) wing collisions in bees flying past obstacles from right to left (see supplementary movies). Black outlines show the moment of each encounter. Gray outlines show body positions 20 ms before and after encounters. White circles show obstacle positions. Bottom row: (d) frequencies of encounter types, and (e) frequencies grouped by wind condition and, for wing collisions, by flight direction. ‘Up’ and ‘Down’ refer to separate directions in the flight tunnel. (f) Number of wing collisions per flight (excluding flights without wing collisions), grouped by wind condition and obstacle motion. Brackets show statistical comparisons (p < 0.05 for significance; ‘n.s.’ = not significant). Only environmental factors (i.e., wind, flight direction, obstacle motion) retained by the models are shown.

To test which experimental conditions contributed to the occurrence of encounters, we used a generalized linear mixed-effects model (GLMM). Models were implemented as logistic regression models with the ‘glmer’ function in the R package lme4 [29], with variables for wind (yes versus no), flight direction (upstream versus downstream), and obstacle motion (moving versus stationary). Bee identity was included as a random effect to account for multiple observations per individual. We allowed for statistical interactions between all experimental variables and generated alternative models by removing terms in a stepwise selection process. Models were compared by their Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC) via the ‘AIC’ function in the R package stats [30]. Models with the lowest AIC were evaluated with the ‘Anova’ function from the R package car [30].

Among flights with obstacle encounters, there was wider variation in the number of wing collisions per flight (range = 1-25) compared to body collisions (range = 1-2) or leg collisions (maximum = 1). We tested which experimental variables best predicted the number of wing collisions per flight by using a GLMM with a Poisson distribution on flights with at least one wing collision. Model selection and evaluation were carried out as described above. Post-hoc comparisons of model terms were conducted with Tukey HSD tests using the ‘lsmeans’ function in the R package emmeans [33].

We assessed how obstacle encounters affected flight performance. In every video, we identified the first occurrence of each encounter type and defined a 20-ms period before and after each event. This temporal window allowed us to quantify performance immediately before and after encounters, as in [20]. Occasionally, pre- and post-encounter periods contained additional collisions, a common outcome when flying near clutter, but the narrow analysis window allowed us to examine changes in flight performance primarily occurring around the focal obstacle encounter. Videos yielded either one, two, or three encounter types (n = 42, 26, and 9 flights, respectively).

For each encounter, we measured the change in horizontal ground speed and yaw angle between the pre- and post-encounter periods, as well as the post-encounter yaw rate, where yaw angle was the body angle about the vertical axis. To calculate kinematics, we smoothed three-dimensional position data with cubic smoothing spline curves via the ‘smooth.spline’ function in stats. Horizontal ground speed was calculated as the change in x-y position (lateral and longitudinal movements, omitting vertical motion) per time. Yaw was calculated by converting the Cartesian coordinates of the head and thorax to spherical coordinates via the ‘cart2sph’ function in the R package pracma [31] and finding the horizontal angle between the body points and the tunnel’s long axis. Yaw rate was calculated as change in yaw per time.

We used a linear mixed-effects model to test whether the change in flight metrics depended on encounter type, wind condition, and/or obstacle motion. Models were implemented with the ‘lme’ function in the R package nlme [32]. Model selection, evaluation, and post-hoc comparisons were carried out as described above. Assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variances were checked with Shapiro’s Tests and Levene’s Tests, respectively. When necessary, variance structures of model terms were modified using the ‘varIdent’ function in nlme.

Results

Of the 145 recorded flights from Xylocopa varipuncta, 20.7% (n = 30) included a body collision, 21.4% (n = 31) included a leg collision, and 41.4% (n = 60) included a wing collision; overall, more than half of flights (53.1%, n = 68) included some type of obstacle encounter (Fig. 1d). Body collisions were more likely to occur in wind (frequency = 28.2%, n = 20/71 flights) versus still air (13.5%, n = 10/74 flights) (GLMM: χ2 = 4.673, df = 1, p = 0.031), whereas leg collisions showed similar frequencies between wind (18.3%, n =13/71 flights) and still air (24.3%, n = 18/74 flights; Fig. 2e) (χ2 = 1.426, df = 1, p = 0.232). Wing collision frequency depended on wind and flight direction (χ2 = 6.341, df = 1, p = 0.012), such that flights in headwinds (but not in tailwinds) were more likely to contain wing collisions (58.1%, n = 25/43 flights) than flights in the same direction with still air (23.3%, n = 7/30 flights; Fig. 2e) (Tukey HSD tests: p = 0.013). Notably, our AIC-based model selection process indicated that obstacle motion was not a strong predictor of the likelihood of any encounter type occurring.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 2.

Effects of obstacle encounters on flight. Changes in (a) horizontal ground speed and (b) yaw angle between the pre- and post-encounter periods, and (c) post-encounter yaw rate (n = 30 body collisions, 31 leg collisions, 60 wing collisions). Increases in yaw (b) indicate rotations away from the tunnel’s centerline. Brackets show statistical comparisons (p < 0.05 for significance; ‘n.s.’ = not significant).

Among flights with wing collisions, the number of wing collisions per flight depended on wind and obstacle motion (χ2 = 7.011, df = 1, p = 0.008; Fig. 1f). Flights in still air with stationary obstacles had fewer wing collisions (5.3 ± 3.5 wing collisions; mean ± SD) than flights in still air with moving obstacles (9.9 ± 6.5 wing collisions) (Tukey HSD test: p = 0.006) or flights in wind with moving obstacles (8.6 ± 4.8 wing collisions) (p = 0.046).

Body, leg, and wing collisions (Fig. 1a-c) each had distinctive effects on ground speed, with large decreases in speed after body collisions (−0.07 ± 0.08 m/s), small decreases after leg collisions (−0.02 ± 0.05 m/s), and small increases after wing collisions (0.01 ± 0.04 m/s) (Fig. 2a)(χ2 = 25.896, df = 2, p < 0.005; Tukey HSD tests: p < 0.05).

Changes in bees’ yaw angle resulting from collisions were not affected by encounter type (χ2 = 0.574, df = 2, p = 0.751) (Fig. 2b) but were affected by wind, with larger changes in yaw angle for bees flying in still air (7.29 ± 6.12º) versus wind (3.96 ± 10.33º) (χ2 = 7.378, df = 1, p = 0.007). Post-encounter yaw rate depended on encounter type (χ2 = 21.284, df = 2, p < 0.005) (Fig. 2c), with slower rates after wing collisions (305.6 [333.6] º/s; median [interquartile range]) versus body collisions (501.5 [491.4] º/s) (Tukey HSD test: p = 0.020) and leg collisions (799.2 [440.3] º/s) (p < 0.005) (Fig. 2c).

Discussion

We found that carpenter bees experienced three distinct types of obstacle encounters when traversing challenging flight environments. Wing collisions were the most frequent encounter (occurring in about 40% of flights), but body and leg collisions each occurred in approximately 20% of flights as well. These results suggest that collisions with obstacles may be common for insects flying through natural, complex environments, such as cluttered vegetation, and that these encounters are diverse – both in terms of the environmental conditions that increase their likelihood and in their effects on flight performance.

Wind increased the likelihood of some encounters, but this effect was not uniform. Body collisions occurred more frequently in wind, but leg collisions were not affected – and wing collisions occurred more frequently only for bees flying into a headwind. Obstacle motion did not affect the likelihood of any type of encounter, but obstacle motion (in still air or in wind) did lead to a greater number of wing collisions for flights in which wing collisions occurred. Given that our experiment tested only one obstacle arrangement, one type of obstacle motion, and one mild wind speed, it is clear that additional studies would greatly improve our understanding of how environmental variables affect the likelihood and consequences of obstacle encounters.

Most insect flight studies focus on collision-free flight, yet in our study more than half of the flights recorded (53.1%) contained at least one type of collision, suggesting that additional research is needed on flights involving obstacle encounters. In addition, most previous studies addressing collisions have focused only on wing collisions, in part because wing collisions lead to cumulative damage that can impair flight performance over time. However, our work suggests that other types of obstacle encounters, such as body and leg collisions, may have more important immediate effects on flight performance, leading to larger reductions in flight speed and larger changes in body rotation rates than wing collisions. Overall, our work suggests that the incidence, diversity, and potential effects of obstacle encounters experienced by flying insects remains vastly underexplored in the scientific literature.

Data accessibility

Data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository https://doi.org/10.25338/B8M939.

Conflict of interest declaration

We declare we have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

Nicholas P. Burnett: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Formal analysis, Data curation, Writing – original draft, Visualization, Funding acquisition. Stacey A. Combes: Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Project administration, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Science Foundation [1711980 to N.P.B.].

Acknowledgements

The authors thank M. Badger for assistance with DeepLabCut and MATLAB.

Footnotes

  • https://doi.org/10.25338/B8M939

References

  1. 1.↵
    Roth AJ, Jones GS, French TW. 2002 Incidence of naturally-healed fractures in the pectoral bones of North American accipiters. J Raptor Res 36, 229–230.
    OpenUrl
  2. 2.↵
    Robinson SK, Holmes RT. 1984 Effects of plant species and foliage structure on the foraging behavior of forest birds. The Auk 101, 672–684. (doi:10.2307/4086894)
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  3. 3.↵
    Comba L. 1999 Patch use by bumblebees (Hymenoptera Apidae): temperature, wind, flower density and traplining. Ethol Ecol Evol 11, 243–264. (doi:10.1080/08927014.1999.9522826)
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  4. 4.↵
    Hennessy G, Harris C, Eaton C, Wright P, Jackson E, Goulson D, Ratnieks Fflw. 2020 Gone with the wind: effects of wind on honey bee visit rate and foraging behaviour. Anim Behav 161, 23–31. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.12.018)
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  5. 5.↵
    Ravi S, Siesenop T, Bertrand O, Li L, Doussot C, Warren WH, Combes SA, Egelhaaf M. 2020 Bumblebees perceive the spatial layout of their environment in relation to their body size and form to minimize inflight collisions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 117, 31494–31499. (doi:10.1073/pnas.2016872117)
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. 6.↵
    Ravi S, Siesenop T, Bertrand O, Li L, Doussot C, Fisher A, Warren WH, Egelhaaf M. 2022 Bumblebees display characteristics of active vision during robust obstacle avoidance flight. J Exp Biol, jeb.243021. (doi:10.1242/jeb.243021)
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  7. 7.↵
    Baird E, Dacke M. 2012 Visual flight control in naturalistic and artificial environments. J Comp Physiol A 198, 869–876. (doi:10.1007/s00359-012-0757-7)
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. 8.
    Baird E, Dacke M. 2016 Finding the gap: a brightness-based strategy for guidance in cluttered environments. Proc R Soc B 283, 20152988. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.2988)
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. 9.↵
    Crall JD, Ravi S, Mountcastle AM, Combes SA. 2015 Bumblebee flight performance in cluttered environments: effects of obstacle orientation, body size and acceleration. Journal of Experimental Biology 218, 2728–2737. (doi:10.1242/jeb.121293)
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. 10.↵
    Burnett NP, Badger MA, Combes SA. 2020 Wind and obstacle motion affect honeybee flight strategies in cluttered environments. J Exp Biol 223, jeb222471. (doi:10.1242/jeb.222471)
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. 11.↵
    Fabian ST, Sumner ME, Wardill TJ, Gonzalez-Bellido PT. 2022 Avoiding obstacles while intercepting a moving target: a miniature fly’s solution. J Exp Biol 225, jeb243568. (doi:10.1242/jeb.243568)
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  12. 12.↵
    Jayaram K, Full RJ. 2016 Cockroaches traverse crevices, crawl rapidly in confined spaces, and inspire a soft, legged robot. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1514591113)
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. 13.↵
    Jayaram K, Mongeau J-M, Mohapatra A, Birkmeyer P, Fearing RS, Full RJ. 2018 Transition by head-on collision: mechanically mediated manoeuvres in cockroaches and small robots. J R Soc Interface 15, 20170664. (doi:10.1098/rsif.2017.0664)
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. 14.↵
    Wang Y, Othayoth R, Li C. 2022 Cockroaches adjust body and appendages to traverse cluttered large obstacles. J Exp Biol 225, jeb243605. (doi:10.1242/jeb.243605)
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  15. 15.↵
    Foster DJ, Cartar RV. 2011 What causes wing wear in foraging bumble bees? J Exp Biol 214, 1896–1901. (doi:10.1242/jeb.051730)
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. 16.↵
    Mountcastle AM, Combes SA. 2014 Biomechanical strategies for mitigating collision damage in insect wings: structural design versus embedded elastic materials. J Exp Biol 217, 1108–1115. (doi:10.1242/jeb.092916)
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. 17.
    Mountcastle AM, Alexander TM, Switzer CM, Combes SA. 2016 Wing wear reduces bumblebee flight performance in a dynamic obstacle course. Biol Lett 12, 20160294. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2016.0294)
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. 18.↵
    Ravi S, Bertrand O, Siesenop T, Manz L-S, Doussot C, Fisher A, Egelhaaf M. 2019 Gap perception in bumblebees. J Exp Biol 222, jeb184135. (doi:10.1242/jeb.184135)
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. 19.↵
    1. R Blockley,
    2. W Shyy
    Combes SA. 2010 Materials, Structure, and Dynamics of Insect Wings as Bioinspiration for MAVs. In Encyclopedia of Aerospace Engineering (eds R Blockley, W Shyy), p. eae404. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. (doi:10.1002/9780470686652.eae404)
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  20. 20.↵
    Mountcastle AM, Helbling EF, Wood RJ. 2019 An insect-inspired collapsible wing hinge dampens collision-induced body rotation rates in a microrobot. J R Soc Interface 16, 20180618. (doi:10.1098/rsif.2018.0618)
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. 21.↵
    Phan HV, Park HC. 2020 Mechanisms of collision recovery in flying beetles and flappingwing robots. Science 370, 1214–1219. (doi:10.1126/science.abd3285)
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. 22.↵
    Somanathan H, Saryan P, Balamurali GS. 2019 Foraging strategies and physiological adaptations in large carpenter bees. J Comp Physiol A 205, 387–398. (doi:10.1007/s00359-019-01323-7)
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  23. 23.↵
    Roberts SP, Harrison JF, Dudley R. 2004 Allometry of kinematics and energetics in carpenter bees (Xylocopa varipuncta) hovering in variable-density gases. J Exp Biol 207, 993–1004. (doi:10.1242/jeb.00850)
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  24. 24.↵
    Burnett NP, Badger MA, Combes SA. 2022 Wind and route choice affect performance of bees flying above versus within a cluttered obstacle field. PLoS ONE 17, e0265911. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0265911)
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. 25.↵
    Heikkila J, Silven O. 1997 A four-step camera calibration procedure with implicit image correction. In Proceedings of IEEE Computer Society Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition, pp. 1106–1112. San Juan, Puerto Rico: IEEE Comput. Soc. (doi:10.1109/CVPR.1997.609468)
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  26. 26.↵
    Zhang Z. 2000 A flexible new technique for camera calibration. IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Machine Intell 22, 1330–1334. (doi:10.1109/34.888718)
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  27. 27.↵
    Mathis A, Mamidanna P, Cury KM, Abe T, Murthy VN, Mathis MW, Bethge M. 2018 DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat Neurosci 21, 1281–1289. (doi:10.1038/s41593-018-0209-y)
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. 28.↵
    Hedrick TL. 2008 Software techniques for two- and three-dimensional kinematic measurements of biological and biomimetic systems. Bioinspir Biomim 3, 034001. (doi:10.1088/1748-3182/3/3/034001)
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. 29.↵
    Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. 2015 Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Soft 67. (doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01)
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. 30.↵
    R Core Team. 2020 R: A language and environment for statistical computing.
  31. 31.↵
    Borchers HW. 2021 pracma: practical numerical math functions.
  32. 32.↵
    Pinheiro J, DebRoy S, R Core Team. 2020 nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models.
  33. 33.↵
    Lenth RV. 2022 emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squared means.
  34. 34.
    van Breugel F, Dickinson MH. 2012 The visual control of landing and obstacle avoidance in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. J Exp Biol 215, 1783–1798. (doi:10.1242/jeb.066498)
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. 35.
    Fuller SB, Straw AD, Peek MY, Murray RM, Dickinson MH. 2014 Flying Drosophila stabilize their vision-based velocity controller by sensing wind with their antennae. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111, E1182–E1191. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1323529111)
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. 36.
    Chang JJ, Crall JD, Combes SA. 2016 Wind alters landing dynamics in bumblebees. J Exp Biol 219, 2819–2822. (doi:10.1242/jeb.137976)
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted October 24, 2022.
Download PDF

Supplementary Material

Data/Code
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Close encounters of three kinds: impacts of leg, wing, and body collisions on flight performance in carpenter bees
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Close encounters of three kinds: impacts of leg, wing, and body collisions on flight performance in carpenter bees
Nicholas P. Burnett, Stacey A. Combes
bioRxiv 2022.10.21.513269; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.10.21.513269
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo LinkedIn logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Close encounters of three kinds: impacts of leg, wing, and body collisions on flight performance in carpenter bees
Nicholas P. Burnett, Stacey A. Combes
bioRxiv 2022.10.21.513269; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.10.21.513269

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Zoology
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (4229)
  • Biochemistry (9118)
  • Bioengineering (6753)
  • Bioinformatics (23949)
  • Biophysics (12103)
  • Cancer Biology (9498)
  • Cell Biology (13746)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (7618)
  • Ecology (11666)
  • Epidemiology (2066)
  • Evolutionary Biology (15479)
  • Genetics (10621)
  • Genomics (14298)
  • Immunology (9468)
  • Microbiology (22808)
  • Molecular Biology (9083)
  • Neuroscience (48900)
  • Paleontology (355)
  • Pathology (1479)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (2566)
  • Physiology (3828)
  • Plant Biology (8320)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (1467)
  • Synthetic Biology (2294)
  • Systems Biology (6172)
  • Zoology (1297)