Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

Revealing how memory shapes time-dependent computational processes in plant tropisms

View ORCID ProfileMathieu Rivière, View ORCID ProfileYasmine Meroz
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.01.06.522981
Mathieu Rivière
1Faculty of Life Sciences, School of Plant Science and Food Security, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Mathieu Rivière
Yasmine Meroz
1Faculty of Life Sciences, School of Plant Science and Food Security, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Yasmine Meroz
  • For correspondence: jazz@tauex.tau.ac.il
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Supplementary material
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

Mounting evidence suggests that plants engage complex computational processes to quantify and integrate sensory information over time, enabling remarkable adaptive growth strategies. However, quantitative understanding of these computational processes is limited. We report experiments probing the dependence of gravitropic responses of wheat coleoptiles on previous stimuli. First, building on a mathematical model that identifies this dependence as a form of memory, we use experimental observations to reveal the mathematical principles of how coleoptiles integrate multiple stimuli over time. Next, we perform two-stimulus experiments, informed by model predictions, to reveal fundamental computational processes. We quantitatively show that coleoptiles respond not only to sums but also to differences between stimuli over different timescales. This result constitutes first evidence that plants can compare stimuli, crucial for search processes.

One sentence summary Quantitative evidence that plants effectively sum and subtract stimuli over different timescales

Introduction

Plants, decentralized systems, negotiate fluctuating environments by adapting their morphology advantageously through growth-driven processes called tropisms, whereby organs such as shoots or roots redirect their growth according to directional stimuli, such as gravity or light. The last decade has seen an increasing body of evidence that plants quantify and integrate sensory information about their environment at the tissue level (1–16), detecting and climbing spatial signal gradients despite environmental and 8 internally-driven noise (17). While it is generally accepted that tropic responses are a product of complex computational processes (1, 14, 18–22), attempts to characterize these processes mathematically—that is, to obtain an understanding of the rules according to which plants quantify and process the sensory information they acquire over time—are limited. There is a need for comprehensive experimental and mathematical frameworks that can be used, respectively, to derive quantitative observations regarding plants’ growth in response to various stimuli, and to decode the underlying principles.

Here, we report experiments that probe the dependence of gravitropic responses of wheat coleoptiles on the presence of previous stimuli, building on previous findings that plant shoots respond to the integrated history of stimuli rather than responding instantaneously (13,23–29). Our work comprises two phases. First, we subject coleoptiles to a single gravitational stimulus, of varying duration, and record their gravitropic response over time. From these observations we extract a functional representation of the collection of computational processes underlying plants’ gravitropic responses. This phase relies on a mathematical model that was previously developed to describe temporal integration in plant tropisms, grounded in Response Theory (see below) (12,13). In the second phase of this study, on the basis of the characteristic shape of the extracted function, we make predictions (supported by model simulations) regarding how plants integrate multiple stimuli over different timescales. We then conduct two-stimulus experiments which corroborate our predictions. These results provide the first quantitative evidence that coleoptiles effectively respond not only to the sums of stimuli, as previously reported (13, 23–29), but also to the differences between stimuli over different timescales. The latter suggests that plants have the ability to compare signals—a critical capability in search processes.

Results

Fig. 1a shows an example of the evolution of the gravitropic response of a wheat coleoptile tilted horizontally on a platform, redirecting its growth in the direction of gravity. For a given snapshot at time t we represent the coleoptile’s geometrical shape relative to the frame of reference of the platform, described in Fig. 1a. The shape is defined by (i) the local angle from the vertical θ(s, t) at point s along the organ, where s = 0 at the base and s = L at the tip, and (ii) the local curvature Embedded Image, which is the rate of change of the angle along the organ. We denote the angle at the base θbase, the angle at the tip θtip, and the angle of the stimulus θP, here the direction of the platform relative to gravity.

Figure 1:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 1: Transient gravistimulation of wheat coleoptiles.

(A) Typical gravitropic response of a wheat coleoptile to an inclination of θP = π/2 with respect to the direction of gravity Embedded Image (snapshots taken every 2 hrs). After 6 hrs the tip is aligned with the direction of gravity. Coleoptiles are described in the frame of the rotating platform (Embedded Image, ŷ). The arclength s spans the whole midline, with s = 0 at the base and s = L at the tip, where L is the organ length. The local angle relative to ŷ is given by θ(s, t). In addition, we denote the angle at the base θ(s = 0, t) = θbase, and the angle at the tip θ(s = L, t) = θtip. (B) Sketch of the experimental protocol. Coleoptiles are attached to the rotating platform and are either tilted once (θP = π/2) for a duration τs and then tilted back to vertical position, or receive two successive stimuli of equal duration τs delayed by τd.

The mathematical model at the basis of our approach (12,13) incorporates concepts from Response Theory, commonly used in the description of input-output systems, or signal transducers (30). In its simplest linear form, the response y (t) of a system is described as a weighted integral of the history of stimuli x (t) over time, where the weight is given by the response function, or memory kernel, µ(t), following Embedded Image In simple cases where the dynamical equation describing an input-output system is known, the solution to the equation can be rewritten in the form described in Eq. 1, and the memory kernel is directly related to the original equation (30). Response Theory is however particularly useful for describing systems in which the dynamical equations are not known, such as complex biological systems (31–34). In principle, the form of the memory kernel in such formulations is a mathematical representation of underlying equations, a transfer function, agnostic of the building blocks of the system.

Approaching the case of plant gravitropism through the lens of Response Theory, we identify the dependence of gravitropic responses on past stimuli as a form of memory, captured by the memory kernel. Examples of memory formation at the tissue level are widespread (18, 21, 22, 35, 36), even in non-biological matter (37, 38). Here we assume that the input signal depends on the relative angle between the organ and the direction of gravity, following the well-known sine law sin (θ(s, t) − θP) (39, 40). The signal is zero if the organ is aligned with gravity, and maximal if it is perpendicular to it. We assume the system is linear (verified in Fig. S1), and following Eq. 1 we convolute the signal with the memory kernel µ(t), yielding the transduced signal as an output. This transduced signal, in turn, dictates the tropic dynamics following (12): Embedded Image The tropic dynamics are represented by the change of curvature in time ∂κ(s, t)/∂t, and is driven by the transduced signal due to a varying inclination angle θP(t), and a relaxation term ϒκ(s, t) associated with proprioception – the tendency of a plant to grow straight in the absence of any external stimuli (41). The coefficients β and ϒ are, respectively, the gravitropic and proprioceptive sensitivities or gains, which vary across species (41). Growth is considered as the implicit driver of the tropic response, and is not explicitly taken into account. Eq. 2 therefore holds within the growth zone of length Lgz from the tip. In the mature zone, where no growth-driven bending can occur, the equation reduces to Embedded Image.

Here, the memory kernel represents the dynamics of a hierarchy of stochastic processes underpinning gravitropic responses (12, 13, 19), the details of which are currently not well understood (42), such as statolith sedimentation in gravity-sensing cells, and the ensuing asymmetrical redistribution of PIN proteins and the growth hormone auxin (43). Previous studies adopting a Response Theory approach to model tropic responses assumed an arbitrary form of the memory kernel, and showed that the model qualitatively reproduced observations of temporal integration of multiple stimuli over limited timescales (12, 13), however could not make quantitative predictions, or explain negative responses observed for transient stimuli at longer times (13). To obtain a quantitative picture of plants’ computational capabilities, in terms of quantifying and processing sensory information, it is necessary to extract the mathematical form of the memory kernel.

Accordingly, in the first phase of this study, we sought to extract the memory kernel by probing the dependence of gravitropic responses on the history of stimuli. To this end, we exposed wheat coleoptiles to transient gravistimulation protocols. Coleoptiles were placed vertically on a platform, which inclined horizontally for a stimulus duration τs, then rotated back to the vertical (shown schematically in Fig. 1b). We recorded the gravitropic responses of coleoptiles to different values of stimulus duration, ranging from τs = 6 min, the shortest to elicit a clear response, to a permanent stimulus τs = ∞ (in practice, τs = 10 hours). The average gravitropic response, represented by the average evolution of the tip angle θtip(t), are shown in Fig. 2a for the limiting cases of τs = 6 and τs = ∞. Fig. 2b shows the normalized response Embedded Image (44) for the same two examples. The normalized response accounts for variations in growth rate Embedded Image and organ radius R, allowing us to better compare experimental trajectories to simulations later on. For each response trajectory, of a single coleoptile, we numerically solved Eq. 2, and extracted the mathematical form of the memory kernel (elaborated in Methods), substituting the measured values of θ(s, t) and κ(s, t), and the stimulus profile with θP(t) = π/2 during the stimulus duration τs, and θP(t) = 0 otherwise. For each stimulus duration τs we averaged over the extracted memory kernels, and the average kernels µτs (t) are shown in Fig. 2c. In order to verify our model accurately captures the average response over time, we simulated the gravitropic response for each τs, based on the corresponding average memory kernel Embedded Image. We compared the simulations to the average tip angle responses and normalized responses. These were found to be in excellent agreement, as shown in the examples in Fig. 2a-b. In the SM we show that the assumption of temporal linearity holds (Fig. S1), and that the response is robust to small perturbations in the extracted graviceptive and proprioceptive gains β and ϒ (Fig. S2), as well as perturbations in τs (Fig. S3).

Figure 2:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 2: Estimated memory kernels are consistent with experiments and capture the dynamics of gravitropism.

(A) The average tip angle trajectory θtip(t)− θbase (t) of coleoptiles inclined for a short stimulus time τs = 6 min (solid blue) and permanent stimulus τs → ∞ (solid red). Shaded areas represent standard deviation. Simulated tip dynamics (dashed lines), based on average memory kernels estimated from the respective data, µ6 and µ∞, agree with experimental data. (B) Similar comparison for the normalized gravitropic response Δ(t) = R∂t (θtip− θbase)/∂t L. Δmax is the maximal response after the stimulus. (C) Averaged memory kernels extracted from experiments with various values of τs. (D). Maximum gravitropic response Δmax for increasing stimulus duration τs. Values increase for short stimulus times, and saturate at longer times. Each experimental value (black dots) was obtained from an average with N ≥ 30 repetitions, errors represent standard deviation. The empty dot represents a permanent stimulus. Simulations based on the limiting memory kernels extracted from responses to the shortest and longest stimulus times, τs = 6 min (blue) and τs → ∞ (red), capture both regimes.

We observe that all memory kernels extracted from responses to different τs (Fig. 2c), exhibit similar characteristics; a positive peak followed by a negative peak, which then goes to zero. The timescales and magnitudes of the two peaks differ slightly across kernels (detailed in the SM), suggesting that the memory kernel probes different underlying biological processes. This is in line with arguments tying the dependence of gravitropic responses on stimulus duration (13) to the different timescales of underlying processes, from statolith sedimentation, dynamics of PIN proteins and auxin transport, and the ensuing growth response (42). Following Chauvet et al. (13), we plotted the maximal values of the average normalized responses Δmax = max {Δ(t)} for different τs (Fig. 2d). We recovered two regimes, namely, increasing responses for short τs, and saturation at long τs (13). We examined the dependence of the gravitropic responses on the form of the memory kernels, finding that simulations of responses to the range of τs, using only the limiting memory kernels µ6 and µ∞ extracted from the responses to τs = 6 min and τs = ∞, recovered the behavior in both regimes (Fig. 2d). This result further corroborates our model and the extracted memory kernels.

In the second phase of our study, we sought to gain an understanding of the specific computational processes represented by the characteristic form of the extracted memory kernel. To this end, we examined coleoptiles’ gravitropic responses to two consecutive stimuli of τs = 6 min each, separated by a delay time τd (Fig. 1b). For clarity, in what follows we will denote the responses to single stimuli or double stimuli as Δ1(t) and Δ2(t) respectively. To provide intuition as to the patterns we might expect to see for different values of τd, and the computational processes they reveal, Figs. 3a-c show schematics of stimuli with different timescales overlaid on a characteristic memory kernel. For short τd, both stimuli fit within the first positive peak, and are both weighted positively (Fig. 3a). The coleoptile is therefore expected to respond to the weighted sum of the two stimuli; i.e., the response to the second stimulus is expected to be stronger than the response to a single stimulus. Fig. 3d shows the average experimental response Δ2(t) for τd = 10 min, well within the timescale of the first peak (compare to µ6 in Fig. 2c). For reference, the two stimuli are marked with blue bars, as well as the maximal response Embedded Image for a single stimulus τs = 6 min represented by a vertical line (taken from Fig. 2b). The maximal value Embedded Image after the second stimulus is significantly higher than the response to a single stimulus Embedded Image (illustrated by an arrow), verifying the prediction of our model. For intermediate τd, the second stimulus occurs within the positive peak and the first stimulus within the negative peak, and is therefore multiplied by a negative value (Fig. 3b). The coleoptile is therefore expected to respond to the weighted difference of the two stimuli; i.e., the response to the second stimulus is expected to be weaker than the response to a single stimulus. This prediction is experimentally verified for τd = 75 min (Fig. 3e). Finally, for τd longer than the timescale of µ(t), while the second stimulus occurs within the positive peak, the first stimulus is multiplied by a null value of µ(t) (Fig. 3c), and is effectively forgotten. The response to the second stimulus is expected to be similar to that of a single stimulus, corroborated in Fig. 3f for τd = 10 hr.

Figure 3:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 3: Model prediction and experimental observations of summation and subtraction regimes in two-stimulus experiments.

(A–C) Graphical illustration of µ (blue line) compared to two stimuli (dashed bars) separated by different delay times τd. Shadowed areas correspond to stimuli weighted by µ. (A) For short τd, both stimuli occur within first peak, and are weighted positively. Coleoptiles are expected to respond to the weighted sum of stimuli, with a stronger response than for a single stimulus: verified in (D) the experimental response Δ2(t) for τd = 10 min (compare to µ6 in Fig. 1c). For reference, the dashed line represents Embedded Image for single stimulus. (B) For intermediate τd, second stimulus occurs within the positive peak, weighted positively, and the first within the negative peak, weighted negatively. Coleoptiles expected to respond to weighted difference between stimuli, with a weaker response compared to a single stimulus: verified in (E), the response for τd = 75 min, roughly the time between the two peaks in µ6. (C) For τd larger than the timescale of µ, the second stimulus occurs within the positive peak, weighted positively, while the second is multiplied by zero. The response is expected to be similar to that for a single stimulus: verified in (F), for τd = 5 hrs. (G) Maximal response of two-stimulus experiments relative to single stimulus Embedded Image, for a range of τd. Dots represent experimental data, and the blue line represents the simulated response based on the extracted kernel µ6. Shadowed area delineates the three regimes predicted in (A—C).

We extended these two-stimulus experiments, in addition to simulations, for a range of values of τd between 0 and 3 hours, spanning the timescale of µ6. In Fig. 3g we report the values of the maximal response Embedded Image following the second stimulus, relative to Embedded Image as a reference, as a function of the delay time τd, both for experiments and simulations.

Our two-stimulus experiments, supported by simulations, reveal the three regimes we predicted earlier based on the form of the extracted memory kernel, illustrated in Fig. 3a-c: (i) for short τd coleoptiles respond to the weighted sum of stimuli, in line with previous observations of temporal integration (12, 13, 25–29), and the response to the recent stimulus is stronger than for a single stimulus; (ii) for τd of the order of the time between peaks, coleoptiles respond to the weighted difference of stimuli, and the response is weaker; and (iii) for longer τd the coleoptiles respond to the recent stimulus alone. In the SM we show that these results are not an artifact of the relative orientation of the organs at the time of the second stimulus (Fig. S4). We also show that, while it is possible that proprioception may have a role in the observed memory kernel, a neglected kernel in the proprioceptive term would not significantly affect the extracted gravitropic kernel for similar timescales (Fig. S5).

Discussion

Together, these findings provide the first quantitative evidence that plants respond to the sum and difference of stimuli over different timescales, framed within a general mathematical framework. These computational processes can be identified as fundamental elements of natural search algorithms, common across diverse biological organisms (45). The summation of stimuli is effectively a moving average, or a lowpass filter, improving the signal-to-noise ratio of an environmental signal. Subtraction of stimuli, which has never been reported before in plants, is required in order to compare signals over time, a strategy commonly used by a variety of living organisms in order to detect and climb signal gradients (46). For example, Segall et al. (47, 48) extracted a memory kernel describing the chemotactic response of bacteria to a chemical stimulus, and found that it, too, was characterized by a positive peak followed by a negative one. On this basis they found that bacteria compare chemical concentrations sampled over time, enabling them to identify chemical gradients.

Within this context, our findings suggest that plant shoots may detect a light gradient by measuring changes in light intensity over time as they circumnutate, or enhance posture control during growth by measuring changes in their organ angle relative to the direction of gravity (14). Put together, our study provides the backdrop for understanding how plants may combine memory and movement in order to enhance their sensing capabilities in the face of weak signals and fluctuating environments.

Materials and Methods

Wheat growth conditions

Wheat coleoptiles were grown as described in (13, 44). Wild-type wheat seeds (Triticum aestivum cv. Ruta) were initially glued with floral glue to plastic boxes with draining holes at the bottom (germ pointing down). The boxes were filled with cotton and placed in a light-proof container, immersed in water. Coleoptiles were kept in a growth chamber at a temperature of 24 °C for 3 to 4 days. Coleoptiles were manipulated in darkness or dim green light to minimize exposure to actinic light.

Experimental setup

Our experimental apparatus consisted of an upright rotating platform holding 3D-printed casings each hosting a coleoptile box. A DC motor inclined the platform, and the inclination angle was measured and controlled with two optical fork sensors and a custom 3D-printed encoder wheel. During experiments images were taken every 1, 5 or 10 min, depending on the experiment, using a DSLR camera with a wide-angle lens, controlled by the Arduino. Pictures were taken with a dim green flash of 333 ms, placed perpendicular to the plane of motion. The setup was controlled by an Arduino microcontroller board running an in-house developed program. Each experiment started by placing upright coleoptile boxes on the rotating platform, and waiting for an acclimation phase of 10 h in the dark. Coleoptiles were then gravistimulated by rotating the platform horizontally, θp = π/2, during a stimulation time τs, and then rotated back to the vertical position. We employed two types of protocols (schematically illustrated in Fig. 1B): (i) single stimulus, for a range of τs values (in minutes: 1, 3, 5, 6, 12, 24, 35, 45, 60, 90 and 600), (ii) double stimulus; apply two successive stimuli of τs = 6 min each, separated by a delay time τd (in minutes: 1, 3, 4.5, 6, 10, 15, 24, 30, 36, 45, 60, 75, 90, 105, 120, 150, 180, 210 and 240). The rotation of the platform takes 6 seconds to tilt horizontally. Each experiment included at least 30 repetitions.

Data collection

As described in Fig. 1a, the local frame of the rotating platform is (Embedded Image, ŷ), where ŷ is parallel to the upright position of coleoptiles. The angle of the stimulus θp is defined as the angle between the vertical of the platform and the direction of gravity, cos θp(t) = ĝ · ŷ(t). We extracted the center line of each coleoptile, for each image, using a version of the Python-based software Interekt (49). From the center line we extracted the local angle θ(s, t) of the coleoptile with respect to ŷ, the apical and basal angles θtip(t) = θ(L, t) and θbase(t) = θ(0, t), the total length L(t), and the radius averaged over s, R(t). The quantities θtip and θbase were obtained by locally averaging θ(s) over 2 mm, i.e. less than Lgz/10 where Lgz is the growth zone. This length scale also sets the minimum required size for a coleoptile to be included in our analysis, 4 mm. We also calculated ∫⊥(t) = ⎰ds sin (θ(s, t) − θP(t)), the projection of the organ perpendicular to ĝ. These quantities were further smoothed with a combination of a median filter and a lowpass filter to remove aberrant data points and allow the computation of smooth time derivatives.

Numerical simulations

We ran numerical simulations implementing a discretized version of Eq. 2. The code was written in python and based on (12), however allowing to implement any memory kernel or inclination protocol (including transient tilts or clinostat). Required parameters, such as µ(t), β, ϒ, L, and θ(s, t = 0), were extracted from experiments (Fig. S6).

Estimation of gravitropic and proprioceptive gains β and ϒ

The estimation of the β and ϒ was carried out from the permanent stimulation experiments. In order to compare the dynamics of the growing coleoptiles with our model and simulations, which do not take growth into account explicitly, we consider the normalized gravitropic response defined as Δ(t) = ∂t (θtip − θbase) · R/vg, where R is the radius of the organ and vg = dL/dt is the growth rate. The maximal value is termed the dimensionless gravitropic sensitivity Embedded Image (44, 49, 50). In the limit of negligible growth we have Embedded Image where Embedded Image is the average elongation rate of the plant organ and Lgz the length of its growth zone (41). ϒ is then extracted from the convergence length Lc = ϒ/β, found by fitting the steady-state angular profile θ(s) of each organ to an exponential (41): θ(s) = Θ − (1− exp ((s −s0)/Lc)) + θ0. For each plant, the fit was repeated and averaged over the last 100 min of the experiment. When s0 > 0 we can approximate Lgz≈ L − s0. Extracted values are found in Fig. S6 in the SM.

Memory kernel estimation

In the case of transient inclination, the gravitropic sensitivity β is assumed constant, while the inclination angle θP(t) is time-dependent. Integrating Eq. 2 over s, and assuming gravistimulation is zero for t < 0, yields: Embedded Image. This can then be rewritten as a Volterra integral equation of the first kind on µ with 𝓁⊥ as a kernel: Embedded Image, where g = 1/β (∂t + ϒ) (θtip + θbase). This integral equation is then solved in the classical way by first turning it into a Volterra integral of the second kind by differentiating it with respect to time, and then discretizing it (51). The initial value is Embedded Image, and the solution is then built by successive iterations, for each i: Embedded Image. In the case of finite stimuli τs, we approximate the sharp transitions of θP with a sigmoid curve with characteristic time Δt = 1 min. We have successfully tested this algorithm on a set of Volterra integral equations of the first kind where an analytical solution was known (52, 53). We validated the described method for our model in Eq. 2. We ran simulations for transient inclinations of duration τs with arbitrarily chosen memory kernels µin. From the simulated dynamics, memory kernels were estimated using the method described here, and were found to be consistent with the original, regardless of the shape of the kernel or the stimulus duration τs (see Fig. S7). Extracting of memory kernel from experimental data: for each coleoptile we extracted µ(t) from the trajectory θtip(t). We discarded instances where the algorithm diverged.We then averaged over repetitions resulting in an average µ. We do not take an explicit account of growth when estimating of µ, and to the first approximation we neglect the possible slow drift of l⊥ due to growth.

Funding

Israel Science Foundation Grant (1981/ 14); European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under Grant Agreement No. 824074 (GrowBot); Human Frontier Science Program, Reference No. RGY0078/2019.

Author contributions

Methodology, M.R.; Formal analysis, M.R.; Investigation, M.R. and Y.M.; Writing – Original draft, M.R. and Y.M.; Writing – Review and editing, M.R. and Y.M.; Funding acquisition, Y.M.; Supervision, Y.M.; Conceptualization, Y.M.

Competing interests

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Data and materials availability

available by emailing Y.M. or M.R.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Figures S1 to S7

References

  1. 1.↵
    J. F. Cahill Jr, et al., Plants integrate information about nutrients and neighbors, Science 328, 1657–1657 (2010).
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. 2.
    E. Dener, A. Kacelnik, H. Shemesh, Pea Plants Show Risk Sensitivity, Current Biology 26, 1763–1767 (2016).
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  3. 3.
    A. Hodge, Root decisions, Plant Cell Environment 32, 628–640 (2009).
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  4. 4.
    A.-L. Paul, C. E. Amalfitano, R. J. Ferl, Plant growth strategies are remodeled by spaceflight, BMC Plant Biology 12, 232 (2012).
    OpenUrl
  5. 5.
    M. Ciszak, E. Masi, F. Baluška, S. Mancuso, Plant shoots exhibit synchronized oscillatory motions, Communicative & Integrative Biology 9 (2016).
  6. 6.
    M. Faget, et al., Root–root interactions: extending our perspective to be more inclusive of the range of theories in ecology and agriculture using in-vivo analyses, Annals of Botany 112, 253–266 (2013).
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. 7.
    M. L. Pereira, V. O. Sadras, W. Batista, J. J. Casal, A. J. Hall, Light-mediated selforganization of sunflower stands increases oil yield in the field, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, 7975–7980 (2017).
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. 8.
    A. M. Paya, J. L. Silverberg, J. Padgett, T. L. Bauerle, X-ray computed tomography uncovers root–root interactions: quantifying spatial relationships between interacting root systems in three dimensions, Frontiers in Plant Science 6 (2015).
  9. 9.
    M. Gruntman, D. Groß, M. Májeková, K. Tielbörger, Decision-making in plants under competition., Nat. Commun. 8, 2235 (2017).
    OpenUrl
  10. 10.
    W. T. McCleery, N. A. Mohd-Radzman, V. A. Grieneisen, Root branching plasticity: collective decision-making results from local and global signalling., Curr. Opin. Cell Biol. 44, 51–58 (2017).
    OpenUrl
  11. 11.
    R. Karban, J. L. Orrock, A judgment and decision-making model for plant behavior., Ecology 99, 1909–1919 (2018).
    OpenUrl
  12. 12.↵
    Y. Meroz, R. Bastien, L. Mahadevan, Spatio-temporal integration in plant tropisms, Journal of the Royal Society Interface 16, 20190038 (2019).
    OpenUrl
  13. 13.↵
    H. Chauvet, B. Moulia, V. Legué, Y. Forterre, O. Pouliquen, Revealing the hierarchy of processes and time-scales that control the tropic response of shoots to gravistimulations, Journal of Experimental Botany 70, 1955–1967 (2019).
    OpenUrl
  14. 14.↵
    B. Moulia, S. Douady, O. Hamant, Fluctuations shape plants through proprioception, Science 372, eabc6868 (2021).
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. 15.
    H. Fromm, Root plasticity in the pursuit of water, Plants 8, 236 (2019).
    OpenUrl
  16. 16.↵
    B. Moulia, E. Badel, R. Bastien, L. Duchemin, C. Eloy, The shaping of plant axes and crowns through tropisms and elasticity: an example of morphogenetic plasticity beyond the shoot apical meristem, New Phytologist 233, 2354–2379 (2022).
    OpenUrl
  17. 17.↵
    R. Bastien, Y. Meroz, The kinematics of plant nutation reveals a simple relation between curvature and the orientation of differential growth, PLoS computational biology 12, e1005238 (2016).
    OpenUrl
  18. 18.↵
    L. van Schijndel, B. L. Snoek, K. ten Tusscher, Embodiment in distributed information processing:”solid” plants versus “liquid” ant colonies, Quantitative Plant Biology 3, e27 (2022).
    OpenUrl
  19. 19.↵
    D. E. Moulton, H. Oliveri, A. Goriely, ultiscale integration of environmental stimuli in plant tropism produces complex behaviors., PNAS 117, 32226–32237 (2020).
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. 20.
    Y. Meroz, Plant tropisms as a window on plant computational processes, New Phytologist 229, 1911–1916 (2021).
    OpenUrl
  21. 21.↵
    S. Duran-Nebreda, G. W. Bassel, Plant behaviour in response to the environment: information processing in the solid state, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 374, 20180370 (2019).
    OpenUrl
  22. 22.↵
    G. W. Bassel, Information processing and distributed computation in plant organs, Trends in plant science 23, 994–1005 (2018).
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  23. 23.↵
    H. Fitting, Untersuchungen à Œber den geotropischen Reizvorgang. Teil I. Die geotropische Empfindlichkeit der Pflanzen. Teil II. Weitere Erfolge mit der intermittierenden Reizung.., Jahrbücher für Wissenschaftliche Botanik 41, 221-330, 331-398 (1905).
    OpenUrl
  24. 24.
    A. Nathansohn, E. Pringseheim, Uber die summation intermittierren der lichtreize., Jahrb Wiss Bot 45, 137–190 (1908).
    OpenUrl
  25. 25.↵
    D. Volkmann, M. Tewinkel, Gravisensitivity of cress roots: investigations of threshold values under specific conditions of sensor physiology in microgravity, Plant, Cell and Environment 19, 1195–1202 (1996).
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. 26.
    B. G. Pickard, Geotropic response patterns of the Avenacoleoptile. I. Dependence on angle and duration of stimulation, Canadian Journal of Botany 51, 1003–1021 (1973).
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  27. 27.
    H. Kataoka, Phototropic Responses of Vaucheria geminata to Intermittent Blue Light Stimuli., Plant Physiology 63, 1107–1110 (1979).
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. 28.
    W. R. Briggs, Light Dosage and Phototropic Responses of Corn and Oat Coleoptiles., Plant Physiology 35, 951–962 (1960).
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  29. 29.↵
    A. Johnsson, C. Karlsson, T.-H. Iversen, D. K. Chapman, Random root movements in weightlessness, Physiologia Plantarum 96, 169–178 (1996).
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. 30.↵
    D. Des Cloizeaux, Theory of condensed matter. Lectures presented at an international course (1968).
  31. 31.↵
    S. M. Block, J. E. Segall, H. C. Berg, Impulse responses in bacterial chemotaxis, Cell 31, 215–226.
  32. 32.
    J. E. Segall, S. M. Block, H. C. Berg, Temporal comparisons in bacterial chemotaxis., Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 83, 8987–8991.
  33. 33.
    E. D. Lipson, White noise analysis of Phycomyces light growth response system. Normal intensity range., Biophysical Journal 15, 989 (1975).
    OpenUrlPubMed
  34. 34.↵
    H. V. Prentice-Mott, et al., Directional memory arises from long-lived cytoskeletal asymmetries in polarized chemotactic cells, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, 1267–1272 (2016).
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. 35.↵
    C. Kaspar, B. Ravoo, W. G. van der Wiel, S. Wegner, W. Pernice, The rise of intelligent matter, Nature 594, 345–355 (2021).
    OpenUrl
  36. 36.↵
    D. Peak, J. D. West, S. M. Messinger, K. A. Mott, Evidence for complex, collective dynamics and emergent, distributed computation in plants, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101, 918–922 (2004).
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. 37.↵
    N. C. Keim, J. D. Paulsen, Z. Zeravcic, S. Sastry, S. R. Nagel, Memory formation in matter, Reviews of Modern Physics 91, 035002 (2019).
    OpenUrl
  38. 38.↵
    K. Bhattacharyya, D. Zwicker, K. Alim, Memory formation in adaptive networks, Physical Review Letters 129, 028101 (2022).
    OpenUrl
  39. 39.↵
    J. Sachs, über orthotrope und plagiotrope pflanzentheile, Arb. Bot. Inst. Wurzburg 2, 226–284 (1882).
    OpenUrl
  40. 40.↵
    B. Moulia, M. Fournier, The power and control of gravitropic movements in plants: a biomechanical and systems biology view, Journal of experimental botany 60, 461–486 (2009).
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  41. 41.↵
    R. Bastien, T. Bohr, B. Moulia, S. Douady, Unifying model of shoot gravitropism reveals proprioception as a central feature of posture control in plants, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110, 755–760 (2013).
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. 42.↵
    N. Levernier, O. Pouliquen, Y. Forterre, An integrative model of plant gravitropism linking statoliths position and auxin transport, Frontiers in Plant Science 12, 474 (2021).
    OpenUrl
  43. 43.↵
    M. Nakamura, T. Nishimura, M. T. Morita, Gravity sensing and signal conversion in plant gravitropism, Journal of experimental botany 70, 3495–3506 (2019).
    OpenUrlPubMed
  44. 44.↵
    H. Chauvet, O. Pouliquen, Y. Forterre, V. Legué, B. Moulia, Inclination not force is sensed by plants during shoot gravitropism, Scientific reports 6, 1–8 (2016).
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  45. 45.↵
    A. M. Hein, F. Carrara, D. R. Brumley, R. Stocker, S. A. Levin, Natural search algorithms as a bridge between organisms, evolution, and ecology, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, 9413–9420 (2016).
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. 46.↵
    M. E. Nelson, M. A. MacIver, Sensory acquisition in active sensing systems, Journal of Comparative Physiology A 192, 573–586 (2006).
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  47. 47.↵
    J. E. Segall, S. M. Block, H. C. Berg, Temporal comparisons in bacterial chemotaxis., Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 83, 8987–8991 (1986).
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. 48.↵
    H. C. Berg, E. coli in Motion (Springer, 2004).
  49. 49.↵
    F. P. Hartmann, et al., Plant Gravitropism (Springer, 2022), pp. 117–131.
  50. 50.↵
    R. Bastien, S. Douady, B. Moulia, A unifying modeling of plant shoot gravitropism with an explicit account of the effects of growth, Frontiers in plant science 5, 136 (2014).
    OpenUrl
  51. 51.↵
    W. H. Press, S. A. Teukolsky, W. T. Vetterling, B. P. Flannery, Numerical recipes 3rd edition: The art of scientific computing (Cambridge university press, 2007).
  52. 52.↵
    P. Linz, Numerical methods for volterra integral equations of the first kind, The computer journal 12, 393–397 (1969).
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  53. 53.↵
    E. Babolian, Z. Masouri, Direct method to solve volterra integral equation of the first kind using operational matrix with block-pulse functions, Journal of Computational and Applied Mathematics 220, 51–57 (2008).
    OpenUrlCrossRef
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted January 06, 2023.
Download PDF

Supplementary Material

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Revealing how memory shapes time-dependent computational processes in plant tropisms
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Revealing how memory shapes time-dependent computational processes in plant tropisms
Mathieu Rivière, Yasmine Meroz
bioRxiv 2023.01.06.522981; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.01.06.522981
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo LinkedIn logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Revealing how memory shapes time-dependent computational processes in plant tropisms
Mathieu Rivière, Yasmine Meroz
bioRxiv 2023.01.06.522981; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.01.06.522981

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Plant Biology
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (4229)
  • Biochemistry (9109)
  • Bioengineering (6753)
  • Bioinformatics (23944)
  • Biophysics (12102)
  • Cancer Biology (9498)
  • Cell Biology (13742)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (7617)
  • Ecology (11664)
  • Epidemiology (2066)
  • Evolutionary Biology (15479)
  • Genetics (10620)
  • Genomics (14297)
  • Immunology (9467)
  • Microbiology (22795)
  • Molecular Biology (9078)
  • Neuroscience (48894)
  • Paleontology (355)
  • Pathology (1479)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (2565)
  • Physiology (3824)
  • Plant Biology (8309)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (1467)
  • Synthetic Biology (2290)
  • Systems Biology (6172)
  • Zoology (1297)