Abstract
Social behaviors are innate and supported by dedicated neural circuits, but it remains unclear whether these circuits are developmentally hardwired or established through social experience. Here, we revealed distinct response patterns and functions in social behavior of medial amygdala (MeA) cells originating from two embryonically parcellated developmental lineages. MeA cells in male mice that express the transcription factor Foxp2 (MeAFoxp2) are specialized for processing male conspecific cues even before puberty and are essential for adult inter-male aggression. In contrast, MeA cells derived from the Dbx1-lineage (MeADbx1) respond broadly to social cues and are non-essential for male aggression. Furthermore, MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells show differential anatomical and functional connectivity. Altogether, our results support a developmentally hardwired aggression circuit at the level of the MeA and we propose a lineage-based circuit organization by which a cell’s embryonic transcription factor profile determines its social information representation and behavior relevance during adulthood.
Highlights
MeAFoxp2 cells in male mice show highly specific responses to male conspecific cues and during attack while MeADbx1 cells are broadly tuned to social cues.
The male-specific response of MeAFoxp2 cells is present in naïve adult males and adult social experience refines the response by increasing its trial-to-trial reliability and temporal precision.
MeAFoxp2 cells show biased response to males even before puberty.
Activation of MeAFoxp2, but not MeADbx1, cells promote inter-male aggression in naïve male mice.
Inactivation of MeAFoxp2, but not MeADbx1, cells suppresses inter-male aggression.
MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells show differential connectivity at both the input and output levels.
Introduction
Innate social behaviors, such as mating, fighting and parenting, are indispensable for the survival and propagation of a species, and therefore present widely in the animal kingdom. These behaviors are considered innate as they can take place without learning although the efficiency in performing these behaviors can be improved with repeated execution1. The developmental mechanisms for the establishment of innate social behaviors and the role of experience in shaping these circuits remain poorly understood.
An array of interconnected brain regions, collectively referred to as the social behavior network (SBN), were proposed to be important for diverse social behaviors2,3. The medial amygdala (MeA), especially its posterior division (pMeA), is considered a key node of the SBN based on its connectivity, activity, gonadal hormone receptor expression and numerous lesion studies2. At the input level, MeA is the primary recipient of accessory olfactory bulb (AOB) inputs-the exclusive relay of the vomeronasal organ (VNO) specialized in detecting pheromones4. Volatile information from the main olfactory system also converges onto MeA cells via the cortical amygdala5,6. Consistent with the strong olfactory inputs, c-Fos, a surrogate of neural activation, is highly expressed in the MeA following exposure to conspecific chemosensory cues7-9. In vivo electrophysiological recording and Ca2+ imaging further revealed response of MeA cells to a wide array of conspecific and heterospecific cues, including males, females, pups, and predator odors6,10. Unsurprisingly, MeA lesion, which impedes the flow of social sensory information, causes deficits in multiple social behaviors, including male sexual behavior, aggression and maternal behaviors11-15. These studies collectively support an important role for the MeA in processing and relaying olfactory information related to conspecifics.
Recent functional experiments suggest a more direct role of pMeA in driving social behaviors. Hong et. al. first showed that optogenetic activation of GABAergic pMeA cells (the major cell type in the dorsal pMeA) acutely induced mounting or attack in male mice depending on stimulation intensity16. Later, Unger et. al. reported that silencing or ablating aromatase expressing MeA cells decreased aggression in both males and females17. Padilla et. al. found that optogenetic activation of the projection from MeA Npy1r expressing cells to bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) promoted male aggression18 and Miller et. al. reported similar aggression-promoting effect of the MeAD1R to BNST pathway19. Nordman et. al. showed that high frequency stimulation of MeA CaMKII cells could prime aggression through its projection to BNST and ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH)20. Most recently, MeA GABAergic cells were also found to drive social behaviors besides aggression, including pup grooming, infanticide and allogrooming21,22.
These results raised several questions regarding the MeA function in social behaviors. First, does the MeA mainly encode olfactory cues or also carry action-related information? While MeA cells have been consistently found to be activated by conspecific olfactory cues6,7,10, the responses of MeA cells during the action phase of adult-directed social behaviors, such as attack and mount, remains unexplored. Second, are there dedicated subpopulations in the MeA for distinct social behaviors or can any random subsets of MeA cells generate any social behavior in a context and intensity dependent manner? An answer to this question remains unclear as activating multiple subpopulations of MeA cells can all induce aggression17-20, while activating the same GABAergic MeA population induces diverse social behaviors16,21,22. Third, how much of the MeA cell response is developmentally hardwired vs. determined by adult experience? Through immediate-early gene mapping, Choi et. al. found that MeA cells relevant for social behaviors and predator defense are marked by distinct transcription factors, suggesting developmental hardwiring of social vs. non-social signals7. However, recent imaging studies revealed that MeA cell responses to social stimuli can be altered with adult experience, suggesting that the exact social response of MeA cells may not be pre-determined23.
Taken together, although the MeA is clearly a central node of SBN, how the MeA mediates social behaviors remains elusive. In our previous studies, we identified two distinct MeA populations that arise from separate embryonic lineages in the telencephalic preoptic area (POA), marked by the transcription factors, Dbx1 and Foxp29,24. In adults, although Dbx1 is no longer expressed in the MeA, Dbx1-lineage cells remain distinct from Foxp2 expressing cells despite being spatially intermingled9 (Figure 1). In addition, these two subpopulations differ in their gene expression patterns and intrinsic electrophysiological properties9. Therefore, we reason that these two developmentally distinct and transcriptionally-defined subpopulations could provide a unique opportunity to address whether MeA cells are hardwired for social behaviors or not. Specifically, are social cue representation and social function of MeA cells predetermined by their developmental lineage? Here, we performed a series of in vivo population recordings, functional manipulations and tracing experiments to compare the neuronal responses, functions, and connectivity of MeADbx1 and MeAFoxp2 cells in male social behaviors and revealed the response pattern of MeAFoxp2 cells over development.
(a) Immunostaining of Foxp2 and GFP (Dbx1-derived cells) in the MeA of Dbx1cre;Ai6 male mice. Left bottom shows the enlarged view of boxed areas.
(b) Percentage of MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells in the total MeA population.
(c) The number of counted Foxp2, Dbx1-derived and double positive cells in each side of the MeA from Bregma −1.4mm to −2.1mm.
(d) The total number of counted Foxp2, Dbx1-derived and double positive cells in each side of the posterodorsal and posteroventral MeA (MeApd and MeApv).
(e) The total number of counted Foxp2, Dbx1-derived and double positive cells in the MeApd sub-compartments from Bregma −1.6mm to −2.1mm.
(f) Total number of Foxp2, Dbx1-derived and overlap cells in each side of the posterior MeA.
For b-f, every third of 50µm brain sections were counted. The Allen Brain Reference Atlas was used to determine the MeA subdivisions and sub-compartments. (b) Two-tailed unpaired t-test. (e) One-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s multiple comparisons test. Data are mean ± S.E.M., *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, ****p<0.0001, n=3 mice.
Results
Distribution of MeADbx1 and MeAFoxp2 cells in male mice
To visualize the spatial distribution of MeADbx1 and MeAFoxp2 cells in adults, we crossed Dbx1cre mice25 with a ZsGreen reporter line (Ai6)26 and immunostained for Foxp2. MeADbx1 cells make up approximately 28% of total posterior MeA cells (MeAp, Bregma level −1.4 to −2.1mm) and are found in both dorsal and ventral subdivisions (MeApd and MeApv) (Fig. 1a-c). In comparison, MeAFoxp2 cells are relatively fewer, constituting only 10% of pMeA cells, and largely absent from caudal MeA (Fig. 1a-c). Between MeApd and MeApv, both MeADbx1 and MeAFoxp2 cells show a dorsal bias: with approximately twice as many cells in MEApd than MeApv (Fig.1d). Within the MeApd, Foxp2 cells are most prominent in the lateral compartment while Dbx1-derived cells are biased towards the medial compartment (Fig. 1e). Importantly, consistent with our previous study, MeADbx1 and MeAFoxp2 are largely distinct, even when they occupy the same MeA region (Fig. 1c-1f). Of all MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells, only 1.8% are double positive.
Distinct MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cell responses to social sensory cues in head-fixed naïve male mice
To address whether MeAFoxp2and MeADbx1 are hardwired to respond to different social cues, we recorded the Ca2+ activity of each population in head-fixed naive adult male mice while presenting various social stimuli in a pseudo-random order (Fig. 2a). Naïve mice are animals without any social interaction except with their littermates. To record MeAFoxp2 cells, we injected a Cre-dependent GCaMP6f virus into the MeA of Foxp2cre+/- male mice27 (Foxp2GCamP). To record MeADbx1 cells, we generated Dbx1cre+/-;LSL-FlpO+/- mice. In these animals, the transient Cre expression during development leads to permanent Flp expression allowing targeting of Dbx1-derived cells in adult mice28. We injected either a Flp-dependent GCaMP6f or a Flp-dependent Cre virus together with a Cre-dependent GCaMP6f virus, into the MeA of Dbx1cre;LSL-FlpO male mice (Dbx1GCamP) (Fig. 2b). A 400-µm optic fiber was placed above the injection site to collect fluorescence signal (Fig. 2b). Histological analysis revealed that 88% of GCaMP6f cells express Foxp2 in Foxp2GCamP mice while only 5% of GCaMP6f cells were co-labeled with Foxp2 in Dbx1GCamP mice, confirming the specificity of the recorded populations (Fig. 2c, d).
(a) Schematics showing the timeline of stimulus presentation.
(b) Schematics of viral injection strategy for targeting MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells.
(c) Representative histology images of viral injection, denoting GCaMP6f expression (green), Foxp2 antibody (red) and DAPI (blue) staining in Foxp2cre and Dbx1cre;LSL-FlpO mice. White dotted lines represent location of fiber implant.
(d) Percentage of cells co-expressing Foxp2 and GCaMP6f over the total number of GCaMP6f cells in the MeA of Foxp2cre and Dbx1cre;LSL-FlpO mice.
(e1-e4) Top: Representative Ca2+ traces of MeAFoxp2 cells during the presentation of a male (e1), female (e2), pup (e3) and object (e4) stimuli. Colored shades represent the duration of stimulus presentation. Bottom: corresponding heat-maps of the z-scored Ca2+ responses (Fz score) per animal before and after the onset of each stimuli in MeAFoxp2 cells.
(f1-f4) Responses of MeADbx1 cells to various stimuli in head-fixed naïve male mice.
(g and h) Average peri-stimulus histograms (PSTH) of Ca2+ signals from MeAFoxp2 (g) and MeADbx1 cells (h) aligned to the onset (left) and offset (right) of various stimulus presentations. Open circles indicate significantly increased responses (q<0.05) from the baseline (Fz=0). Colored lines and shades represent mean responses ± S.E.M. across animals. Dashed lines mark time 0.
(i and j) Peak Fz signal of MeAFoxp2 (i) and MeADbx1 cells (j) during the presentation of social and non-social stimuli.
(k) Preference index (PI) of MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells to different social stimuli. For example, PImale is calculated as (Fzmale − 0.5 × (Fzfemale + Fzpup))/(Fzmale + 0.5×|Fzfemale + Fzpup|).
(d) Two-tailed unpaired t-test. (g-h) One sample t-test for each stimulus, corrected for multiple comparisons with a false discovery rate (FDR) 0.05. (i-j) One-way repeated-measures ANOVA followed by Tukey’s multiple comparisons test. (k) Two-way repeated measures ANOVA followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons test. n = number of animals. Data are mean ± S.E.M.; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, ****p<0.0001.
We found that MeAFoxp2 cells in naïve male mice showed robust GCamp6 increases only during presentation of an adult male but not any other social stimuli (Fig. 2e, g, i, k). In contrast, MeADbx1 cells responded to all social stimuli with the highest activity increase during presentation of an adult female (Fig. 2f, h, j-k). Neither MeAFoxp2 nor MeADbx1 cells responded to a novel object, suggesting their social specific tuning (Fig. 2e-f, i-j). In addition to differential response selectivity, MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells also differed in their response dynamics. While MeAFoxp2cells responded slightly after the stimulus onset, defined as when the stimulus animal reached its minimum distance to the nose of the recording mouse, MeADbx1 cells significantly increased activity right at the onset of stimulus presentation (Fig. 2g, h). Furthermore, MeAFoxp2 cells returned to the baseline activity slowly (>10s) after removal of the male stimulus, while the MeADbx1 cell activity returned to the baseline quickly (< 3s) (Figure 2g, h). Overall, MeAFoxp2 cells showed male-specific and slow responses while MeADbx1 cells showed broad and fast responses to social cues (Fig. 2g-k). These results strongly support distinct response patterns of MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells to social stimuli independent of fighting or mating experience, with MeAFoxp2 cells displaying a select tuning to male cues.
Distinct responses of MeADbx1 and MeAFoxp2 cells during social behaviors in freely moving male mice
Next, we examined responses of male MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells during social behaviors in freely moving male mice to address whether the cells increase activity only to sensory cues, e.g. during investigation, or also respond during the action phase of the behavior, e.g. attack and mount (Extended Data Fig. 1a). Prior to recording, all test animals went through repeated interactions with an adult male and a receptive female until they showed consistent aggression and sexual behaviors. During recording, an adult male intruder, a female, a pup and a novel object were introduced into the home cage of the recording mice, one at a time, with 5 minutes in between (Extended Data Fig. 1b). MeAFoxp2 cells showed significantly higher activity increase upon introduction of a male than any other social and non-social stimuli (Fig. 3a-c, g, k). During subsequent male investigation and attack, MeAFoxp2 cells also showed a significant activity increase (Fig. 3h). To address whether the attack response could largely be due to the continuous conspecific sensory input while attacking, instead of the attack per se, we separated investigation trials based on whether they were followed by attack or not. We found that activity increases during investigation-followed-by-attack trials was significantly higher than that during investigation-only trials at both investigation onset and offset (Extended Data Fig. 1c). This result suggests that MeAFoxp2 respond during both sensory and action phases of aggression and the attack response is not simply due to temporally-linked sensory inputs. In contrast to the strong activity increase during male interaction, MeAFoxp2 cells showed either no change or slightly suppressed activity during female investigation and all phases of sexual behaviors (Fig. 3b, h). Similarly, no activity change was observed during pup interaction, supporting a highly male specific response of MeAFoxp2 cells (Fig. 3c, h l).
(a-f) Representative Ca2+ traces and peri-event histograms (PETHs) of MeAFoxp2 (a-c) and MeADbx1 cells (d-f) during interactions with male, female and pup stimuli. Dashed black lines in PETHs represent the behavior onset at time zero; blue lines in Ca2+ traces indicate time 0 when the intruder is introduced.
(g and i) Introduction responses of MeAFoxp2 (g) and MeADbx1 cells (i), calculated as the peak Ca2+ signal within the first 100 sec after stimulus introduction.
(h and j) Average Ca2+ responses of MeAFoxp2 (h) and MeADbx1 cells (j) during various behaviors towards various conspecific intruders and a novel object.
(k) Preference indexes of MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells showing the relative introduction response magnitudes across different social stimuli.
(l) Preference indexes of MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells denoting the relative investigation response magnitudes across different social stimuli.
(g) One-way repeated-measures ANOVA followed by Tukey’s multiple comparisons test. (h, j) Mixed-effects analysis followed by Tukey’s multiple comparisons test. One sample t-test for each behavior, corrected for multiple comparisons with a false discovery rate (FDR) 0.05. (i) Friedman test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons test. (k-l) Mixed-effects analysis followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons test. n= 7 mice during male and female presentation for MeAFoxp2 group; n= 6 mice during pup investigation and n= 4 mice attacking pup for MeAFoxp2 group; n= 9 mice during male and female presentation for MeADbx1 group; n= 9 mice during pup investigation and n=7 attacking pup for MeADbx1 group. Data are mean ± S.E.M.; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, ****p<0.0001; # q<0.05.
In contrast to the response pattern of MeAFoxp2 cells, MeADbx1 cells in experienced male mice showed activity increases in response to all social stimuli (Fig. 3d-f). Upon initial intruder introduction, MeADbx1cells increased activity to all intruders with a slightly lower response to male intruder than females and pups (Fig. 3i, k). During investigation of a female, male and pup, MeADbx1cell activity increased to a similar extent (Fig. 3j, l). Although MeADbx1 cells also showed significant activity increase during inter-male attack, we did not find a difference in response between investigation-followed-by-attack trials and investigation-only trials, suggesting that MeADbx1 cell response during attack could be largely due to activity increases induced by sensory cues (Fig. 3j, Extended Data Fig. 1d). During copulation, the activity of MeADbx1 cells did not increase during mounting –a series of fast movements to establish an on-top position, but slightly increased during intromission (Fig. 3e, j). During ejaculation, MeADbx1 cells increased activity robustly, higher than the responses during any other behaviors (Fig. 3e, j). No activity increase of MeADbx1 cells was observed when males attacked pups (Fig. 3f, j). Consistent with the response in head-fixed animals, neither MeAFoxp2 nor MeADbx1 responded during object investigation (Extended Data Fig. 1e, f), supporting the social-specific response patterns of the cells.
Overall, male MeAFoxp2 cells show highly specific responses during both the investigatory and action phases of behaviors towards a conspecific male whereas MeADbx1 cells appear to respond mainly to olfactory and possibly penile sensory inputs.
Refinement of male MeAFoxp2 cell responses with adult social experience
In a subset of Foxp2GCamP animals, we also performed Ca2+ recording during freely moving social interactions after head-fixed recording and before repeated social experience. Only one naïve male showed brief attack towards a male intruder and others only investigated the intruders. Consistent with recordings in head-fixed naïve animals, MeAFoxp2 cells responded specifically during male investigation (Fig. 4a-f). However, when we compared the response patterns of MeAFoxp2 cells in naïve vs. experienced animals, we noticed a clear difference. In comparison to naïve animals, activity of MeAFoxp2 cells in experienced animals increased faster and with higher reliability (Fig. 4g-i). In naïve animals, MeAFoxp2 cells responded (Zincrease > 1 during investigation) in approximately 40% of trials while this number increased to 60% in experienced animals (Fig. 4j). Among the responsive trials, the average latency to respond in experienced animals is approximately half of that in naïve animals (Fig. 4k). Overall, the mean activity increase during male investigation is significantly higher in experienced animals than in naïve animals although the male preference index (PI) did not differ between these two groups (Fig. 4l-m). The difference in response is not due to changes in investigatory behaviors: the average duration of investigation episodes was similar in naïve vs. experienced animals (Fig. 4n). It is worth noting that the difference in response patterns between naïve and experienced animals does not depend on the expression of aggression. Two experienced males did not attack the intruder during the recording (green circles in Fig. 4j-n) and their MeAFoxp2 cell responses were comparable to those in aggressive experienced males (Fig. 4j-n). These results suggest that although adult social experience is not required for the male specific responses of MeAFoxp2 cells, it refines the response by improving its consistency and temporal precision.
(a-d) Representative Ca2+ traces of MeAFoxp2 cells during the presentation of a male (a), female (b), pup (c) and object (d) in naïve male mice.
(e) Average PETHs of MeAFoxp2 cell responses aligned to investigation onset in naïve male mice. The dashed black line represents the behavior onset at time zero.
(f) Average Fz score of MeAFoxp2 cells during investigation of different stimuli in naïve male mice.
(g and h) Representative heat-maps showing trial-by-trial Ca2+ signal (Fz-Fz at time 0) of MeAFoxp2 cells during investigation of a male intruder in naïve (g) and socially experienced
(h) in a male mouse. Black short lines denote the time points when Fz >=1. Black dots denote the offsets of investigation.
(i) Average PETHs of MeAFoxp2 cell responses aligned to investigation onset in naïve (purple) and socially experienced (pink) male mice. The dashed black line represents the behavior onset at time zero.
(j) Percent of trials in which MeAFoxp2 cells show Fz>1 during male investigation in naïve and experienced male mice.
(k) Latency of MeAFoxp2 cells to respond (Fz>1) in responsive trials.
(l) Average Fz score of MeAFoxp2 cells during male investigation in naïve and experienced male mice.
(m) Male preference index of MeAFoxp2 cell responses during investigation in naïve and experienced male mice.
(n) Average male investigation duration in naïve and experienced male mice.
Green circles in (j-n) represent male mice with repeated social experience but did not attack during the recording session. (f) One-way repeated-measures ANOVA followed by Tukey’s multiple comparisons test. (j-n) Two-tailed unpaired t-test. Parenthesis and n= number of mice. Data are mean ± S.E.M.; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.
The male-specific response of male MeAFoxp2 cells exists before puberty
To further address whether the highly male specific MeAFoxp2 cells is developmentally hardwired or established through adult experience, we recorded the responses of MeAFoxp2 cells to social stimuli during early life. Puberty (P30-P38) is a critical development period when aggression starts to emerge29-31, thus we focused on MeAFoxp2cell responses before (P25), at the onset of (P30-32) and after puberty (P40-44). To achieve this goal, we injected Cre-dependent GCaMP6f virus into the MeA of P11 Foxp2cre mice and placed a 400-µm fiber just dorsal to the MeA at P24 (Fig. 5a-b). After a 24hr recovery window, we recorded the Ca2+ activity of MeAFoxp2 cells when the animals were exposed to an anaesthetized adult male or female mouse or a pup (Fig. 5c). To minimize the impact of social experience, all animals were singly housed post-weaning at P21. We found that in P25 juvenile male mice MeAFoxp2 cells already showed higher activity during interaction with an adult male than other social stimuli (Fig. 5d). At P30-32, a similar male-biased response was observed (Fig. 5e). At P40-44, the difference between male and female responses further increased and this trend continued at >P56 (Fig. 5f). Notably, the divergence of MeAFoxp2 cell responses to male and non-male cues over age appears to be mainly driven by a decrease in responses to females and pups (Fig. 5h). Behaviorally, juvenile (P25 and P30-32) and young adult (P40-44) males tended to investigate adult females more than adult males (Fig. 5i). However, this behavior difference did not explain the differential MeAFoxp2 responses to males and females as no correlation between response magnitude and time spent on investigation was found (Fig. 5j). Finally, the male preference index (PI) across all 4 time-points did not differ, supporting that the male-specific cell responses exist before puberty (Fig. 5k). Altogether, these results suggest that MeAFoxp2 cells are predisposed to preferentially respond to male-related sensory information even before puberty. We suggest that discriminability between male and non-male cues further improves after puberty by reducing responses to non-male cues.
(a) Schematics of virus injection and a representative histology image indicating GCaMP6f expression (green), Foxp2 antibody (red) and DAPI (blue) staining in Foxp2cre male mice. White dotted lines mark the fiber ending.
(b) Timeline of virus injection, fiber placement and recordings.
(c) Timeline of behavioral test during the recording day. Stimuli were presented in a pseudo-random fashion.
(d-g) Representative Fz scored Ca2+ traces of MeAFoxp2 cells during interactions with an anesthetized (d1-f1) or freely-moving male (g1), an anesthetized (d2-f2) or freely-moving female (g2) or a pup (d3-g3) in a male mouse at different ages. Average Fz score during social investigation (d4-g4) of animals at different ages.
(h) Average Fz score of MeAFoxp2 cell responses during male (purple), female (red) and pup (blue) investigation in mice at different ages.
(i) Percent of time the test male spent investigating a male and female intruder.
(j) No correlation between the Fz score during male or female investigation and the percent of time spent investigating in all recording sessions across ages.
(k) Male investigation PI at different ages.
(d4-g4) One-way repeated-measures ANOVA followed by Tukey’s multiple comparisons test. (h-i) Two-way repeated measures ANOVA followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons test. (j) Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient (k) Kruskal-Wallis test. n=5 (P25), 6 (P30-32), 6 (P40-44) and 7 mice (>P56). Data are mean ± S.E.M.; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, ****p<0.0001.
Differential inputs to MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells
Given the differential responses of MeAFoxp2and MeADbx1 cells to social cues, we next asked whether these the two populations receive different direct inputs. To test this, we used monosynaptic rabies virus tracing. We injected Cre-dependent or Flp-dependent AAVs expressing TVA-mCherry and rabies G protein into the MeA of Foxp2creor Dbx1cre;LSLFlpO male mice, and four weeks later EnvA-ΔG rabies virus expressing GFP (Fig. 6a-d). We found that the major inputs to MeAFoxp2 arise from other amygdala nuclei including posterior amygdala (PA), central amygdala (CeA) and BNST (Fig. 6e-g). In contrast, MeADbx1 cells receive inputs mainly from primary olfactory relays, including AOB, cortical amygdala (COA) and piriform cortex (Pir) (Figure 6e, h, i). Hypothalamus, mainly medial preoptic area (MPOA) and zona incerta (ZI), provided moderate inputs to both MeAFoxp2and MeADbx1 cells (Fig. 6e-i). Sparsely retrogradely labeled cells from both MeAFoxp2and MeADbx1 cells were also observed in hippocampus, striatum and pallidum (Fig. 6e-i).
(a) Schematics showing timeline of monosynaptic retrograde rabies tracing of MeAFoxp2 cells. Pie chart showing the distribution of starter cells (mCherry+ eGFP+).
(b) Representative image showing the location of starter MeAFoxp2 cells, denoting TVA-mCherry (red), Rabies-eGFP (green) and DAPI (blue) staining. Inset showing an enlarged view of boxed area. Scale bars: 1mm and 100µm (inset).
(c) Schematics showing the retrograde monosynaptic tracing from MeADbx1 cells and the starter cell distribution.
(d) Representative histology of the location of starter MeADbx1 cells in a Dbx1cre;LSL-FlpO mouse. Red: TVA-mCherry. Green: Rabies-eGFP, Blue: DAPI staining. Scale bars: 1mm and 100µm (inset).
(e) Distribution of cells in various brain regions that are retrogradely labelled from MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells.
(f and h) Representative histological images showing cells in various regions that are retrogradely labelled from MeAFoxp2 (f) or MeADbx1 (h) cells.
(g and i) Overview of inputs into MeAFoxp2 (g) and MeADbx1 (i) cells.
(j and l) Recording strategy to examine functional inputs from AOB to MeAFoxp2 (k) and MeADbx1 (l) cells.
(k and m) Representative images showing ChrimsonR (red) expression in the olfactory bulb (OB) and ChrimsonR fibers in the MeA. Green: GFP expressed in Foxp2 (k) and Dbx1 (m) cells. Blue: DAPI staining.
(n and t) Pie charts showing the distribution of synaptic responses of MeAFoxp2 (n) and MeADbx1 (t) cells to optogenetic activation of OB terminals.
(o and u) Representative traces showing optogenetically (1 ms, 605 nm) evoked IPSCs (oIPCSs) and EPSCs (oEPSCs) before and after bath application of TTX and TTX + 4-AP.
(p-s) Characterization of oIPSCs and oEPSCs in MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells, including amplitude (p, r) and latency (q, s).
(v-w) oIPSCs in both MeAFoxp2 (v) and MeADbx1 (w) cells were abolished by bath application of TTX and failed to recover after applying TTX+4-AP.
(x) oEPSCs in MeADbx1 cells were abolished by TTX but recovered after TTX+4-AP application.
SI: substantia innominate. NDB: diagonal band nucleus. DG: dentate gyrus. Sub: subiculum. CA3: field CA3. CP: caudoputamen. GPe: globus pallidus, external segment. SNr: substantia nigra, reticular part. Thal: thalamus. (e) Two-way ANOVA followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons test; n=4 mice in each group. (r and s) Mann Whitney test. (v-x) Friedman test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons test. (n-s) n=23 cells from 3 male mice for MeAFoxp2 group; n=33 cells from 3 male mice for MeADbx1group; (v) n=7 cells from 3 male mice; (w, x) n=6 cells from 3 male mice. Data are mean ± S.E.M.; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, ****p<0.0001.
The lack of retrogradely labeled cells in AOB from MeAFoxp2 starter cells was particularly surprising given that the MeA is the primary target of the AOB, where the output neurons are excitatory (Fig. 6e-g)4,32,33. To further understand the inputs from the AOB to MeAFoxp2 cells, we performed optogenetic assisted circuit mapping from AOB to MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells. We expressed ChrimsonR-tdTomato in the olfactory bulb, virally labeled MeAFoxp2 cells with GFP (Fig. 6j, k) and visualized MeADbx1 cells using Dbx1cre;Ai6 mice (Fig. 6l, m). 4-weeks after injection, we prepared brain slices containing the MeA and recorded the responses of GFP+ MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells to 605 nm 1-ms light pulses. Among a total of 23 MeAFoxp2 cells, we observed light evoked excitatory postsynaptic currents (oEPSCs) in only 2 cells while majority (18/23) of recorded cells showed light evoked inhibitory post-synaptic currents (oIPSCs) (Fig. 6n, o). In contrast, 18/33 MeADbx1 cells showed oEPSCs and the vast majority (29/33) showed oIPSCs (Fig. 6t, u). The oIPSCs of MeADbx1 and MeAFoxp2 cells were similar in magnitude and both were of long latencies (>10ms) (Fig. 6p-s). Bath application of TTX or TTX+4-AP completely abolished oIPSCS in both populations, suggesting that both MeAFoxp2and MeADbx1 cells receive polysynaptic inhibitory inputs (Fig. 6o, u, w). oEPSCs of MeADbx1cells are of shorter latency (∼4 ms) than oIPSCs (Fig. 6s) and bath application of TTX+4-AP did not abolish oEPSCs, supporting that AOB cells provide monosynaptic excitatory inputs to MeADbx1 cells (Fig. 6u, x).
These results confirmed that AOB targets MEAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells differently, consistent with the idea that the distinct in vivo responses of these two populations are hardwired. The fact that MeAFoxp2 cells receive minimum direct inputs from the AOB and other primary olfactory relays suggests that sensory information reaching MeAFoxp2 cells could be more processed, which may explain the higher response selectivity of MeAFoxp2 cells than MeADbx1 cells.
MEAFoxp2 cells are sufficient to promote inter-male aggression in naïve mice
To understand the functional importance of MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells in social behaviors, we bilaterally injected Cre- and Flp-dependent hM3Dq viruses into the MeA of Foxp2cre and Dbx1cre;LSL-FlpO naïve male mice respectively (Foxp2hM3Dq and Dbx1hM3Dq) (Fig. 7a, b). Control animals were injected with mCherry virus in the MeA (Foxp2mCherry and Dbx1mCherry). Three weeks later, we intraperitoneally (i.p.) injected saline and clozapine-N-oxide (CNO) on two separate days and 30 mins later introduced a pup, an adult male and a female intruder into the cage sequentially, each for 5 to 10 minutes, with 5 minutes in between (Fig. 7c). While only 4/10 Foxp2hM3Dq male mice attacked a male intruder after saline injection, all Foxp2hM3Dq males attacked the intruder repeatedly after CNO injection (Fig. 7e). In comparison, only 4/8 Foxp2mCherry initiated attack after CNO injection (Fig. 7e). The total attack duration of Foxp2hM3Dq males significantly increased after CNO injection (Fig. 7f) although the latency to attack did not decrease in animals that attacked on both days (Extended Data Fig. 2a). Possibly due to increased aggression, Foxp2hM3Dq mice spent less time investigating the male intruder after CNO injection (Fig. 7g). No changes in locomotion were observed in Foxp2hM3Dq males after CNO injection suggesting that increases in attack was not due to an increase in general arousal (Extended Data Fig. 2b). Additionally, the increased aggression is adult male-specific as we did not observe an increase in infanticidal behavior after activating MEAFoxp2 cells (Extended Data Fig. 2c). The overall pup interaction was also unchanged (Extended Data Fig. 2d). Similarly, male sexual behaviors, including female investigation, mounting and intromission, were not affected by MEAFoxp2 activation (Extended Data Fig. 2e-k). Control Foxp2mCherry animals showed no significant change in any social behavior after CNO injection in comparison to saline injection (Fig. 7d-g, Extended Data Fig. 2a-k).
(a) Strategies for chemogenetic activation of MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells.
(b) Representative histological images of hM3Dq (red) expression in the MeA of Foxp2cre and Dbx1cre;LSL-FlpO mice. Blue: DAPI.
(c) Experimental timeline of chemogenetic activation experiments.
(d) Representative raster plots showing behaviors towards male intruders of 5 Foxp2hM3Dq and 5 Foxp2mCherry male mice after i.p. injection of saline or CNO.
(e) Percentage of Foxp2hM3Dq and Foxp2mCherry male mice that attacked a male intruder after saline or CNO injection.
(f-g) Percent of time Foxp2hM3Dq and Foxp2mCherry mice spent attacking (f) and investigating (g) a male intruder.
(h-k) Follow conventions in d-g. CNO injection into Dbx1hM3Dq mice caused a reduction in social investigation but did not change aggressive behaviors towards a male intruder.
(l) Strategies for chemogenetic inactivation of MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells.
(m) Representative histological images showing hM4Di (red) expression in the MeA of Foxp2cre and Dbx1cre;LSL-FlpO mice. Blue: DAPI.
(n) Experimental timeline of chemogenetic inactivation experiments.
(o) Representative raster plots showing the behaviors of 5 Foxp2hM4Di and 5 Foxp2mCherry mice after i.p. injection of saline or CNO in the presence of a male intruder.
(p-r) Percent of time Foxp2hM4Di and Foxp2mCherry male mice spent investigating (p) and attacking (q) a male intruder, and the latency to first attack (r).
(s-v) Follows the conventions in o-r. CNO injection into Dbx1hM4Di or Dbx1mCherry mice did not change any male-directed behaviors in comparison to those after saline injection.
(e, i) McNemar’s test. (f, g, j, k, p-r, t-v) Two-way repeated measures ANOVA followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons test. n = number of animals. Data are mean ± S.E.M.; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, ****p<0.0001.
We found that Dbx1cre;LSL-FlpO male mice tend to be more aggressive than Foxp2cre male mice possibly due to their slight difference in genetic background27,28. Specifically, majority of Dbx1hM3Dq and Dbx1mCherry animals attacked the intruder during the first encounter (after saline injection) and nearly all animals attacked the intruder during the second encounter (after CNO injection) (Fig. 7h, i). Importantly, there is no difference between Dbx1hM3Dq and Dbx1mCherry groups in the percentage of animals that attacked (Fig. 7i). The latency to attack and attack duration also did not differ on CNO- and saline-injected days in both Dbx1hM3Dq and Dbx1mCherry groups (Fig. 7j, Extended Data Fig. 3a) although Dbx1hM3Dq male mice investigated the male intruder less after CNO injection (Fig. 7k). Activating MeADbx1 cells didn’t change the probability of infanticide, male sexual behaviors or locomotion significantly (Extended Data Fig. 3b-k). Thus, MEAFoxp2 cells can specifically drive inter-male aggression in even non-aggressive naïve male mice whereas activating MEADbx1 cells does not promote any specific social behaviors to a significant level.
MeAFoxp2 cells but not MeADbx1 cells are necessary for inter-male aggression in experienced animals
We next asked whether MeAFoxp2and MeADbx1 cells are necessary for social behaviors, including inter-male aggression. We injected Cre- and Flp-dependent hM4Di-mCherry into the MeA of Foxp2cre and Dbx1cre;LSLFlp male mice respectively (Foxp2hM4Di and Dbx1hM4Di). Control animals were injected with mCherry virus (Fig. 7l, m). Three weeks after viral injection, all animals went through repeated resident-intruder test until they showed stable level of aggression (Fig. 7n). Then, we i.p. injected saline and CNO on separate days in a randomized order and 30 minutes later tested the behaviors against a male and then a receptive female intruder, each for 10 minutes (Fig. 7n). After CNO injection, Foxp2hM4Di mice spent more time investigating the male intruders and less time attacking the intruder (Fig. 7o-q). The latency to first attack increased significantly (Fig. 7r). Foxp2mCherry mice showed no difference in male investigation or attack duration between CNO and saline injected days (Fig. 7o-r). In contrast, CNO injection in Dbx1hM4Di mice did not result in significant changes in male investigation, aggressive behaviors, or latency to attack (Fig. 7s-v). CNO injection in Foxp2hM4Di or Dbx1hM4Di mice caused no change in female investigation or any aspects of male sexual behaviors except an increase in mount number in both Dbx1hM4Di and Dbx1mcherry groups (Extended Data Fig. 2l-r, 3l-r). These results suggest that MeAFoxp2 cells are required specifically for inter-male aggression while MeADbx1 cells are not.
Differential outputs of MeADbx1 and MeAFoxp2 cells
As MeADbx1 and MeAFoxp2 cells play differential roles in driving social behaviors, presumably through their differential impact on downstream cells, we next asked whether these two MeA subpopulations differ in their projections using anterograde virus tracing (Fig. 8a-d). We observed that both MeA subpopulations project mainly to other extended amygdala areas, such as PA, COA and posterior BNST (BNSTp), and medial hypothalamus (MH) (Fig. 8e-i, Extended Data Fig. 4). Although the average density of projections originating from MeADbx1 and MeAFoxp2 did not differ in any brain region (Fig. 8e), we observed that MeADbx1 and MeAFoxp2 showed differential projection patterns in the pBNST and MH. While MeADbx1 cells targeted primarily the principal nucleus of the BNST (BNSTpr), MeAFoxp2 cells projected to both principal and interfascicular parts of the BNST (BNSTpr and BNSTif) (Fig. 8j, k). In the MH, we observed that MeADbx1 cells generally provided more inputs to structures in the anterior MH (Bregma level: 0.14– −0.75 mm) than posterior MH (Bregma level: −1.25-−2.15 mm) whereas MeAFoxp2 cells projected to the anterior and posterior MH similarly (Fig. 8l, m).
(a and c) Strategies for anterograde viral tracing of MeAFoxp2 (a) and MeADbx1 (c) cells. Pie charts showing the distribution of primary infected cells.
(b and d) Representative histological images showing the primary infected cells in Foxp2cre (b) and Dbx1cre;LSL-FlpO mice (d).Green: eGFP or GCaMP6f expression. Blue: DAPI staining.
(e) The intensity of MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 projection field in various regions, calculated as the average pixel intensity in a given region divided by the maximum average value across all regions.
(f and h) Representative histological images showing MeAFoxp2 (f) and MeADbx1 (h) projections at various downstream regions.
(g and i) Overviews of MeAFoxp2 (g) and MeADbx1 (i) cell outputs.
(j) Images showing MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cell outputs at pBNST.
(k) The intensity of fibers, originating from MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells, at BNSTpr over that in BNSTif.
(l) Representative histological images showing MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 projections at the anterior and posterior medial hypothalamus (aMH and pMH).
(m) The intensity of fibers, originating from MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells, at pMH over that in aMH. pMH: Bregma −1.25 mm to −2.15mm; aMH: Bregma 0.14mm to −0.75mm.
COApm: posteromedial cortical amygdala; COApl: posterolateral cortical amygdala; AAA: anterior amygdalar area; BA: bed nucleus of the accessory olfactory tract; AVPV: anteroventral periventricular nucleus; PMv: ventral premammillary nucleus. (e) Two-way repeated measures ANOVA followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons test. (k, m) Two-tailed unpaired t-test. n = number of mice. Data are mean ± S.E.M.; *p<0.05.
Discussion
In this study, we showed that two MeA subpopulations with different development lineages play distinct roles in social behaviors. They receive differential anatomical inputs and are responsive to distinct conspecific sensory cues. The male specific responses of MeAFoxp2 cells exist prior to puberty and aggression onset, suggesting that it is largely developmentally hardwired. The reliability, but not specificity, of MeAFoxp2 cell responses improve with adult social experience, demonstrating distinct roles of nature vs. nurture in establishing the social behavior circuit.
MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cell activity and function in innate social behaviors
Our previous work had identified two developmentally distinct GABAergic MeA subpopulations, marked by the expression of Dbx1 and Foxp29,24. These two subpopulations differ in their sex steroid hormone receptor expression, ion channel composition, and intrinsic electrophysiological properties9,34. Our current study further revealed their distinct functions in social behaviors that are well matched with their connectivity and in vivo response patterns. These results suggest that social circuits at the MeA could be largely hardwired according to transcription factor-defined genetic programs.
MeAFoxp2 cells responded strongly during both male investigation and attack. Importantly, MeAFoxp2 show higher responses during male investigation when it is followed by attack, suggesting that the attack response is not simply due to sensory inputs when the animals are in close proximity. These results suggest that MeA is not merely a sensory relay, instead, it could serve a direct role in driving consummatory social actions. Consistent with this hypothesis, activation of MeAFoxp2 cells promoted male-directed attack even in inexperienced non-aggressive male mice.
Hong et. al. showed originally that optogenetic activation of MeA GABAergic cells can induce attack16 but a recent study found the manipulation was ineffective35. These opposite results appear to be caused by the different photocurrent magnitude of the chosen opsin: only ChR2 variants with large, but not small photocurrents, can induce attack from MeA GABAergic cells16 35,36. Given that MeAFoxp2 cells have lower resting membrane potential, lower input resistance and lower spontaneous firing rate in comparison to MeADbx1 cells9, we speculate that MeAFoxp2 cells could be relatively hard to activate, which may explain why strong optogenetic activation is needed to induce attack from the MeA. Importantly, as activating MeADbx1 cells, which are three times more abundant than MeAFoxp2 cells, do not elicit attack, our study clearly argues that aggression generation requires activation of specific, instead of a random subset of MeA GABAergic cells.
In contrast to MeAFoxp2 cells, MeADbx1 cells are tuned to broad social cues, including those from males, females and pups, but respond minimally during the action phase of social behaviors. They are suppressed during mounting and showed similar responses in male investigation-only and investigation-followed-by attack trials. Consistent with their lack of activity change during social actions, inactivation of MeADbx1 cells does not impair male sexual and aggressive behaviors. Given the response pattern of MeADbx1 cells, we consider their main role as to process social cues during the investigatory phase. However, animals with inactivated MeADbx1 cells properly directed their attack towards males and mount towards females, suggesting that MeADbx1 cells are dispensable for sex discrimination. The lack of behavior deficits after MeADbx1 manipulation is possibly due to the existence of other extended amygdala populations that can readily distinguish male and female cues during social investigation, e.g. MeAFoxp2 and aromatase cells in BNSTpr37.
Previous work has shown that MeA GABAergic cells are activated during pup-directed attack and can promote infanticide22. However, neither MeAFoxp2 nor MeADbx1 cells increased activity during pup-directed aggression or affected infanticide when being artificially activated. This result suggests that MeAFoxp2 is specialized for aggression towards males. Other GABAergic subclasses likely exist for driving infanticide and remain to be identified.
Developmentally wired vs. experientially wired
There is an ongoing debate whether the responses of cells in the SBN are developmentally hardwired or established through adult social experience. In the VMHvl, an essential region for male aggression38-40, individual cell responses to male and female cues overlap extensively in naïve adult male mice and only diverge after repeated interaction with females41. In contrast, aromatase expressing cells in male BNSTpr were found to preferentially respond to female cues over male cues even in naïve animals37. Ca2+ imaging in the MeA revealed that approximately half of MeA cells are tuned to one stimulus in naïve animals and after sexual experience the proportion of cells that are responsive to the opposite sex increases, denoting experience-dependent activity refinement10. In our study, MeAFoxp2 cells showed strong male-biased responses in naïve animals suggesting that male olfactory inputs are developmentally wired to target MeAFoxp2 cells. However, the responses of MeAFoxp2 cells in naïve males are slow and unreliable and only become fast and consistent after repeated social interactions, suggesting that adult social experience plays an important role in refining the hardwired circuit to improve its input (sensory cue)-output (spiking) transformation efficiency.
How is the male specific response of MeAFoxp2 cells achieved during development? The classical ‘organization/activation’ model states that gonadal hormones act in two phases to establish sex-specific circuits42-44. First, during the organization stage, gonadal hormones during prenatal development set up the basic structure and connection of the circuit. Then, the circuits are activated by gonadal hormones during puberty to generate appropriate sex-specific social behaviors. In male mice, puberty occurs between P30 and P40 when testosterone spikes and aggression emerges29,42. Previous single-unit recordings found that social response selectivity of MeA cells in anaesthetized juveniles (P18-21) is lower than that in adults, suggesting that sex-hormone mediated circuit “activation” during puberty is important for establishing adult MeA responses6. Here, our longitudinal recording revealed male-biased responses of MeAFoxp2 cells even before puberty, suggesting that the male cues have already been wired preferentially to MeAFoxp2 cells during the organization stage. After puberty, MeAFoxp2 cells show enhanced male-biased responses due to decreased responses to other social cues, e.g. female. As MeAFoxp2 cells do not express aromatase, which is important for the activation of the male territorial aggression circuit during development, and only express low levels of steroid hormone receptors9, hormone actions onto MeAFoxp2 cells might be limited. Therefore, we speculate that changes in the synaptic inputs that suppress non-male related inputs could be the main mechanism responsible for the increased specificity after puberty. As the animals become full adults and acquire social experiences, the response to non-male cues remains low while responses to males continue to increase. Altogether, we propose that the response specificity of MeAFoxp2 cells during development is achieved through a multistage process, including pre-pubertal hardwiring, pubertal refinement, and adult social experience-dependent potentiation. Future microcircuit studies could help further validate this model and its potential generality in the SBN.
Social behavior circuits beyond MeA
In mice, olfactory inputs are the most essential for determining the identity of a conspecific, e.g. its sex, age, social ranking and health state (e.g. sickness)45. Since MeAFoxp2 cells receive little direct input from the AOB and other primary olfactory relays, we speculate that MeAFoxp2 cells obtain highly “processed” olfactory information from the PA. Recent work from our group and others revealed that PA cells that project to the VMHvl are crucial for territorial aggression and these cells are activated during both male investigation and attack46,47. The PA also projects strongly to MeA; however, whether this projection is essential for aggression remains to be explored. On the contrary, MeADbx1 cells receive abundant inputs from AOB and other primary olfactory processing regions, which could be responsible for the broad and fast responses of MeADbx1 cells to various social cues.
At the output level, MeADbx1 and MeAFoxp2 cells project to distinct pBNST subnuclei: MeADbx1 cells project primarily to the BNSTpr while MeAFoxp2 cells project mainly to the BNSTif. Miller et al recently demonstrated that MeA cells that express D1R primarily targets the BNSTif and activating MeAD1R-BNST projections increased territorial aggression towards a conspecific48. This highlights the relevant role of BNSTif in aggression, and a potential downstream mechanism by which MeAFoxp2 cells mediate aggressive action. Additionally, MeADbx1 cells project mainly to anterior MH while MeAFoxp2 project similarly to anterior and posterior MH. Given that anterior MH, such as the anteroventral periventricular nucleus (AVPV) and the MPN, is most relevant for sexual behaviors, while the posterior MH, such as the VMHvl and the ventral premammillary nucleus (PMv), is central for male aggression39,40,49, the stronger projection of MeAFoxp2 cells to posterior MH in comparison to MeADbx1 is consistent with the essential role of MeAFoxp2 cells in male aggression.
Transcription factor code in the limbic system
Analogous to the transcriptional code observed in the spinal cord and basal ganglia for cellular specificity of intrinsic physiology, connectivity and motor control, we suggest a transcription factor code in the limbic system by which distinct sets of transcriptionally-defined subpopulations differing in their intrinsic properties and connectivity mediate diverse behavioral functions50,51. Previous work investigating the LIM-homeodomain family of transcription factors, found two distinct MeA subpopulations expressing Lhx6 and Lhx9 that are relevant for reproduction and predator defense behaviors respectively7. Our results provide an in vivo understanding of additional transcriptionally defined subpopulations relevant for specific social behaviors, such as aggression.
A role of MeAFoxp2 in generating aggression is consistent with a role of Foxp2 in the basal ganglia, cerebellum and cortex in modulating motor actions52. Previous work has shown that Foxp2 expression is required for distinct components of motor actions: Foxp2 in the cerebellum is essential for appropriate response-time and motor execution, in the striatum it decreases response variability, while in the cortex it is needed for appropriate motor performance52. In addition, given its expression in sensory processing regions, such as thalamus and association cortex, Foxp2 has been considered essential for sensorimotor integration of external cues, particularly auditory, for appropriate limbic movements53-55. Similar to cortical and subcortical regions, MeAFoxp2 cells are relevant for attack, a stereotyped aggressive action. Importantly, here we demonstrate that MeAFoxp2 involvement in motor generation appears to be tightly linked to its direct role in processing male specific olfactory inputs, going beyond its known role in auditory information processing. In parallel to our findings regarding MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1, the globus pallidus externa (GPe) has been shown to comprise different inhibitory subpopulations arising from distinct progenitor pools in the medial and lateral/central ganglionic eminences, including an arkypallial subtype that expresses Foxp2 (GPeFoxp2) with intrinsic biophysical properties similar to that of MeAFoxp2 cells, and a prototypical subtype with intrinsic properties similar to those of MeADbx1 cells9,34,51. Nevertheless, differences across regions remain. The GPeFoxp2 functions primarily as a movement generator, similar to cerebellum and cortex, while MeAFoxp2 cells do not encode moment-to-moment movement, but instead, a specific behavior output (i.e. attack) comprised of a complex sequence of actions.
Overall, our study identified a developmentally hardwired circuit at the MeA that transforms male conspecific cues to attack command. It revealed the distinct contribution of development vs. experience in social information processing and highlighted a lineage-based organization strategy that enables the same SBN to drive diverse social behaviors2.
Methods
Mice
All animal procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of NYU Langone Health. Adult experimental and stimulus mice were housed under a 12 hr light-dark cycle (10a.m. to 10p.m. dark) with water and food ad libitum. After surgical procedures, all experimental animals were single-housed. The Foxp2cre mice were originally provided by Dr. Richard Palmiter (now Jackson stock no. 030541)27. The Dbx1cre mice were originally provided by Dr. Alessandra Pierani and crossed to the Flp excised and Cre-inducible LSL-FlpO mouse line or to the Ai6 mouse line (Jackson stock no. 028584 and no. 007906 respectively)25,26,28. Both Foxp2cre and Dbx1cre mice are black, while the fur color of LSL-FlpO mice is agouti. Stimulus animals were C57BL/6N and 129S4/SvJae group-housed females, pups (P1-P7) and group-housed BALB/c males purchased from Charles River and bred in-house. Females were considered receptive if an experienced male was able to mount and intromit the female in at least 3 attempts.
Viruses and stereotaxis surgery
For fiber photometry experiments, we injected 100nl of AAV2-CAG-Flex-GCaMP6f (2.21 × 1013 vg/ml or 1.82×1012 vg/ml; UPenn viral core) unilaterally into the MeA (AP: −1.5mm, ML= 2.15mm, DV: −5.1mm) of Foxp2cre+/- male mice. For Dbx1cre+/-;FlpO+/- mice we injected either 100nl AAV8-Ef1a-fDIO-GCaMP6f (1×1013 vg/ml; kindly provided by Dr. Uchida) or 120nl of mixed AAV9-Ef1a-fDIO-Cre (2.5×1013 vg/ml; Addgene) and AAV2-CAG-Flex-GCaMP6f (1:2; 2.21×1013 vg/ml; UPenn viral core) or 150nl of AAV2-Ef1a-fDIO-GCaMP6f (4.1 × 1012 vg/ml;UNC vector core) into the MeA. For fiber photometry recordings in Foxp2cre+/- juvenile mice we injected 100nl of AAV1-CAG-Flex-GCaMP6f (9.4 × 1012 vg/ml; UPenn viral core) unilaterally into the developing MeA (AP: −0.7mm, ML= 2.03mm, DV: −5.05mm). For chemogenetic experiments, we bilaterally injected either 400-600nl of AAV1-Ef1a-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mcherry, 150nl of AAV2-hSyn-DIO-hM3D(Gq)-mcherry or 150-600nl of AAV2-hSyn-DIO-mCherry (3×1012 vg/ml, 5.1×1012 vg/ml and 5.6×1012 vg/ml, respectively; Addgene and UNC Vector Core) into the MeA of Foxp2cre+/- mice. For chemogenetic experiments in Dbx1cre+/-;FlpO+/- mice, we injected 300nl AAVDJ-hSyn-fDIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry, 50-60nl AAV2-Ef1a-fDIO-hM3D(Gq)-mCherry (Vigene) and 60-120nl AAV2-Ef1a-fDIO-mCherry (2.65×1013 vg/ml, 1.84×1013 vg/ml and 1.1×1013 vg/ml, respectively; Addgene). For monosynaptic retrograde rabies experiments in Foxp2cre+/- mice we injected unilaterally into the MeA 250-500nl of mixed AAV1-CA-Flex-RG and AAV8-Ef1-Flex-TVA-mCherry (1:1; 3×1012 vg/ml and 5.4 × 1012 vg/ml; UNC vector core) and 4 weeks later 800nl EnvA G-Deleted Rabies-eGFP (Salk viral vector core). For monosynaptic retrograde rabies experiments in Dbx1cre+/-;FlpO+/- mice we injected mixed 110-120nl AAV8-Flex(FRT)-G and AAV8-Flex(FRT)-TVA-mCherry (1:1; 1.82×1013 vg/ml and 1.39×1013 vg/ml; Stanford gene vector and viral Core) and 4 weeks later 800nl EnvA G-Deleted Rabies-eGFP (Salk viral core). We also unilaterally injected 80-100nl of AAVDJ-Ef1a-fDIO-EYFP (2.1×1012 vg/ml; UNC vector core) into the MeA of Dbx1cre+/-;FlpO+/- mice for anterograde tracing experiments. For Chr2-assisted circuit mapping, we injected 150nl of AAV2-Flex-GFP (3.7×1012 vg/ml; UNC vector core) unilaterally into the MeA of Foxp2cre+/- mice and 40-200nl AAV9-hSyn-ChrimsonR-tdTomato (5.5 × 1012 vg/ml; Addgene) unilaterally into the olfactory bulb (AP: 4.45mm, ML= 0.25mm, DV: −1.55mm) of Foxp2cre+/- and Dbx1cre+/-;Ai6+/- mice. EnvA G-deleted Rabies virus titers were >1.00×108transforming units per ml.
During surgery, adult male mice were anaesthetized with isoflurane (2%) and then placed in a stereotaxic apparatus (Kopf Instruments). For fiber photometry recordings in juvenile mice, P11 pups were anesthetized with isoflurane (2%) and placed in a stereotaxic apparatus modified with a neonatal anesthesia head holder and zygoma ear cups (Kopf Instruments). The virus or tracer was then delivered into the target region of interest in pups or adults through a glass capillary by using a nanoinjector (World Precision Instruments). For fiber photometry experiments in adults, a 400μm optical fiber (Thorlabs, FT400EMT) attached to a ceramic ferrule (Thorlabs, CF440-10) was placed 0.3mm dorsal to the viral injection site and cemented with adhesive dental cement (C&B metabond, S380). A 3D printed head-fix ring was also secured with cement to the skull. For juvenile experiments, juveniles at P24 were implanted with the optical fiber in the MeA (AP: −0.7mm, ML= 2.03mm, DV: −4.75mm) but the head-fix ring was not utilized. Histology analysis was performed for all animals and only animals with correct virus expression and fiber placement were used for final analysis.
Behavioral assays and analysis
Behavior was recorded by two synchronized top and side cameras (Basler, acA640-100gm) at 25 frames/second and a digital video recording software (Streampix 5, Norpix) in a dark-room with infrared lights. Behaviors were manually annotated on a frame-by-frame basis by using a custom Matlab function named ‘BehaviorAnnotator’ (https://github.com/pdollar/toolbox).
For male-male interactions we annotated investigation, groom, mount, and attack. For fiber photometry analysis, investigation and groom have been combined as ‘investigation’. For male-female interactions, we recorded investigation, mount, intromission and ejaculation. For male-pup interactions, we recorded investigation, groom, carry and infanticide. ‘Investigation’ was considered as nose-contact to any body part of the target mouse. ‘Groom’ was classified when a mouse has its front paws holding the back or face of the target mouse and is licking either face or back. ‘Attack’ was determined as a series of actions by which the male mouse lounged, bite, chased and pushed the target mouse. ‘Mount’ was defined as a series of fast movements by which the male mouse placed its front paws on the target mouse and positioned itself on top of the target mouse. ‘Intromission’ was annotated as rhythmic deep thrusts following mount. ‘Ejaculation’ was considered when the male stopped deep thrusting and froze in place for several seconds while strongly holding the target female mouse and then slumping to the side. ‘Carry’ involved the male mouse grabbing the pup with mouth, lifting and dropping it off at another location in the cage. ‘Infanticide’ was considered as biting the pup that result in tissue damage. For chemogenetic analysis, pup investigation and groom, were combined as ‘pup investigation’.
Fiber photometry
Foxp2cre+/- and Dbx1cre+/-;FlpO+/- male mice aged 2-8 months were used for adult fiber photometry recordings. Foxp2cre male mice starting at age P25 were used for juvenile fiber photometry experiments. For adult head-fixed experiments, the mice were naïve and did not have had any interactions with other conspecifics outside of their littermates. The recording mouse was head-fixed using a 3D printed head-ring and placed on a 3D printed wheel 56. Mice were trained on the wheel for a minimum of three days for at least 10 minutes each day. Each stimulus was presented 5 times for 10 sec with a 50 sec interval in between presentations and a minimum of 5 min break in between different stimuli. Male and receptive female stimulus mice were anaesthetized with ketamine (100mg/kg) and xylazine (10mg/kg).
Fiber photometry was performed as previously described 46,57,58. To analyze changes in Ca2+ activity, Matlab function ‘msbackadj’, with a moving window of 25% of the total recording time, was utilized to obtain the instantaneous baseline signal (Fbaseline). The instantaneous ΔF/F was calculated as (Fraw –Fbaseline)/Fbaseline). The z-score of the ΔF/F (Fz) was obtained by using the Matlab function ‘zscore’ for the whole trace. The peri-event histograms (PETHs) were calculated by aligning the Fz of each trial to either the onset or offset of each behavior. In recordings of head-fix naïve male mice (Fig. 2), the Fz peak was calculated by obtaining the average of the maximum value during stimulus presentation. The male preference index (PI) was calculated as (Zinvestigate male – 0.5 × (Zinvestigate female + Zinvestigate pup))/( Zinvestigate male + 0.5 × |Zinvestigate female + Zinvestigate pup|); The female PI was calculated as (Zinvestigate female – 0.5 × (Zinvestigate male + Zinvestigate pup))/( Zinvestigate female + 0.5 × |Zinvestigate male + Zinvestigate pup|); The pup PI was calculated as (Zinvestigate pup – 0.5 × (Zinvestigate male + Zinvestigate female))/( Zinvestigate pup + 0.5 ×
|Zinvestigate female + Zinvestigate male|).
When recording from freely moving naïve mice (Figs. 3-5), a receptive female, an adult male mouse and pup were introduced into the cage for 10 mins each except the pup (P1-7) which was introduced for 5 mins. For freely-moving experienced male mice, a pup was introduced into the resident’s cage for 5 mins, the male intruder was placed in the cage for a minimum of 10 mins until the recording mice elicited >6 attacks, without exceeding a total of 1 hour in the cage. A receptive female was introduced until 5 mins after the recording mouse ejaculated. The response elicited during a behavior was calculated as the average Fz during that behavior, while the Fz peak during introduction was calculated as the peak Fz during the first 100 sec after intruder introduction. The male, female and pup PIs were calculated as aforementioned for head-fixed mice. The introduction male, female and pup PIs were calculated using the average Fz during the first 100 sec of stimulus introduction.
When comparing naïve freely-moving and experienced male mice responses, the latency to respond was calculated as the time lapse from behavior onset to when the response reaches Z >= 1. The ‘percent of trials to respond’ was calculated as the percentage of trials that reached Z >= 1. ‘Sniff per trial(s)’ was calculated as the average duration of all male investigation trials. Heatmaps were constructed as FZ – Fz at time 0 for each trial.
Chemogenetic mediated activation and silencing
For chemogenetic activation experiments, experimental male mice were naïve and had no prior social experience except their littermates. On day 1, male mice were i.p. injected with saline and 30 min after injection, video recordings started. After a 5 min baseline period, a pup intruder was placed into the cage for 5 mins, followed by a 10 min presentation of an adult male, and a 10 min presentation of a receptive female, with 5 min breaks in between stimulus presentation. On day 2, male mice were i.p. injected with 1mg/kg of CNO (Sigma, C0832) and pup, adult male and receptive female were introduced as in day 1.
For chemogenetic silencing experiments, experimental male mice were trained to attack by introducing an adult male mouse daily for 10-30mins minutes/day until they could reliably attack within a 10 min period. Mice were then i.p. injected with saline or CNO (1mg/kg) on interleaved days for two rounds. Thirty minutes after injection, behavioral recordings started and after a 5 min baseline period, an adult male or a receptive female was introduced into the cage for 10 mins each with a 5 min break. Animals with correct bilateral histology were included for analysis.
Animal body tracking
The velocity (pixels/frame) of each animal after 30 mins of saline or CNO i.p. injection was obtained during the first 5 mins of the chemogenetic assay prior to introduction of any stimulus. The location of each animal was tracked using the top-view camera recordings and analyzed using a custom-written Matlab GUI and code (https://github.com/pdollar/toolbox) 39.
Immunohistochemistry and imaging analysis
Mice were anesthetized and perfused with 1x PBS followed by 20ml 4% PFA. Brains were fixed in 4% PFA for 6-12 hrs at 4°C and dehydrated in 15% sucrose overnight. Brains were embedded in O.C.T. compound (Sakura, 4583) and cut in 50µm sections using a cryostat (Leica CM1950). Every third section was used for immunohistochemistry. Free floating sections were incubated with primary antibody in PBST (0.3% Triton X in PBS) and blocked in 10% normal donkey serum (Jackson ImmunoResearch, 017-000-121) at room temperature in a shaker overnight. The brain sections were then washed 5x in PBST for 10 mins and placed in secondary antibody in PBST and blocked in 10% normal donkey serum for 4 hours at room temperature or overnight at 4°C degrees. Brain sections were then washed 5x in PBST for 10 mins, mounted (Fisher Scientific, 12-550-15) and cover-slipped using fluoromount mounting media with DAPI (ThermoFisher, 00-4959-52). Primary antibodies used were rabbit anti-Foxp2 (1:500, abcam ab16046), rat anti-GFP (1:1000, Nacalai 04404-84), and rabbit anti-mCherry (1:1000, TaKaRa Living Colors DsRed Polyclonal Ab 632496). Secondary antisera used were donkey anti-rat Alexa 488 (1:300; Jackson ImmunoResearch 712-545-150), and donkey anti-rabbit Cy3 (1:1000, Jackson ImmunoResearch 711-165-152). Sections were imaged using a slide scanner (Olympus, VS120) or a confocal microscope (Zeiss LSM 800). Brain sections were identified based on the Allen Mouse Brain Atlas and counted manually using Adobe Photoshop. Cells stained with DAPI were counted using the ImageJ software to automatically count these cells using the ‘analyze particles’ feature and manually corrected.
Monosynaptic-retrograde rabies input mapping
To determine the inputs to MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells we injected adult male mice with Cre or Flp dependent AAV-G and AAV-TVA-mCherry viruses and 4 weeks later with EnvA G-Deleted Rabies-eGFP. After 7 days, mice were perfused and every one in three brain sections were collected (50µm thickness sections). Starter cells were considered TVA-mCherry and Rabies-eGFP double positive. Upstream Rabies-eGFP cells were then counted using the ImageJ software. Due to close proximity with the MeA starter cell location, the LH, anterior MeA and AAA were excluded from analysis. Brains with more than 70% of starter cells in the MeA were considered for further analysis. Regions with more than 2% of total inputs to MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells were included in Fig. 6.
Output axonal projection mapping
To determine the projection patterns of MeAFoxp2 and MeADbx1 cells, every one in three brain sections were collected (50µm thickness). A box area encompassing each region of interest was selected and average pixel intensity was obtained using Adobe Photoshop and calculated as Iraw. On the same image, a box area of the same size but on the contralateral side with no terminals was used to calculate the background intensity as Ibackground. The Isignal was obtained by subtracting Ibackground from Iraw and then normalizing the value by the maximum Isignal across all brain regions for each animal (Inorm) 57. The average Inorm was then calculated for all animals to obtain the average axonal projection intensity for each terminal field. Animals with more than 65% of starter cells in the MeA were considered for analysis. Regions with more than 0.2 normalized intensity were included in Fig. 8. The LH and anterior MeA were excluded from analysis due to close proximity to the starter cells.
Brain slice electrophysiology
For AOB to MeA circuit mapping experiments, we injected AAV2-Flex-eGFP and AAV9-hSyn-ChrimsonR-tdTomato into the MeA and the AOB, respectively, of Foxp2cre+/- male mice; or AAV9-hSyn-ChrimsonR-tdTomato into the AOB of Dbx1cre+/-Ai6+/- male mice. Whole cell patch-clamp recordings were performed on MeA slices from all mice.
Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane, and brains were removed and submerged in ice-cold cutting solution containing (in mM): 110 choline chloride, 25 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 7 MgCl2, 0.5 CaCl2, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 25 glucose, 11.6 ascorbic acid and 3.1 pyruvic acid. Coronal sections of 275 um were cut on a Leica VT1200s vibratome and incubated in artificial cerebral spinal fluid (ACSF) containing (in mM): 125 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, 1 MgCl2, 2 CaCl2 and 11 glucoses at 34°C for 30 min and then transferred to room temperature for cell recovery until the start of recording. Whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings were performed with micropipettes filled with intracellular solution containing (in mM): 135 CsMeSO3, 10 HEPES, 1 EGTA, 3.3 QX-314 (chloride salt), 4 Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na-GTP and 8 sodium phosphocreatine (pH 7.3 adjusted with CsOH). Signals were recorded using MultiClamp 700B amplifier, digitized by DigiData1550B with sampling rate 20 kHz (Molecular Devices, USA). Data were analyzed using Clampfit (Molecular Devices) or MATLAB (Mathworks). To activate ChrimsonR-expressing axons, brief pulses of full field illumination (pE-300 white; CoolLED, 605 nm, 1 ms duration, 10 repeats, with 6 s interval) were delivered onto the recorded cell. Optogenetically-evoked EPSCs and IPSCs (oESPSs and oIPSCs) were recorded by holding the membrane potential of recorded neurons at −70 and 0 mV, respectively. ACSF, TTX (1 µM), TTX (1 µM) and 4-AP (100 mM were sequentially used to test if optogenetically evoked responses are monosynaptic. All drugs were pre-applied for 5 min in the slice chamber prior to data acquisition. Latency was measured as the time difference when the current exceeded 1.5 folds of standard deviation of baseline compared to the light onset.
Data and code availability
Data to support the findings and custom-written data analysis code (Matlab) is available upon reasonable request from the corresponding authors.
Statistics
All statistical analysis was performed using Matlab or Graphpad Prism software. Statistical analysis performed were two-tailed. Parametric tests, including paired and unpaired t-test and one-way ANOVA, were used if distributions passed Shapiro-Wilk normality test (except one-way ANOVA with missing values, and for sample size ≤4 and two-way ANOVA, in which data normality was assumed, but not tested). If data was not normally distributed, non-parametric tests were used. To determine differences between a group and a hypothetical value, a one sample test was performed, followed by an analysis of multiple p-values using the original FDR method of Benjamini and Hochberg at Q=5%, to correct for multiple comparisons. For comparisons between more than 2 groups, one-way ANOVA or RM one-way ANOVA was performed followed by Tukey’s multiple comparisons test (normally distributed data); Friedman test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons test (RM, not normally distributed data); or Kruskal-Wallis test followed by Dunn’s multiple comparisons test (non-matching groups, not normally distributed data). For differences between groups with two independent variables, two-way ANOVA was performed followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons test. All significant p-values <0.05 were indicated on the figures. *p< 0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001; ****p<0.0001. For detailed statistical analysis, see statistic summary table.
Author contributions
J.E.L., D.L. and J.G.C. conceived the project. J.E.L. and D.L. designed experiments, analyzed the data and co-wrote the manuscript. D.L. supervised the project. J.E.L. conducted most experiments. L.Y. performed in vitro electrophysiology experiments. C.S. assisted with chemogenetic and fiber photometry experiments. J.B. G.S. and M.G. assisted with histology and behavior annotation. N.P. worked on preliminary characterization of axonal projections. J.G.C. provided feedback throughout the course of the study and supervised N.P.
Declaration of Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Extended Data Figures and Legends
(a) Schematic of viral strategy for fiber photometry recordings and fiber photometry setup.
(b) Experimental timeline for Ca2+ recordings in freely-moving naïve and experienced male mice.
(c) Average PETHs of MeAFoxp2 Ca2+ signal aligned to the onset (left) and offset (right) of investigation only (blue) and investigation followed by attack (purple). Open circles denote the time period when the investigation-only and investigation-followed-by-attack responses are significantly different (q<0.05).
(d) Average PETHs of MeADbx1 cell responses aligned to the onset (left) and offset (right) of investigation only (blue) and investigation followed by attack (purple). The two traces do not differ significantly at any time point.
(e and f) Representative Ca2+ traces (e1, f1) and PETHs (e2, f2) of MeAFoxp2 (e) and MeADbx1 (f) cells during the presentation of a novel object.
(c and d) One sample t-test, corrected for multiple comparisons with FDR 0.05. n= number of mice. Data are mean ± S.E.M.
(a) In Foxp2hM3Dq and Foxp2mCherry male mice, latency to attack a male intruder after CNO injection did not differ from that after saline injection. Only animals that showed attack after both saline and CNO injections were included for this analysis.
(b) No changes in velocity (pixels/frame) in Foxp2hM3Dq or Foxp2mCherry male mice were observed in a 5 min period 30 min after CNO or saline injection when the test animal was alone in its cage.
(c) Number of Foxp2hM3Dq or Foxp2mCherry male mice that attacked pups vs. those that did not after saline or CNO injection. Each circle represents one mouse.
(d) Percentage of time Foxp2hM3Dq or Foxp2mCherry male mice spent investigating the pup after saline or CNO injection.
(e) Representative raster plots showing the behaviors of 5 Foxp2hM3Dq and 5 Foxp2mCherry mice after i.p. injection of saline or CNO in the presence of a female intruder.
(f-k) Between CNO-injected and saline-injected days, there is no difference in any parameters related to male sexual behaviors in Foxp2mCherry as well as Foxp2hM3Dq male mice.
(l) Representative raster plots showing the behaviors of 5 Foxp2hM4Di and 5 Foxp2mCherry mice after i.p. injection of saline or CNO in the presence of a female intruder.
(m-r) Between CNO-injected and saline-injected days, there is no difference in any parameters related to male sexual behaviors in Foxp2mCherry as well as Foxp2hM4Di male mice.
(b, d, f-k, m-r) Two-way repeated measures ANOVA followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons test; (c) McNemar’s test. n = number of mice. Data are mean ± S.E.M.
(a) In Dbx1hM3Dq and Dbx1mCherry male mice, latency to attack a male intruder after CNO injection did not differ from that after saline injection. Only animals that showed attack after both saline and CNO injections were included for this analysis.
(b) Velocity (pixels/frame) of Dbx1hM3Dq or Dbx1mCherry male mice in a 5 min period after 30 min CNO or saline injection when the test animal was alone in its home cage.
(c) Number of Dbx1hM3Dq and Dbx1mCherry male mice that attacked pups vs. those that did not after saline or CNO injection. Each circle represents one mouse.
(d) Percentage of time Dbx1hM3Dq and Dbx1mCherry male mice spent investigating the pup after saline or CNO injection.
(e) Representative raster plots showing the behaviors of 5 Dbx1hM3Dq and 5 Dbx1mCherry mice after i.p. injection of saline or CNO in the presence of a female intruder.
(f-k) No difference in male sexual behaviors after CNO injection in comparison to saline injection in Dbx1mCherry nor in Dbx1hM3Dq male mice.
(l) Representative raster plots showing the behaviors of 5 Dbx1hM4Di and 5 Dbx1mCherry mice after i.p. injection of saline or CNO in the presence of a female intruder.
(m-r) No difference in male sexual behaviors after CNO injection in comparison to saline injection in Dbx1mCherry nor in Dbx1hM4Di male mice.
(a, b, d, f-k, m-r) Two-way repeated measures ANOVA followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons test; (c) McNemar’s test. n = number of animals. Data are mean ± S.E.M.
(a-b) Representative images of 10 brain regions showing the GFP fibers originating from MeAFoxp2 (a) and MeADbx1 (b) cells. The gain of PA and BNST images in (b) was reduced to avoid complete saturation.
Acknowledgements
We thank all members of Lin lab, Drs. R. Sullivan, J. Dasen and M. Long for their inputs during the course of this study. We thank Y. Jiang and the Genotyping Core Laboratory of NYU Langone Health for genotyping of the mice for this study. We also thank Dr. Naoshige Uchida for kindly providing the AAV8-fDIO-GCaMP6f virus. We thank Drs. A. Pierani and L. Vigier for providing the Dbx1cre+/- mouse line and for primer sequences for genotyping. We thank Dr. R. Palmiter for providing the Foxp2cre+/- mice. This research was supported by a Leon Levy Neuroscience Fellowship and NIMH K99MH127295 (to J.E.L); NIH grants R01MH101377, 1R01HD092596 and U19NS107616 (D.L.); the Mathers Foundation (D.L.); Dean’s Undergraduate Research Funds (to J.B, G.S. and M.G); Collegiate Research Initiative (to J.B.); R01DA020140, R21MH129995 and the PNC Charitable Trust (J.G.C).