Abstract
Dominant motor control theories propose that the brain predicts and attenuates the somatosensory consequences of actions, referred to as somatosensory attenuation. Support comes from psychophysical and neuroimaging studies showing that touch applied on a passive hand elicits attenuated perceptual and neural responses if it is actively generated by one’s other hand, compared to identical touch from an external origin. However, recent experimental findings have challenged this view by providing psychophysical evidence that the perceived intensity of touch on the passive hand is enhanced if the active hand does not receive touch simultaneously with the passive hand (somatosensory enhancement) and by further attributing attenuation to the double tactile stimulation of the hands upon contact. Here, we directly contrasted the hypotheses of the attenuation and enhancement models regarding how action influences somatosensory perception by manipulating whether the active hand contacts the passive hand. We further assessed somatosensory perception in the absence of any predictive cues in a condition that turned out to be essential for interpreting the experimental findings. In three preregistered experiments, we demonstrate that action does not enhance the predicted touch (Experiment 1), that the previously reported “enhancement” effects are driven by the reference condition used (Experiment 2), and that self-generated touch is robustly attenuated regardless of whether the two hands make contact (Experiment 3). Our results provide conclusive evidence that action does not enhance but attenuates predicted touch and prompt a reappraisal of recent experimental findings upon which theoretical frameworks proposing a perceptual enhancement by action prediction are based.
Highlights
Dominant motor control theories propose that action attenuates or cancels predicted touch.
Recent theories propose that action enhances predicted touch.
We show that action does not enhance but attenuates predicted touch.
Introduction
Dominant motor control theories propose that the brain uses an internal forward model in combination with a copy of the motor command (efference copy) to predict the sensory consequences of our movements 1–3. For example, the brain predicts the upcoming touch as one reaches toward an object. These predictions allow for the correction of motor errors without relying on sensory feedback that suffers from intrinsic delays 4, thereby improving the estimation of the current state of our body by combining the predicted touch with the actual sensory input 5–7. These predictions attenuate the perception of the self-generated input (sensory reafference) compared to that of externally generated input 8–10 and infer whether the cause of the sensory input is the self or the environment 11–13. A classic example of this attenuation is that we are unable to tickle ourselves with our own touch, yet we are easily tickled by the touch of others 14. The attenuation of sensory reafference – also referred to as sensory cancelation – is thought to be necessary to compensate for the limited capacity of the sensory systems by optimally prioritising the perception of more informative externally generated stimuli 15,16. Thus, the attenuation model proposes that we dampen perceptual representations of expected self-generated stimuli to reduce redundancy and to highlight behaviourally relevant unexpected externally generated stimuli.
In contrast, an alternative theoretical framework proposes that predictions, including those arising from our motor commands, should not attenuate but instead enhance sensory signals, thereby allowing for sharper (i.e., more accurate) representations of predicted compared to unpredicted sensory events 17–19. This enhancement account – also referred to as the sharpening account – posits that predictions based on our motor commands are equivalent to expectations formed by statistical regularities in sensory input or from prior knowledge (e.g., at the North Pole, one expects to see a polar bear rather than an elephant) and that these predictions should bias our perception towards our expectations. The proposal mainly stems from experimental research outside the domain of action and argues that weak, noisy, or ambiguous sensory input that is in line with prior expectations should be enhanced to achieve, on average, more accurate representations. For example, we are more biased to report the presence of visual events that are statistically likely to occur rather than unlikely events 20,21, more sensitive to low-level visual features that are in line with prior expectations 22,23, and show greater biases when perceiving visual events that are congruent with our expectations 24,25. Such effects are thought to result from mechanisms that increase the “gain” of expected information by altering the weights of different sensory signals 26. Thus, the enhancement model proposes that we amplify the perceptual representations of expected compared to unexpected sensory input.
In the somatosensory domain, evidence supporting the attenuation model has repeatedly shown that touch delivered to one (passive) hand by the other (active) hand (i.e., self-generated touch) is perceived as weaker or less ticklish 27–41 and evokes attenuated somatosensory cortical activation 42–46 compared to touch of identical intensity applied on the passive hand that is externally generated. In contrast, evidence supporting the enhancement model has shown that the action of the active hand results in an increase in the perceived intensity of touch on the passive hand, provided that the active hand never receives touch simultaneously with the passive hand (i.e., hands do not make contact) 47. This enhancement finding has been recently used to support the sharpening model and to argue that attenuation effects are due to unspecific gating processes caused by the simultaneous double tactile stimulation of the two hands 48.
The attenuation (or cancellation) and enhancement (or sharpening) models present strikingly different hypotheses regarding how action influences the perception of sensory input and are supported by contradictory experimental evidence, leading to debates between researchers 12,17,36,49,50. Clarifying how predictions about the sensory consequences of our movements affect our perception is fundamental to understanding the interaction between perception and action 7,16,51 but also for clinical and neurobiological theories of psychosis spectrum disorders, such as schizophrenia 46,52–58 and schizotypy 41, as well as functional movement disorders 59, Parkinson’s disease 60 and ageing 61.
The present study aimed to contribute to this debate by directly contrasting the enhancement and attenuation models within the same paradigm. To this end, the same force discrimination task employed in earlier studies reporting attenuation 27,28,30,31,36,41,62–64 and enhancement 47 was used to determine (a) whether movement of the right hand enhances or attenuates the perceived magnitude of touch applied on the left hand when the two hands do not make contact (Experiments 1 and 2) and (b) whether attenuation effects are due to double tactile stimulation caused by the contact of the two hands or action prediction (Experiment 3). Capitalizing on the fact that any conclusion about whether action prediction “attenuates” or “enhances” the perception of the somatosensory input needs to be made with a comparison to one’s somatosensory perception in the absence of action, we also included a condition in which participants passively received externally generated touch, with which we compared the participants’ perception in all experimental conditions. Consequently, if participants perceive a touch as less or more intense during action than in the absence of action, we can infer that the received touch was attenuated or enhanced, respectively. This is a critical methodological novelty compared to previous studies 28,47 because if such baseline conditions are missing, the same patterns of results can be incorrectly attributed to “attenuation” or “enhancement”: for example, if one condition produces less attenuation than another, it may be interpreted as enhancement, and vice versa. Indeed, it was this baseline condition that turned out to be critical for distinguishing between attenuation and enhancement effects. All studies and analysis plans were preregistered on the Open Science Framework prior to data collection (Methods).
Results
Experiment 1. Action does not enhance predicted touch
Thirty naïve participants moved their right index finger towards their left index finger to generate the touch on their left index finger with (contact condition) or without (no-contact condition) simultaneous stimulation on their active finger. A baseline condition in which the participants did not move their right index finger and received touch on the left index finger passively (externally generated touch) was included in order to distinguish between effects of attenuation or enhancement (Figure 1A).
During the force-discrimination task, participants judged the intensity of a ‘test’ force and a ‘comparison’ force (100 ms duration each) separated by a random interval ranging from 800 ms to 1200 ms. The forces were delivered to the pulp of the left index finger by a cylindrical probe attached to a lever driven by a DC electric motor. In each trial, participants reported which force they felt was stronger (the test or the comparison). The intensity of the test force was fixed at 2 N, while the intensity of the comparison force was systematically varied among seven force levels (1, 1.5, 1.75, 2, 2.25, 2.5 or 3 N). In the contact condition, participants moved their right index finger towards their left index finger after an auditory cue and actively tapped on a force sensor placed on top of, but not in direct contact with, the probe. The participant’s active tap on the force sensor triggered the test force on their left index finger, thereby producing simultaneous stimulation of both fingers in the contact condition and the sensation of pressing with the right index finger against the left index finger through a rigid object. Participants were told that they would always make contact between their fingers (through the probe) in this condition. In the no-contact condition, following the same auditory cue, participants moved their right index finger towards their left index finger. At the beginning of the condition, the force sensor was removed and replaced with a distance sensor that detected the relative distance of their active finger as it approached their left index finger to trigger the test force, similar to 28,47. The distance threshold was set such that the position of the right index finger when triggering the test force was equivalent to that in the contact condition. Participants were told that they would never make contact between their fingers. Before the experiment, participants were trained to make similar movements with their right index finger in the contact and no-contact conditions. Finally, in the baseline condition (externally generated touch), participants were told to relax their right hand, and each trial began with the same auditory cue followed by the test force delivered to the participants’ left index finger 800 ms after the cue. In all trials, the comparison force was delivered after the test force, and participants verbally reported their judgement (Methods).
Participants’ responses were fitted with logistic psychophysics curves, and we extracted the point of subjective equality (PSE), which represents the intensity of the comparison force at which the test force feels equally as strong. Consequently, a PSE in a movement condition (contact or no-contact) that is lower than the PSE of the baseline condition indicates attenuation, while a PSE that is higher than the PSE of the baseline condition indicates enhancement (Methods). According to the attenuation model (Figure 1B), attenuation of the somatosensory input on the left hand in the contact condition compared to the baseline condition should be observed (i.e., lower PSEs) due to the test force being a predicted consequence of the action of the right hand. However, regarding the no-contact condition, earlier studies have shown that the mere movement of one hand is not sufficient to produce predictions of somatosensory input simultaneously applied on the other hand 27,44,65–69. Instead, a bimanual sensorimotor context is needed such as during bimanual object manipulation and bimanual contact of the hands (Blakemore et al., 1998). Thus, while touch in the no-contact condition is self-generated in the sense that it is a consequence of one’s movement, the efference copy and the sensory feedback are inconsistent with the sensorimotor context of touching oneself; in this case, the context of applying force on one’s finger with another finger. In other words, the attenuation model suggests that not any voluntary movement will attenuate upcoming sensory input. Based on this, neither attenuation nor enhancement should be observed in the no-contact condition (i.e., no change in PSEs from baseline). In contrast, according to the enhancement model, if action enhances the received sensation 47, then higher PSEs in the no-contact condition compared to the baseline condition should be observed (Figure 1C). Finally, since the enhancement model proposes that attenuation effects are attributed to unspecific (nonpredictive) gating effects caused by the simultaneous tactile stimulation of the two hands, touch in the contact condition should also be perceived as weaker, albeit not due to action prediction. In summary, Experiment 1 explicitly assessed whether action enhances the received touch when the index fingers of the two hands do not make contact.
The results showed a robust attenuation of the perceived touch when the two fingers made contact (contact condition) (Figure 1D, E, F): the PSEs were significantly lower in the contact condition than in the baseline condition (W = 422.00, p < 0.001, rrb = 0.82, CI95 = [0.08 0.25]). Similarly, the PSEs were significantly lower in the contact condition than in the no-contact condition (W = 441.00, p < 0.001, rrb = 0.90, CI95 = [0.13 0.26] (Figure 1D, E, G). Critically, however, in the comparison that contrasts the hypotheses of the two models, the no-contact condition did not produce any significant change in the perceived magnitude of the touch compared to the baseline condition (W = 187.00, p = 0.360, rrb = −0.20, CI95 = [−0.08 0.030]) (Figure 1D, E, H). A Bayesian factor analysis provided moderate support for the absence of any difference (BF01=3.58). (See also Supplementary Material for individual fits and additional analyses).
In summary, we used both frequentist and Bayesian statistics and found no evidence that action of the right index finger produces an enhancement of the touch received on the left index finger when the fingers did not make contact. Thus, Experiment 1 does not support the hypothesis of the enhancement model that action enhances predicted touch but supports the hypothesis of the attenuation model.
Experiment 2. Previous ‘enhancement’ effects are driven by the baseline used
Having found no evidence for somatosensory enhancement in Experiment 1, Experiment 2 aimed to understand the potential source of the previously reported enhancement effects47. One critical methodological difference between Experiment 1 and previous evidence for enhancement concerns the baseline condition, as Thomas et al. 47 compared the participants’ perception in the no-contact condition to a condition where the participants prepared their right index movement but received a NOGO cue to inhibit the movement, and it was that comparison that revealed enhancement effects. However, motor inhibition (i.e. planning the hand movement but not executing it) can lead to suppression of somatosensory input both on the hand that is planned to move 71–73 and the hand that would receive touch if the movement was executed 69; moreover, such conditions result in a competition for attentional resources to inhibit the motor response with processes that encode sensory stimuli into memory 74–76 (e.g., the test force in this paradigm). Therefore, if the motor inhibition condition used by Thomas et al. 47 results in a suppression of the perceived touch, a comparison of the no-contact condition with such a baseline would produce an apparent enhancement effect.
Thirty new naïve participants participated in Experiment 2, which included a block of contact trials and a block of no-contact trials in which participants moved their right index finger, but each block also contained randomly intermixed NOGO trials where participants were cued to withhold their movement, identical to the design of Thomas et al. 47 (Figure 2A). Participants were trained to make similar movements with their right index finger in the contact and no-contact trials. An externally generated touch condition was also included as an action-free baseline condition (i.e., no action execution or inhibition). If the previously reported enhancement effects are due to the baseline used, attenuated perception of touch during the motor inhibition condition (i.e., NOGO trials) should be found compared to the baseline, which would then lead to apparent enhancement effects upon comparison with the no-contact trials (Figure 2B, C) (Methods).
This hypothesis (Figure 2B) was confirmed. First, all the effects of Experiment 1 were replicated in the new sample (Figure 2D, E, F & H). The contact trials yielded significant attenuation (i.e., lower PSEs) compared to the baseline condition (t(29) = 6.79, p < 0.001, d = 1.24, CI95 = [0.18 0.33]) (Figure 2E & H). Once again, there was no enhancement in the no-contact trials compared to the baseline condition (t(29) = 0.45, p = 0.658, d = 0.08, CI95 = [−0.05 0.07]) (Figure 2D & F), and the Bayesian analysis again yielded moderate evidence for the absence of any effects (BF01 = 4.69). Importantly, the PSEs in the NOGO (motor inhibition) trials were significantly lower than the baseline condition both for the contact NOGO (t(29) = 2.99, p = 0.006, d =0.55, CI95 = [0.02 0.09]) and the no-contact NOGO trials (t(29) = 4.44, p < 0.001, d = 0.81, CI95 = [0.05 0.13]) (Figure 2D, E & G). This demonstrates that NOGO trials resulted in a suppression of perceived touch on the left hand. Critically, this led to an apparent increase in the PSE from the no-contact trials to the NOGO trials in the no-contact block (t(29) = −2.98, p = 0.006, d = −0.54, CI95 = [−0.12 - 0.02]), mimicking an ‘enhancement’ effect. Finally, PSEs were significantly lower in the contact trials than in the NOGO trials (t(29) = 5.91, p < 0.001, d =1.08, CI95 = [0.13 0.27]), while the NOGO trials in the contact and no-contact blocks did not significantly differ (t(29) = 1.58, p = 0.126, d = 0.29, CI95 =[−0.01, 0.08], BF01 = 1.697). (See also Supplementary Material for individual fits and additional analyses).
In summary, identical to Experiment 1, we used both frequentist and Bayesian statistics and did not find any evidence that action of the right index finger produces an enhancement of the touch received on the left index finger when the fingers do not make contact. Moreover, we showed that the purported “enhancement” effect 47 is driven by a suppression of the perceived intensity of touch following a cue to inhibit the planned movement. Thus, Experiment 2 does not support the hypothesis that action enhances predicted touch.
Experiment 3. Action attenuates predicted touch, even without simultaneous stimulation of the active hand
Within the framework of internal forward models, the absence of attenuation in the no-contact conditions of Experiments 1 and 2 is expected, given the lack of a sensorimotor context conducive to perceiving the touch as self-generated: when contact is never made, the forces applied on the passive left hand are only arbitrarily, and not causally, associated with the movement of the active right hand. Alternatively, it could be argued that the absence of attenuation in the no-contact conditions is caused by the lack of simultaneous tactile stimulation of the active hand rather than by predictive mechanisms.
In Experiment 3, this hypothesis was explicitly tested with thirty new naïve participants. The same contact and no-contact conditions were included as in Experiment 1, but their relative frequency was manipulated within the same block (contact trials: 80%, no-contact trials: 20%) (Figure 3A). The force sensor the participants tapped in the contact trials was now attached to a platform that could be automatically retracted. In the contact trials, participants tapped the force sensor to trigger the test force identically to Experiments 1 and 2, but in the no-contact trials, the platform automatically retracted before trial onset, unbeknownst to the participants, leading them to unexpectedly miss the contact with the sensor but still trigger the test force only by the position of their right index finger. Participants’ vision was occluded so that they could not know whether the next trial would be a contact or a no-contact trial. According to the attenuation model, providing a bimanual sensorimotor context in 80% of the trials should lead participants to form predictions about the somatosensory consequences of their movement in most of the trials and thus attenuate the received touch on their passive left hand compared to the baseline, even if the touch of their active hand was unexpectedly missed (Figure 3B). In contrast, if attenuation is a nonpredictive process caused by the mere simultaneous tactile stimulation of the active finger, no attenuation effects in the no-contact trials should be observed with respect to the baseline (Figure 3C).
The results showed a robust attenuation in both contact and no-contact trials with respect to the baseline condition, regardless of whether contact was made (Figure 3D, E, F & H): the PSEs were significantly lower in the contact condition than in the baseline condition (t(29) = 8.06, p < 0.001, d =1.47, CI95 = [0.16 0.27]) and lower in the no-contact condition than in the baseline condition (t(29) = 4.45, p < 0.001, d = 0.81, CI95 = [0.07 0.20]). The magnitude of attenuation was larger in the contact condition than in the no-contact condition, with significantly lower PSEs in the contact condition than in the no-contact condition (t(29) = 3.94, p < 0.001, d = 0.72, CI95 = [0.04 0.12]) (Figure 3G) (see also Supplementary Material for individual fits and additional analyses). The difference in the magnitude of attenuation was modest (≅ 35%). These findings indicate that the perceived intensity of touch on the passive left hand was significantly attenuated both when the active hand received touch or not, thereby ruling out the possibility that attenuation is merely due to simultaneous tactile stimulation 47,49.
The results of Experiment 3 emphasise the predictive nature of somatosensory attenuation, which is observed only when the sensorimotor context allows the formation of such predictions. In Experiments 1 and 2 attenuation was only observed in the contact conditions, whereas attenuation was also observed in the no-contact condition of Experiment 3, where participants expected that they would make contact (i.e., the sensorimotor context was conducive to predicting self-touch). Thus, larger differences between the PSEs in the contact and no-contact conditions were observed for Experiments 1 and 2 compared to Experiment 3. To further strengthen our interpretation, we performed an ANOVA on the difference in the PSEs between the contact and no-contact conditions across all three experiments. The ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of Experiment (F(2, 87) = 8.05, p<0.001, ηp2 = 0.156), with Bonferroni corrected post hoc comparisons indicating significant differences between Experiments 1 and 3 (t(2, 87) = 3.10, p = 0.008, d = 0.80, CI [0.03 0.23]) as well as Experiments 2 and 3 (t(2, 87) = 3.76, p < 0.001, d = 0.97, CI [0.06 0.26]), but no significant differences between Experiments 1 and 2 (t(2, 87) = −0.66, p = 1.000, d = −0.17, CI [−0.13 0.07], BF01 = 3.308). Thus, no-contact trials elicited attenuated perception only when the sensorimotor context allowed for a prediction of self-touch (Experiment 3) and not when the action was only arbitrarily associated with the touch (Experiments 1 and 2).
Discussion
In the present study, two opposing hypotheses regarding how action influences somatosensory perception were contrasted: the attenuation model and the enhancement model. Our findings demonstrate that action does not enhance (Experiments 1 and 2) but attenuates the predicted touch (Experiment 3). Before discussing the findings, it is important to emphasise that to draw conclusions about whether perception is attenuated or enhanced in an experimental condition including action, it is necessary to include a baseline condition without action. Only comparing conditions that include action prevents differentiating a genuine effect of enhancement (or attenuation) from an effect of reduced attenuation (or enhancement) in one of the two conditions. This also applies to experimental manipulations that contrast predicted with unpredicted somatosensory stimuli during action (see 47 for such comparisons). To this end, a baseline condition of pure somatosensory exafference (i.e., externally generated touch) was included in all the present experiments that allowed us to distinguish the direction of the effects.
The results of Experiment 1 showed a robust attenuation of the touch applied on the passive left hand when the two hands made contact (contact condition), but neither attenuation nor enhancement was caused by the mere movement of the right hand (no-contact condition). In contrast, the perceived intensity of touch in the no-contact condition was similar to that of the baseline (i.e., externally generated touch). These findings are in line with the results of Bays et al. 28, who found no change in the participants’ somatosensory perception when the two hands did not make contact, but do not replicate those of Thomas et al. 47, who observed enhancement of touch in a no-contact condition. Experiment 2 further investigated the source of the previously reported enhancement effects and showed that they are in fact driven by the baseline condition used: enhancement was observed only relative to a condition in which the touch was applied rapidly following a cue to inhibit the movement (i.e., a “do not move” cue), as in previous research 47, but not relative to an externally generated touch condition (our baseline). In support of this claim, Experiment 2 showed that a cue to inhibit the movement results in a significant reduction in the perceived intensity of the imperative stimulus (i.e., touch on the passive hand) compared to baseline perception. This is in line with previous evidence showing reduced amplitude of somatosensory evoked potentials for tactile stimuli presented shortly following a cue to inhibit a movement 73, reduced perceived amplitude for tactile stimuli under the mere expectation to move 71,77, and reduced encoding of sensory stimuli following motor inhibition 74,75,78. Therefore, rather than participants’ perception being enhanced in the no-contact condition, it is a reduction of the perceived intensity following the cue to inhibit the movement that leads to an apparent enhancement. In contrast, by including a novel externally generated touch condition as a baseline that involved neither motor planning nor response inhibition, it became clear that there were no enhancement effects.
Experiment 3 demonstrated that action attenuates the predicted touch even if the active hand does not receive simultaneous tactile stimulation with the passive hand. When participants simply moved their right hand to trigger the touch on their left hand (no-contact trials in Experiments 1 & 2), no change in their somatosensory perception was found. This suggests that an arbitrary mapping between the movement of the right hand and the delivery of touch on the left hand is insufficient to elicit attenuation. In contrast, when participants expected to touch their own hand (Experiment 3), significant attenuation was observed, even when the active hand unexpectedly missed the contact (no-contact trials, 20%). This pattern of results can be explained by the attenuation model, which suggests that self-generation of a stimulus is in itself not sufficient for predictive attenuation, but that the action and it’s predicted sensory feedback are consistent with touching one’s own body 70. Thus, a sensorimotor context that closely resembles tapping directly on the left index finger with the right (self-touch) was critical for predictive attenuation 28. The findings of Experiment 3 also contradict the suggestion that attenuation on a passive hand reflects a nonpredictive gating of tactile input during movement of the active hand 47,49. Indeed, several earlier studies showed gating effects only on the moving limb (movement effector) and not on the passive limb 79–84, and we have also recently shown that experimental paradigms identical to the one used in the present study produce attenuation effects but not gating effects on the passive hand 36. Overall, the results suggest that attenuation effects are driven by action prediction and not the simultaneous tactile stimulation of the two hands. From an ecological point of view, in every self-touch behaviour, we necessarily receive somatosensory input on the active hand and the body part that passively receives touch (“touchant-touché” 85), and it is within these sensorimotor contexts that the brain forms predictions about the somatosensory consequences on multiple body parts 70.
Interestingly, the magnitude of attenuation was greater in the contact compared to the no-contact condition by approximately 35% in Experiment 3. This could suggest that the attenuation effect observed in the contact condition arises from multiple sources, one being the motor-based prediction and another source being the simultaneous touch on the active hand. However, this interpretation is inconsistent with the findings that somatosensory attenuation has been previously observed in the absence of simultaneous tactile stimulation, for example, when imagining but not executing the right hand movement 69 or just before the hands make contact 27. Similarly, no somatosensory attenuation is observed in the absence of action prediction, even if the two hands received simultaneous tactile stimulation; for example, the passive displacement of the right hand towards the left hand that is accompanied by double touch 31 or the mere delivery of simultaneous tactile stimulation on both hands 27 does not produce attenuation. Instead, our finding that the magnitude of attenuation was greater in the contact than the no-contact condition in Experiment 3 may instead reflect a decrease of attenuation in the no-contact trials, due to the unexpected omission of contact influencing the perceived magnitude of touch in a postdictive manner. Specifically, the unexpected omission of contact on the minority of trials (20%) could be seen as a form of stimulus omission akin to so called ‘silent oddballs’ that are known to generate prediction errors 86–89. Although participants clearly attenuated the touch predicted by their movement in the no-contact trials, their expectation of contact was necessarily violated. This violation could be considered a novel event, given its infrequency. Novel events can have several consequences for cognition, including transient enhancements of perception90, facilitated encoding of information into working memory 91 as well as changes in the allocation of attentional resources in a postdictive manner (for a review of effects of novelty on cognition see92).
Some authors have criticised the comparison of perceived intensity of sensory stimuli in self-generated and externally generated conditions 17,93,94. A frequent criticism is that tactile input may be “predicted” in self-generated conditions through action and “unpredicted” in externally generated (passive) conditions that do not involve action. This concern can be ruled out since in all three experiments, the stimulus in the baseline (i.e., externally generated touch) condition was delivered at a fixed timepoint (800 ms after the cue); therefore, participants could predict it in the absence of motor-based predictions. Secondly, it has been argued that somatosensory attenuation findings may be driven by dual-task requirements 17 present only in self-generated conditions that could increase the working memory load or result in a shift of attention towards the active hand. A similar criticism has been suggested in which the mere movement of the active hand in self-generated conditions can result in a nonpredictive gating of tactile input. However, these explanations can also be ruled out, given that dual-task requirements and movement of the active hand were present in the contact and no-contact conditions of all three experiments without concomitant attenuation effects. Indeed, if dual-task requirements, or the mere presence of movement during the perceptual task, was sufficient to cause attenuation effects, then attenuation would have been observed in the no-contact conditions of Experiments 1 and 2. Finally, alternative explanations based on differences in other psychophysical parameters, movement kinematics or timings between experiments can also be ruled out (see Supplementary Material for additional analyses).
How can the findings that action prediction attenuates touch be reconciled with those showing that expectations outside the domain of action improve sensory perception 17? While it is difficult to directly compare these lines of research because of differences in the sensory modality investigated, the task designs used and the perceptual measures employed, we speculate that there are numerous possible reasons why action prediction may not have the same effect on perception as prediction mechanisms outside the domain of action. First, research on action-based predictions concerns ubiquitous associations between actions and their sensory consequences that we are continually exposed to throughout the lifespan. For example, we are constantly exposed to associations between our motor behaviours and their tactile consequences during self-touch, even as early as 13 weeks in utero 95. In contrast, research on sensory expectations outside the domain of action primarily concerns arbitrary associations between stimuli that are typically learned only during the time course of a given task. It is therefore conceivable that separable mechanisms may operate to predict action effects versus stimulus-stimulus associations (see 96,97 for discussion). Second, higher-level expectations, such as explicit prior knowledge that a specific sensory event is likely, might not operate in the same way as lower-level action predictions; for example, it has been proposed that action-based predictions inhibit expected stimuli, while sensory expectations potentiate the expected sensory inputs 98. Most importantly, from a theoretical perspective, attenuating the predicted sensory consequences of actions does not necessarily mean that the brain forms inaccurate representations of the world but instead indicates a flexible strategy that prioritises more informative externally generated events.
It is important to emphasize that this study investigated the perception of self-generated touch during an action directed toward one’s own body. Thus, perception was assessed on a passive body part that was the target of a movement of an active body part (i.e., moving the right hand to touch the static left hand). This focus was chosen because extensive previous research has shown deteriorated performance when detecting or discriminating touch (both self-generated and externally generated) delivered on a moving limb, a phenomenon referred to as movement-related gating 99–103. By assessing somatosensory perception on a static limb, we were able to isolate the effect of action prediction on somatosensory perception from other perceptual phenomena related to the movement per se including movement-related gating 36,99,104–108. However, it is interesting to note that during the active exploration of objects through touch, referred to as active touch, self-generated touch also occurs, albeit only on the moving limb. Studies on active touch have observed improved perception of object features, such as texture or edge orientation when actively exploring an object 109,110 compared to passive exposure 111,112. At first, the improved perception observed during active touch might appear paradoxical to observations of movement-related gating and predictive attenuation effects: if anything, active touch should result in worse perception than passive touch. However, it has been shown that humans optimize their exploratory movements to minimize these effects and maximize the quality of their sensory feedback. For example, it is known that the degree of gating decreases as movement speed slows 101,113,114 and it has been shown that participants optimize the speed of movement during active touch to avoid high speeds associated with tactile gating 115. Similarly, when exploring objects through active touch, weaker forces are typically applied (e.g. < 1 N 116,117) that elicit less attenuation than stronger forces, as shown in the classic force-matching task 118. Thus, differences in the task context might primarily influence how one moves (e.g., the act of swatting a fly from one’s arm versus feeling for a bump on one’s skin), which can have disparate ensuing effects on perception depending on the movement kinematics.
Debates between researchers supporting attenuation or enhancement are useful and fruitful for scientific dialogue and advancement. The present study revisited recent findings on somatosensory enhancement during action and showed that when the requirement to inhibit the action is removed from the baseline, action predictions do not enhance but attenuate the received somatosensory input. Our results are in strong alignment with animal studies showing that action attenuates the predicted sensory consequences (for reviews, see 119–123). For example, crickets and mice suppress auditory reafferent signals but maintain their sensitivity to external sounds 124–127, the weakly electric fish attenuates its electrosensory reafference to respond to externally generated electrical signals 128,129, and primates attenuate their vestibular reafference and activate vestibular-related reflexes only when the vestibular input is exafferent 130–132. To this end, the results of the present study prompt a reappraisal of recent experimental findings upon which theoretical frameworks proposing a perceptual enhancement by action prediction are based.
Methods
Participants
Thirty naive adults participated in Experiment 1 (18 female, aged 18-36, 28 right-handed and 2 ambidextrous), thirty naive adults participated in Experiment 2 (17 female, aged 20-37, 29 right-handed and 1 left-handed) and thirty naive adults participated in Experiment 3 (12 female, aged 21-40, 29 right-handed, 1 ambidextrous, 1 left-handed). Current or history of psychological or neurological conditions, as well as the use of any psychoactive drugs or medication to treat such conditions, were criteria for exclusion. The sample size of each Experiment was decided prior to data collection based on our previous studies using the same task 30,36. Handedness was assessed using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory133. All experiments were approved by the Swedish Ethical Review Authority (registration no. 2021-03790). Participants provided written informed consent and were compensated for their time.
Pre-registration of experiments
The method and analysis plan for each experiment was pre-registered on the Open Science Framework (OSF) prior to data collection (Experiment 1 https://osf.io/9jkqt, Experiment 2 https://osf.io/hs8au, Experiment 3 https://osf.io/gkwu7).
Modifications/additions to the pre-registered plan
The only modification from the pre-registered plan was a reduction in the number of trials administered in Experiment 3. Following initial piloting, it was deemed appropriate to reduce the overall length of the task by including fewer trials (see Methods Details). Two additional ANOVAs were also conducted to compare the results across the three experiments (Main Text) and the kinematics across experiments (Supplementary Materials).
Experimental setup
Participants sat comfortably on a chair with their arms placed on a table. The left hand rested palm up, with the index finger placed on a molded support. On each trial, a motor (Maxon EC Motor EC 90 flat; Switzerland) delivered two forces (the test force and the comparison force) on the pulp of the left index finger through a cylindrical probe (25 mm height) with a flat aluminum surface (20 mm diameter) attached to a lever on the motor. A force sensor (FSG15N1A, Honeywell Inc.; diameter, 5 mm; minimum resolution, 0.01 N; response time, 1 ms; measurement range, 0–15 N) within the probe recorded the forces applied on the left index finger. Following the presentation of the two forces, participants were asked to verbally report which of the two forces felt stronger – the first or the second. A second identical force sensor within an identical cylindrical probe (“active force sensor”) was placed on top of, but not in contact with, the probe of the left index finger.
Force-discrimination task
Participants judged the intensity of a test force and a comparison force (100 ms duration each) separated by a random interval between 800ms and 1200ms in a two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) task. The intensity of the test force was 2 N, while the intensity of the comparison force was systematically varied among seven force levels (1, 1.5, 1.75, 2, 2.25, 2.5 or 3 N). In all the conditions, the forces were delivered by the same motor, to precisely control their magnitude, however the source of the force was manipulated across conditions such that the force was triggered by the participants contact with a force sensor (contact condition), their finger movement (no-contact condition) or automatically by the stimulus computer (baseline condition).
Experimental design and procedures
Experiment 1
There were three conditions in Experiment 1 presented in three blocks separately: the baseline (i.e. externally generated touch) condition, the contact condition, and the no-contact condition.
In the baseline condition, participants did not move their limbs but passively received a test and the comparison force on the left index finger. This baseline condition was used to assess the participants’ somatosensory perception in the absence of any movement 27,30,31. Each trial began with an auditory cue (100 ms duration, 997 Hz) followed by the test force delivered to the participant’s left index finger 800 ms after the cue by the motor. The comparison force was then delivered at a random interval between 800 ms and 1200 ms after the test force.
In the contact condition, participants started each trial by holding their right index finger approximately 10 cm above their left index finger. The start position was marked with a visual marker placed next to their right index finger while the final position was the probe of the active force sensor. The same auditory cue was presented as in the baseline condition, but participants now moved their right index finger downwards towards their left index finger and actively tapped on the active force sensor placed on top of, but not in contact with, the probe. The participant’s active tap on the force sensor triggered the motor to apply the test force on their left index finger (threshold 0.2 N). The tap of their right index finger triggered the test force on their left index finger with an intrinsic delay of 36 ms. The comparison force was then delivered at a random interval between 800ms and 1200ms after the test force. As in our previous studies36,41,134, participants were asked to tap, neither too weakly nor too strongly, with their right index finger, “as if tapping the screen of their smartphone”. This instruction was provided to ensure that the relationship between the force they applied with their right index finger on the force sensor and the force they received on their left index finger by the motor (2 N) remained approximately constant throughout the experiment, thereby establishing perceived causality15,135. Indeed, it has been previously shown that the magnitude of attenuation remains unaffected by halving or doubling the gain between the force applied by the active finger and the force delivered on the passive hand15. This should not be surprising given that when one finger transmits a force through an object to another finger, the resulting force depends also on the object properties (e.g., shape, material, contact area) and the angle at which the finger contacts the object.
In the no-contact condition, the participants started each trial by holding their right index finger 10 cm above their left index finger, identically to the contact condition. For this block, the active force sensor was removed and replaced by a distance sensor that detected the position of the right index finger. The distance sensor was placed on top of, but not in contact with, the probe. Following the same auditory cue, participants moved their right index finger towards their left index finger. To restrict the participants’ right index finger movement within similar movement ranges as in the contact condition and avoid contact with the distance sensor placed underneath, a second visual marker indicated the final position of the right index finger above the distance sensor (5 cm from the marker indicating the initial position). The distance sensor was connected to an Arduino microcontroller that controlled a servo motor. The servo motor and the force sensor were placed 2 meters away from the participants’ hands and hidden from view. Once the distance sensor detected that the position of the right index finger became smaller than a preset threshold, it triggered the servo motor that hit the force sensor and triggered the test force. We accounted for the additional delay of the distance sensor by setting the position threshold for the distance sensor slightly higher than the lowest position the participants were asked to reach with their right index finger. Therefore, the test force would be delivered at a similar timing to the participants’ right index finger endpoint between the contact and no-contact conditions. This position threshold was set based on significant pilot testing prior all experiments. Indeed, there was a minimal time difference between the two setups across experiments (17 ms average delay) with the no-contact condition leading, rather than lagging the contact one (see Supplementary Materials). The comparison force was delivered at a random interval between 800ms and 1200ms after the test force. Therefore, in the no-contact condition, there was no touch on the right index finger simultaneously with the test force on the left index finger.
Before the experiment, participants were trained to make similar movements with their right index finger in the contact and no-contact conditions, emphasis was placed on restricting their right index finger movements to between the two visual markers in the no-contact condition. The 3D position of the right index finger was recorded using a Polhemus Liberty tracker (240 Hz). Kinematic information was used to compare the movements between the contact and no-contact conditions and to reject any trials in which the participant did not trigger the test stimulus with their movement, or trials in which the participant did not move as instructed in the training (see below). Participants were administered white noise through headphones to mask any sounds made by the motor to serve as a cue for the task. The loudness of the white noise was adjusted such that participants could hear the auditory tones of the trial.
Each block consisted of 70 trials resulting to 210 trials per participant. Thus, the proportion of contact and no-contact trials was the same (50-50%). The order of the conditions was fully counterbalanced across participants.
Experiment 2
The 2-AFC task was identical to that of Experiment 1. In Experiment 2, there were five conditions in total: the baseline condition, the contact condition, the no-contact condition, and two additional NOGO conditions (NOGO contact and NOGO no-contact). Trials of the NOGO conditions were pseudo-randomly intermixed with trials of the contact and no-contact conditions respectively (GO trials). The five conditions were presented in three separate blocks: the baseline, contact (GO and NOGO trials), and no-contact (GO and NOGO trials) blocks.
In the contact block, 50% of trials began with an auditory “GO” cue (100 ms duration high tone of 2458 Hz) instructing participants to tap the active force sensor to trigger the test force (identically to the contact condition of Experiment 1). On the remaining 50% of trials an auditory “NOGO” cue (100ms duration low tone of 222 Hz) instructed participants to withhold their movement and the test force was then delivered 800ms following the NOGO cue. The no-contact block was identical to the contact block, except that the test force was triggered by the position of the right index finger without contact with the force sensor (identically to the no-contact condition of Experiment 1). The cue tone in the baseline block was the same as the cue tone in the NOGO trials (100 ms duration low tone of 222 Hz).
Therefore, this design replicated the experimental design of Thomas et al. with the additional inclusion of the baseline (i.e. externally generated touch) condition. As in Experiment 1, we recorded the 3D position of the right index finger, and the registered kinematic information was used to compare the movements between the contact and no-contact conditions and reject any trials in which the participant did not trigger the test stimulus with their movement, trials in which the participant did not move as instructed in the training (see below), and trials in which the participants moved while instructed not to do so by the auditory cues (NOGO trials). As in Experiment 1, participants were administered white noise and the loudness of the white noise was adjusted such that participants could clearly hear the GO and NOGO auditory cues of the trial.
The baseline block consisted of 70 trials, and the contact and no-contact blocks consisted of 70 GO trials and 70 NOGO trials each, resulting in 350 trials in total per participant. The order of the blocks was fully counterbalanced across participants.
Experiment 3
The 2-AFC task and the experimental conditions (baseline, contact, and no-contact) were identical to those of Experiment 1, except for the following. Contact trials (80%) were now pseudo-randomly intermixed with no-contact trials (20%) within the same block. The force sensor was now attached to a plastic platform that could be automatically retracted by a servo motor, depending on the trial type. Upon retraction, a distance sensor placed underneath the platform was revealed. In the contact trials, participants tapped the force sensor to trigger the test force identically to Experiments 1 and 2. In the no-contact trials, the platform was automatically retracted before trial onset, unbeknownst to the participant. This led participants to unexpectedly miss the active force sensor and instead trigger the test force only by the position of their right index finger. In all conditions, the participants’ vision was occluded, and white noise was administered via headphones to prevent any visual or auditory cues indicating that the force sensor had been retracted in no-contact trials. The baseline block was identical to that of Experiments 1and 2.
In Experiment 3, the number of trials was reduced from 70 to 56 to shorten the total experiment time to less than 90 minutes, similar to the Experiment 2. Thus, there were 56 no-contact trials (20%) and 224 contact trials (80%). Similarly, there were 56 trials in the baseline condition. This resulted to 336 trials per participant. The order of the two blocks was fully counterbalanced across participants.
Preprocessing of psychophysical trials
Experiment 1
Experiment 1 included 6300 trials in total (30 participants * 70 trials * 3 conditions). Twenty-nine (29) trials were excluded (0.5%) because of 1 missing response, 4 trials in which the force was not applied correctly (1.85 N < test force < 2.15 N), and 24 trials because the test force was not triggered when moving towards the distance sensor. The kinematic analysis also excluded all trials excluded by the psychophysical fits.
Experiment 2
Experiment 2 included 10500 trials in total (30 participants * 70 trials * 5 conditions). One-hundred fourteen (114) trials were excluded (1.1%) because of 2 missing responses, 6 trials in which the force was not applied correctly (1.85 N < test force < 2.15 N), 44 trials because the test force was not triggered when moving towards the distance sensor and 62 trials because the finger moved on a NOGO trial. All trials excluded by the psychophysical fits were also excluded in the kinematic analysis.
Experiment 3
Experiment 3 included 10080 trials in total (30 participants * 224 contact trials + 56 no-contact trials + 56 baseline trials). Two-hundred twenty-four (29) trials were excluded (2.42%) because of 29 missing responses, 86 trials in which the force was not applied correctly (1.85 N < test force < 2.15 N), 81 because the test force was not triggered when moving towards the distance sensor and 48 because the finger contacted the distance sensor on no-contact trials. The kinematic analysis also excluded all trials excluded by the psychophysical fits.
Preprocessing of kinematic recordings
All kinematic trials were co-registered with the force trials through Transistor-Transistor Logic (TTL) signals sent by the motor to both file outputs. The kinematic recordings were corrected for any distortion in the Polhemus sensor from the force sensor and distance sensor based on measurements made with and without the force/distance sensors within the same space. Exclusion of trials based on the kinematics was done by assessing whether the test force was delivered after the position of the active finger reached its minimum value on the vertical plane. Trials in which the force was delivered after the minimum value had been reached were rejected. For experiment 2, trials in which the active finger moved more than 1 cm following a NOGO cue were also rejected.
Fitting of psychophysical responses
For each experiment and each condition, the participant’s responses were fitted with a generalized linear model using a logit link function (Equation 1): We extracted two parameters of interest: the Point of Subjective Equality (PSE) , which represents the intensity at which the test force felt as strong as the comparison force (p = 0.5) and quantifies the perceived intensity, and the JND , which reflects the participants’ discrimination ability. Before fitting the responses, the values of the applied comparison forces were binned to the closest value with respect to their theoretical values (1, 1.5, 1.75, 2, 2.25, 2.5 or 3 N).
For all participants and all conditions, the fitted logistic models were very good, with McFadden’s R squared measures ranging between 0.602 and 0.979.
Normality of data and statistical comparisons
We used R 136, JASP 137 and MATLAB (2020b) to analyze our data. The normality of the PSE and the JND data distributions, as well as the kinematic information data distributions were assessed using Shapiro-Wilk tests. Depending on the data normality, pairwise comparisons between conditions were performed by using either a paired t-test or a Wilcoxon signed-rank test. We report 95% confidence intervals (CI95) for each statistical test. Effect sizes are reported as the Cohen’s d for t-tests or the matched rank biserial correlation rrb for the Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. In addition, a Bayesian factor analysis using default Cauchy priors with a scale of 0.707 was performed for all statistical tests that led to not statistically significant effects, to provide information about the level of support for the null hypothesis compared to the alternative hypothesis (BF01) based on the data. We interpret a factor between 1/3 and 3 as “anecdotal evidence”138,139, indicating that support for either the preferred or null hypotheses is insufficient. For the Analysis of Variance (ANOVA, see Supplementary Material), homogeneity of variance was assessed using Levene’s test for Equality of Variances, which did not reach significance, and the Q-Q plot of the standardized residuals indicated approximately normally distributed residuals. Post-hoc tests were made using Bonferroni corrections. All tests were two-tailed.
Author Contributions
K.K. and X.J. designed the experiments; X.J. collected the data; X.J. analysed the data; X.J. and K.K. wrote the manuscript.
Declaration of interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Data availability
Data (PSEs and JNDs) are publicly available along with preregistered analyses plans on the OSF (Experiment 1: https://osf.io/9jkqt, Experiment 2: https://osf.io/hs8au & Experiment 3: https://osf.io/gk).
Code availability
This paper does not report original code. Any additional information is available from the lead contact upon request.
Acknowledgments
We thank Evridiki Asimakidou and Lili Timar for their assistance during data collection. We also thank Henrik Ehrsson for his helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. K.K. was supported by the Swedish Research Council (VR Starting Grant 2019-01909). X.J. was supported by the Swedish Research Council (VR Starting Grant 2019-01909) and the European Union’s Horizon 2021 research and innovation programme (Marie Skłodowska-Curie 101059348). Experimental costs were covered by the Swedish Research Council (VR Starting Grant 2019-01909).
Footnotes
Text was added to the Discussion.
References
- 1.↵
- 2.
- 3.↵
- 4.↵
- 5.↵
- 6.
- 7.↵
- 8.↵
- 9.
- 10.↵
- 11.↵
- 12.↵
- 13.↵
- 14.↵
- 15.↵
- 16.↵
- 17.↵
- 18.
- 19.↵
- 20.↵
- 21.↵
- 22.↵
- 23.↵
- 24.↵
- 25.↵
- 26.↵
- 27.↵
- 28.↵
- 29.
- 30.↵
- 31.↵
- 32.
- 33.
- 34.
- 35.
- 36.↵
- 37.
- 38.
- 39.
- 40.
- 41.↵
- 42.↵
- 43.
- 44.↵
- 45.
- 46.↵
- 47.↵
- 48.↵
- 49.↵
- 50.↵
- 51.↵
- 52.↵
- 53.
- 54.
- 55.
- 56.
- 57.
- 58.↵
- 59.↵
- 60.↵
- 61.↵
- 62.↵
- 63.
- 64.↵
- 65.↵
- 66.
- 67.
- 68.
- 69.↵
- 70.↵
- 71.↵
- 72.
- 73.↵
- 74.↵
- 75.↵
- 76.↵
- 77.↵
- 78.↵
- 79.↵
- 80.
- 81.
- 82.
- 83.
- 84.↵
- 85.↵
- 86.↵
- 87.
- 88.
- 89.↵
- 90.↵
- 91.↵
- 92.↵
- 93.↵
- 94.↵
- 95.↵
- 96.↵
- 97.↵
- 98.↵
- 99.↵
- 100.
- 101.↵
- 102.
- 103.↵
- 104.↵
- 105.
- 106.
- 107.
- 108.↵
- 109.↵
- 110.↵
- 111.↵
- 112.↵
- 113.↵
- 114.↵
- 115.↵
- 116.↵
- 117.↵
- 118.↵
- 119.↵
- 120.
- 121.
- 122.
- 123.↵
- 124.↵
- 125.
- 126.
- 127.↵
- 128.↵
- 129.↵
- 130.↵
- 131.
- 132.↵
- 133.↵
- 134.↵
- 135.↵
- 136.↵
- 137.↵
- 138.↵
- 139.↵