Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

Genome-wide gene-environment analyses of major depressive disorder and reported lifetime traumatic experiences in UK Biobank

View ORCID ProfileJonathan R.I. Coleman, Wouter J. Peyrot, View ORCID ProfileKirstin L. Purves, View ORCID ProfileKatrina A.S. Davis, Christopher Rayner, View ORCID ProfileShing Wan Choi, View ORCID ProfileChristopher Hübel, View ORCID ProfileHéléna A. Gaspar, View ORCID ProfileCarol Kan, View ORCID ProfileSandra Van der Auwera, View ORCID ProfileMark James Adams, View ORCID ProfileDonald M. Lyall, Karmel W. Choi, Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, View ORCID ProfileErin C. Dunn, View ORCID ProfileEvangelos Vassos, View ORCID ProfileAndrea Danese, Barbara Maughan, View ORCID ProfileHans J. Grabe, View ORCID ProfileCathryn M. Lewis, View ORCID ProfilePaul F. O’Reilly, View ORCID ProfileAndrew M. McIntosh, View ORCID ProfileDaniel J. Smith, View ORCID ProfileNaomi R. Wray, View ORCID ProfileMatthew Hotopf, View ORCID ProfileThalia C. Eley, View ORCID ProfileGerome Breen
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/247353
Jonathan R.I. Coleman
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
2NIHR Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre, South London and Maudsley NHS Trust, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Jonathan R.I. Coleman
Wouter J. Peyrot
3Amsterdam UMC, Vrije Universiteit Medical Center, Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kirstin L. Purves
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Kirstin L. Purves
Katrina A.S. Davis
2NIHR Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre, South London and Maudsley NHS Trust, London, UK
4Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Katrina A.S. Davis
Christopher Rayner
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Shing Wan Choi
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Shing Wan Choi
Christopher Hübel
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
2NIHR Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre, South London and Maudsley NHS Trust, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Christopher Hübel
Héléna A. Gaspar
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
2NIHR Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre, South London and Maudsley NHS Trust, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Héléna A. Gaspar
Carol Kan
4Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Carol Kan
Sandra Van der Auwera
5Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medicine Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Sandra Van der Auwera
Mark James Adams
6Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Mark James Adams
Donald M. Lyall
7Institute of Health and Wellbeing, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Donald M. Lyall
Karmel W. Choi
8Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
9Department of Epidemiology, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, MA, USA
10Stanley Center for Psychiatric Research, The Broad Institute of Harvard and MIT, Cambridge, MA, USA
11Psychiatric and Neurodevelopmental Genetics Unit, Center for Genomic Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
12Consortium members listed in Supplementary Materials
Erin C. Dunn
10Stanley Center for Psychiatric Research, The Broad Institute of Harvard and MIT, Cambridge, MA, USA
11Psychiatric and Neurodevelopmental Genetics Unit, Center for Genomic Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
13Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Erin C. Dunn
Evangelos Vassos
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
2NIHR Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre, South London and Maudsley NHS Trust, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Evangelos Vassos
Andrea Danese
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
14Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
15National and Specialist CAMHS Trauma and Anxiety Clinic, South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Andrea Danese
Barbara Maughan
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hans J. Grabe
5Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medicine Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Hans J. Grabe
Cathryn M. Lewis
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
2NIHR Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre, South London and Maudsley NHS Trust, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Cathryn M. Lewis
Paul F. O’Reilly
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Paul F. O’Reilly
Andrew M. McIntosh
6Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Andrew M. McIntosh
Daniel J. Smith
7Institute of Health and Wellbeing, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Daniel J. Smith
Naomi R. Wray
16Institute for Molecular Bioscience, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
17Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Naomi R. Wray
Matthew Hotopf
2NIHR Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre, South London and Maudsley NHS Trust, London, UK
4Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Matthew Hotopf
Thalia C. Eley
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
2NIHR Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre, South London and Maudsley NHS Trust, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Thalia C. Eley
  • For correspondence: gerome.breen@kcl.ac.uk thalia.eley@kcl.ac.uk
Gerome Breen
1Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
2NIHR Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre, South London and Maudsley NHS Trust, London, UK
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Gerome Breen
  • For correspondence: gerome.breen@kcl.ac.uk thalia.eley@kcl.ac.uk
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Supplementary material
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

Depression is more frequent among individuals exposed to traumatic events. Both trauma exposure and depression are heritable. However, the relationship between these traits, including the role of genetic risk factors, is complex and poorly understood. When modelling trauma exposure as an environmental influence on depression, both gene-environment correlations and gene-environment interactions have been observed. The UK Biobank concurrently assessed Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) and self-reported lifetime exposure to traumatic events in 126,522 genotyped individuals of European ancestry. We contrasted genetic influences on MDD between individuals reporting and not reporting trauma exposure (final sample size range: 24,094-92,957). The SNP-based heritability of MDD was greater in participants reporting trauma exposure (24%) than in individuals not reporting trauma exposure (12%), taking into account the strong, positive genetic correlation observed between MDD and reported trauma exposure. The genetic correlation between MDD and waist circumference was only significant in individuals reporting trauma exposure (rg = 0.24, p = 1.8×10-7 versus rg = −0.05, p = 0.39 in individuals not reporting trauma exposure, difference p = 2.3×10-4). Our results suggest that the genetic contribution to MDD is greater when additional risk factors are present, and that a complex relationship exists between reported trauma exposure, body composition, and MDD.

Introduction

Depression is among the most common mental illnesses worldwide and accounts for 5.5% of all years lost through disability globally 1. In England approximately 28% of individuals self-report depression during their lifetime 2. The most common clinically recognised form of depression is called Major Depressive Disorder (MDD). Both environmental and genetic factors influence MDD. In particular, MDD is more commonly observed among individuals reporting exposure to stressful life events and early-life traumas 3–6. In turn, reported trauma exposure has been robustly correlated with a range of adverse life outcomes including MDD 6–9. The relationship between MDD and reported trauma exposure is complex, with studies showing both that reported trauma exposure is associated with subsequent MDD, and that MDD is associated with subsequent reported trauma exposure 10, 11. However, the majority of people reporting exposure to traumatic experiences do not report MDD 6–9.

Twin studies show that MDD is moderately heritable, with 30-40% of the variance in MDD attributable to genetic factors 12. The proportion of heritability captured by common genetic variants, also known as single nucleotide polymorphism or SNP-based heritability, can be estimated from genome-wide association study (GWAS) data. Such estimates tend to be lower than those obtained from twin approaches, due to the incomplete capture of genetic information in GWAS data among other reasons 13. The most recent major depression GWAS from the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium was anchored in 35 cohorts (including the 23andMe discovery cohort 14) recruited with a variety of methods 15. This meta-analysis identified 44 loci significantly associated with major depression, and estimated a SNP-based heritability of 9-10% 15. GWAS results strongly suggest both the mild and more severe forms of depression are polygenic, with potentially thousands of variants with very small individual effects contributing to risk.

There are far fewer genetic studies of reported trauma exposure than of MDD. However, the available studies have demonstrated that reported trauma exposure is heritable, with twin heritability estimates of 20-50% 16–18 and SNP-based heritability estimates of 30% 19. Combining measures of trauma exposure and depression on a large scale is difficult and, as with many environmental measures, requires careful phenotyping 20. Potential confounds include the (often unavoidable) use of retrospective self-reported measures of trauma exposure, which can be weakly correlated with objective measures of traumatic experiences 9. Furthermore, current (i.e. state) low mood or MDD can increase self-reporting of previous trauma exposure 9, 21. Previous individual study cohorts have generally been too small for effective GWAS, while meta-analyses have contained considerable heterogeneity due to the use of different phenotyping instruments in the included studies.

However, some notable genome-wide analyses of MDD and trauma exposure have been performed. A genome-wide by environment interaction study of depressive symptoms and stressful life events in 7,179 African American women identified a genome-wide association near the CEP350 gene (although this did not replicate in a smaller cohort) 22. A recent investigation in 9,599 Han Chinese women with severe MDD identified three variants associated with MDD specifically in individuals who did not report exposure to traumatic events prior to MDD onset 23.

Several attempts have been made to estimate the interaction of overall genetic risk and trauma by using polygenic risk scores for MDD to perform polygenic risk score-by-trauma interaction analyses. Such studies test whether there are departures from additivity (where the combined effect of risk score and trauma differs from the sum of the individual effects) or from multiplicativity (where the combined effect differs from the product of the individual effects). Results from these have been highly variable, with reports of significant additive and multiplicative interactions 24; significant multiplicative interactions only 25; and, in the largest previous study published (a meta-analysis of 5,765 individuals), no interactions 26.

Studies of gene-environment interaction usually assume the genetic and environmental influences are independent and uncorrelated 27. However, genetic correlations between reported trauma exposure and MDD have been reported, both from twin studies 28–30 and from the genomic literature 22, 26. Reports of the magnitude of this genetic correlation have varied widely, which reflects differences in defining trauma exposure, and in the populations studied. While some studies have identified a very high genetic correlation (95%) 22, others have found no such correlation 23. The relationship between reported trauma exposure and MDD, and the influence of genetic variation on that relationship, is therefore complex and unresolved.

The release of mental health questionnaire data from the UK Biobank resource provides an opportunity to assess the relationship between genetic variation, risk for MDD, and reported trauma exposure in a single large cohort. We performed GWAS of probable MDD with and without reported lifetime trauma exposure in UK Biobank European ancestry individuals. These GWAS were underpowered to detect individual genetic loci at the very low effect sizes seen in previous GWAS of MDD. However, these results enabled us to estimate the SNP-based heritability of MDD in individuals with and without reported lifetime trauma exposure, and so to test whether the relative contribution of genetic influences to MDD differs in the context of reported trauma exposure. We then estimated the genetic correlation between MDD in the two groups, to assess whether the genetic component differed between individuals reporting and not reporting trauma exposure. In addition, we compared the patterns of genetic correlation between MDD in these groups and a wide range of physical and psychiatric traits, in order to assess whether the genetic relationship of MDD to other traits varies in the context of reported trauma exposure. Finally, we performed polygenic risk scoring, using external traits commonly comorbid with MDD, and sought to extend previous analyses of PRS-by-trauma interactions in MDD.

Methods

Phenotype definitions

The UK Biobank assessed a range of health-related phenotypes and biological measures including genome-wide genotype data in approximately 500,000 British individuals aged between 40 and 70 31. This includes 157,366 participants who completed an online follow-up questionnaire assessing common mental health disorders, including MDD symptoms, and 16 items assessing traumatic events (Resource 22 on http://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk) 32. Phenotypes were derived from this questionnaire, using definitions from a recent publication describing its phenotypic structure 32.

Individuals with probable MDD met lifetime criteria based on their responses to questions derived from the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI; Supplementary Table 1). We excluded cases if they self-reported previous diagnoses of schizophrenia, other psychoses, or bipolar disorder. Controls were excluded if they self-reported any mental illness, reported taking any drug with an antidepressant indication, had previously been hospitalised with a mood disorder or met previously-defined criteria for a mood disorder (Supplementary Table 1) 33.

Participants were asked questions relating to traumatic experiences in childhood using the Childhood Trauma Screener (a shortened version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire 34–36) and an equivalent screener for adulthood developed by the UK Biobank Mental Health steering group to mirror the childhood items 32. In addition, participants were asked questions related to events that commonly trigger post-traumatic stress-disorder (PTSD). Responses to individual questions (items) in these three categories (child trauma, adult trauma, PTSD-relevant trauma) were dichotomised and compared between MDD cases and controls (Supplementary Table 2a).

We selected reported items with an odds ratio > 2.5 with MDD, to obtain a single binary variable for stratification that captured exposure to the traumatic events most associated with MDD. Items from all three trauma categories were reported more in MDD cases compared to controls. Of the selected items, three referred to events in childhood (did not feel loved, felt hated by a family member, sexually abused). Another three items referred to events in adulthood (physical violence, belittlement, sexual interference), and one item assessed a PTSD-relevant event (ever a victim of sexual assault). In order to capture increased severity of exposure, only individuals reporting two or more of these items were included as reporting trauma exposure. Individuals reporting none of the items were included as not reporting trauma exposure. Individuals reporting a single trauma item, or who did not provide an answer were excluded from the analyses (Supplementary Table 1). A breakdown of reported traumatic experiences by sex and MDD status is provided in Supplementary Table 2b. Further discussion of the definition of trauma exposure is included in the Supplementary Note.

Phenotype preparation for analyses

Three sets of analyses comparing MDD cases and controls were performed (i) overall, (ii) limited to individuals reporting trauma exposure, and (iii) limited to individuals not reporting trauma exposure (Table 1). In addition, sensitivity analyses were performed on reported trauma exposure (overall and stratified by MDD diagnosis; see Supplementary Methods and Results, and Supplementary Table 3). For each analysis, phenotypes were first residualised on 6 ancestry principal components from the genetic data of the European samples as well as factors capturing initial assessment centre and genotyping batch. More details on phenotype preparation can be found in the Supplementary Methods.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1: Participants available for analysis.

Groups of individuals used in each of the three analyses are in bold.

The superscripts denote the groups used in each of the three main analyses:

a) MDD in all participants (29,475 cases, 63,482 controls, N = 92,957);

b) MDD in participants reporting trauma exposure (13,393 cases, 10,701 controls, N = 24,094);

c) MDD in participants not reporting trauma exposure (9,487 cases, 39,677 controls, N = 49,164).

Phenotype distribution

Previous analyses have shown that, compared to the participants in the UK Biobank as a whole, those who completed the mental health questionnaire were more likely to have a university degree, came from a higher socioeconomic background, and reported fewer long-standing illnesses or disabilities 32. Accordingly, participants were compared across a number of standard demographic variables and common correlates of MDD: sex, age (at questionnaire), education (university degree vs. not), neighbourhood socioeconomic status (SES, as Townsend deprivation index 37) and BMI (recorded from measurements taken at the initial recruitment of the participants into the biobank). For further details on these analyses, see Supplementary Methods.

Genetic data

Genetic data for GWAS analyses came from the full release of the UK Biobank data (N=487,410; 38). Autosomal genotype data from two highly-overlapping custom genotyping arrays (covering ∼800,000 markers) underwent centralised quality control before being imputed in a two-stage imputation to the Haplotype Reference Consortium (HRC) and UK10K (for rarer variants not present in the HRC) reference panels 38–40. In addition to this central quality control, variants for analysis were limited to common variants (minor allele frequency > 0.01) that were either directly genotyped or imputed from the HRC with high confidence (IMPUTE INFO metric > 0.4) 39.

Individuals were excluded where recommended by the UK Biobank core analysis team for unusual levels of missingness or heterozygosity, or if they had withdrawn consent for analysis. Using the genotyped SNPs, individuals with call rate < 98%, who were related to another individual in the dataset (KING r < 0.044, equivalent to removing up third-degree relatives and closer 41) or whose phenotypic and genotypic gender information was discordant (X-chromosome homozygosity (FX) < 0.9 for phenotypic males, FX > 0.5 for phenotypic females) were also excluded. Removal of relatives was performed using a “greedy” algorithm, which minimises exclusions (for example, by excluding the child in a mother-father-child trio). All analyses were limited to individuals of European ancestry, as defined by 4-means clustering on the first two genetic principal components provided by the UK Biobank 42. This ancestry group included 95% of the respondents to the mental health questionnaire - as such, the non-European ancestry groups were considered too small to analyse informatively. Principal components analysis was also performed on the European-only subset of the data using the software flashpca2 43. After quality control, individuals with high-quality genotype data and who had completed the online mental health questionnaire were retained for analysis (N=126,522).

GWAS analyses used the imputed data as described above. Genetic correlation analyses used the results of the GWAS analyses. Polygenic risk score analyses and SNP-based heritability analyses in BOLT-LMM used the genotyped variants 38. These latter analyses were limited to common variants (minor allele frequency > 0.01) with call rate >98% that were in approximate Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (HWE test p > 10-8). The same individuals were used for analyses using the imputed and the genotyped data.

Analyses

Genome Wide Association Studies (GWAS)

GWAS were performed to assess the association of individual genetic variants with MDD. These analyses were first undertaken for the entire sample regardless of reported trauma exposure, then stratified by reported trauma exposure. GWAS were performed using linear regressions on imputed genotype dosages in BGenie v1.2 38, with residualised phenotypes as described above. Phenotypes and genotypes were mean-centred and standardised. Genome-wide significance was defined at the conventional level p < 5 x 10-8 44. Results from each GWAS were clumped to define genetic loci in PLINK2 45. Loci were defined following established protocols (Supplementary Methods) 15.

Betas from the GWAS were converted to odds ratios (OR) using LMOR (http://cnsgenomics.com/shiny/LMOR/) and observed sample prevalences 46. Standard errors were calculated from the p-value and estimated OR 47. Performing GWAS on residuals, rather than including covariates in the analysis, is a restriction imposed by the BGenie software (which was used because it is specifically designed for analysing the UK Biobank genetic data). Sensitivity analyses were performed to test for biases resulting from this method. Specifically, for each GWAS, each variant with nominal significance (p<0.0001) was also tested using logistic regression including covariates in R 3.4.1, in order to confirm the results from BGenie 48.

SNP-based heritability

Results from GWAS were combined to assess the proportion of variance due to the additive effect of common genetic variants (SNP-based heritability). SNP-based heritability was calculated on the observed scale using BOLT-LMM v2.3 49. The estimate for MDD in the cohort was converted to the liability scale in R 3.4.1, assuming a population prevalence of 28% 2, 50. Converting estimates of SNP-based heritability for a case-control trait from the observed scale to the liability scale requires accurate estimates of the lifetime prevalence of the trait in the (sub)population. When comparing a trait stratified by a correlated variable (as is the case when we compare the SNP-based heritability of MDD stratified by reported trauma exposure), the population prevalence in each stratum is unknown. To address this, we approximated the expected prevalence of MDD in individuals either reporting or not reporting trauma exposure (Supplementary Methods). This allowed us to convert the observed scale SNP-based heritability of MDD to the liability scale in both strata (i.e. those reporting and those not reporting trauma exposure). A second challenge is that trauma exposure is itself a heritable trait that is genetically correlated with MDD in this study. The potential impact of this on SNP-based heritability estimation is not intuitive. To benchmark our findings, we performed simulations of SNP-level data to explore the expected SNP-based heritability of MDD in individuals reporting and not reporting trauma exposure, assuming differences in SNP-based heritability resulted only from the genetic correlation between MDD and reported trauma exposure. Further details of these analyses are provided in the Supplementary Methods.

Genetic correlations

Genetic correlations (rg) were calculated to assess shared genetic influences between MDD and other phenotypes, using GWAS summary statistics and LD Score regression v1.0.0 51 using the default HapMap LD reference. Two sets of genetic correlations were calculated. First, we calculated genetic correlations between the phenotypes examined within this paper (internal phenotypes). We calculated the genetic correlation between MDD and reported trauma exposure in the full dataset, and then the genetic correlation between MDD in individuals reporting trauma exposure and MDD in individuals not reporting trauma exposure. Secondly, we also calculated genetic correlations between each GWAS from this analysis and a curated list of 308 publicly-available phenotypes (external phenotypes) 51, 52.

Genetic correlations were tested for difference from 0 (default in LD Score), and for difference from 1 (in Microsoft Excel, converting rg to a chi-square as [(rg- 1)/se]2) 51, 52. Genetic correlations were considered significant if they passed the Bonferroni-adjusted threshold for the effective number of traits studied in each analysis (internal: p < 0.01; external: p < 2.5×10-4). The effective number of traits was calculated as the number of principal components explaining 99.5% of the variance in the pairwise genetic correlation matrix (internal: 5; external: 202). External phenotype GWAS all had heritability estimates such that h2/SE > 2, and produced valid (i.e. non-NA) rg with all other phenotypes tested.

The genetic correlation of MDD with each external phenotype was compared between individuals reporting trauma exposure and individuals not reporting trauma exposure using a two-stage method. First, differences were assessed using two sample z-tests 53. Nominally-significant differences (p < 0.05) by this method were then compared using the block-jackknife (Supplementary Methods) 52, 54, 55. Results using the jackknife were considered significant if they passed the Bonferroni-adjusted threshold (p < 2.5×10-4).

Polygenic Risk Scoring

Polygenic risk scores were calculated to further assess shared genetic influences between MDD and traits known to be correlated to MDD. Specifically, risk scores from analyses of major depression (MDD) 15, schizophrenia (SCZ) 56, bipolar disorder (BIP) 57, body mass index (BMI) 58 and glycated haemoglobin (HbA1c; used as a negative control) 59 were calculated and compared in all participants and stratifying by reported trauma exposure. The PGC major depression GWAS contained participants from UK Biobank, so to derive the MDD risk score we used a restricted set of summary statistics without these individuals (but including individuals from 23andMe, whose diagnoses were self-reported 14). For further discussion of this overlap, see Supplementary Note 15. Risk scores were calculated using PRSice v2 (https://github.com/choishingwan/PRSice 45, 60) at seven thresholds (external GWAS p < 0.001, 0.05, 0.1, 0.2, 0.3, 0.4 and 0.5) to allow assessment of the spread of association between risk score and MDD. Analyses used logistic regression, including all covariates used in creating the residuals for GWAS. In total, five external phenotypes were used to produce risk scores for the three target phenotypes (MDD overall, and stratified by reported trauma exposure/non-exposure), resulting in 15 analyses. A conservative Bonferroni adjustment for multiple testing was used, correcting for 105 tests (seven thresholds and 15 analyses), giving a final threshold for significance of p < 0.0004.

In addition to these stratified analyses, we performed formal risk score-by-environment analyses to estimate the effect on MDD of the interaction between genetic variants across the whole genome (modelled as a risk score) and reported trauma exposure. We calculated interactions between reported trauma exposure and the risk score capturing the most variance from each of the main-effect polygenic risk score analyses. These analyses included the same covariates used in the GWAS, and all risk score-by-covariate and reported trauma exposure-by-covariate interactions 61, 62. Both multiplicative and additive interactions were tested. A significant multiplicative interaction means that the combined effect of the risk score and reported trauma exposure differs from the product of their individual effects. Multiplicative interactions were tested using logistic regression 25, 26. A significant additive interaction means that the combined effect of the risk score and reported trauma exposure differs from the sum of their individual effects. Additive interactions were tested using linear regression (Supplementary Methods).

Sensitivity analyses

Differences in phenotypic variables were observed between cases and controls. To assess the impact of including these variables as covariates, all analyses were rerun retaining all previous covariates and including as further covariates: age (at questionnaire), neighbourhood socioeconomic status (SES, as Townsend deprivation index 37), BMI (at baseline assessment), and a binary variable of education (university degree vs. not). The same covariates were also included in polygenic risk score and SNP-based heritability analyses. Sensitivity analyses focussing on reported trauma exposure as an outcome were similarly rerun (Supplementary Methods).

The majority of the sample with data on both MDD symptoms and reported trauma status were controls who did not report trauma (Table 1). To assess whether this disbalance in sample status affected our results, genetic correlation analyses with external phenotypes were rerun on ten downsampled cohorts, each with 9,487 participants (the number of cases not reporting trauma exposure; see Supplementary Methods).

In order to test whether our definition of trauma exposure affected the main finding of our paper, we performed three further sensitivity analyses, redefining reported trauma exposure. First, we assessed if our main finding was robust to changing the threshold for including MDD-relevant trauma, by redefining reported trauma exposure as a report of i) one or more and ii) three or more of the seven MDD-relevant trauma items. Second, we assessed whether the timing of trauma exposure affected this finding by redefining reported trauma exposure as a report of iii) one or more of the five childhood trauma items. We then re-analysed the heritability of MDD in individuals reporting and not reporting trauma exposure using these three alternative definitions.

Results

Phenotype distribution

Phenotypic and genetic data were available on 24,094 to 92,957 individuals (Table 1). Overall, 36% of individuals met our definition of MDD-relevant trauma exposure, and were more frequently cases (45%) than controls (17%; OR = 5.23; p < 10-50, chi-square test). We assessed a number of phenotypic correlates of depression to confirm that these correlates differed between MDD cases and controls, and to assess whether these differences were affected by trauma exposure. Cases differed significantly from controls overall. Individuals with MDD were mostly females, significantly younger, less likely to have a university degree, came from more deprived neighbourhoods, and had higher BMI at recruitment. These differences persisted when the cohort was limited just to individuals reporting trauma exposure, and when the cohort was limited just to individuals not reporting trauma exposure. Furthermore, cases reporting trauma exposure differed from cases not reporting trauma exposure, in that they were mostly females, younger, more likely to have a degree (note difference from case-control comparisons), came from more deprived neighbourhoods, and had higher BMI at recruitment. The same differences (in the same direction) were observed between controls reporting and not reporting trauma exposure (all p < 0.05; Supplementary Table 4).

Genome-wide association studies

We performed GWAS for MDD overall and stratified by reported trauma exposure to obtain results for heritability and genetic correlation analyses (Supplementary Table 6; Supplementary Figures 1-3). No analysis showed evidence of genome-wide inflation that was attributable to confounding (95% confidence intervals of all regression intercepts from LD Score included 1; Supplementary Table 7). One genome-wide significant locus (rs11515172, Chr 9:11Mb, p = 3.82×10-8) was identified in the analysis of MDD overall, and remained significant when using logistic regression (p = 4.69 x 10-8, OR = 0.96, SE = 0.007; Supplementary Table 6). This locus has been repeatedly associated with depression 15, 63, 64 and with neuroticism 65–68; however, it should be noted that all of these studies included UK Biobank. The locus is intergenic, and is not annotated to any currently known biological feature of interest (Supplementary Table 8).

Figure 1:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 1:

Genetic correlations between MDD (overall and stratified by reported trauma exposure) and selected traits and disorders. Full genetic correlation results are available in Supplementary Table 11. Numbers = genetic correlations. Colour = direction of effect (blue = positive, red = negative). Colour intensity = size of correlation. Upper and lower bars are 95% confidence interval of genetic correlation.

SNP-based heritability

First we estimated the observed scale SNP-based heritability of MDD overall and stratified by reported trauma exposure. Second, in order to assess whether the relative influence of genetic variants on MDD differed by reported trauma status, we converted SNP-heritabilities to the liability scale. We assumed a prevalence of 28% for self-reported MDD in the full population 2. Based on this, and on the ratio of MDD cases:controls in the sample, we estimated the prevalence of MDD in the trauma-exposed population as 52%, and in the unexposed population as 17%. Using these estimates of population prevalence, the liability scale estimate of MDD SNP-based heritability was 20% (95% confidence interval: [18-22%]) overall. In those reporting trauma exposure, the liability scale SNP-based heritability of MDD was 24% [18-31%], and in those not reporting trauma exposure it was 12% [7-16%]. The SNP-based heritability of MDD was significantly greater in individuals who reported trauma exposure compared to those who did not (p = 0.0021, Z-test).

These estimated SNP-heritabilities could be confounded by genetic correlation between MDD and reported trauma exposure. We conducted simulations of SNP-level data to quantify the expected difference in SNP-based heritability from genetic correlation alone. Our simulations yielded expected estimates for the liability scale SNP-based heritability of MDD of 14-15% in those reporting trauma exposure, and 15-16% in those not reporting trauma exposure (Supplementary Methods). This small difference in expected SNP-based heritability for those reporting and not reporting trauma is in the opposite direction to our findings. This suggests that our findings cannot be explained by genetic correlation between MDD and reported trauma exposure, nor by the transformation from the observed scale to the liability scale.

Genetic correlations

Genetic correlations were calculated between MDD and reported trauma to explore the genetic relationship between these traits. Further genetic correlations were calculated between MDD in the two strata to assess whether genetics influences on MDD differ in the context of reported trauma exposure (Supplementary Table 10).

We observed a significant rg between MDD and reported trauma exposure in the full cohort (0.62 [95% CI: 0.76-0.94], p < 10-50). Given that trauma items were selected for association with MDD, we also calculated the genetic correlation between MDD in the full cohort and reported trauma exposure in just the controls, which was also significantly greater than 0 (0.31 [0.18-0.45], p = 4×10-6; Supplementary Table 10). This correlation persisted when using independent major depression GWAS summary statistics, as reported trauma exposure was significantly correlated with the MDD polygenic risk score (Spearman’s rho = 0.0675, p < 10-50) 15. The genetic correlation between MDD in individuals reporting trauma exposure and MDD in individuals not reporting trauma exposure was high and did not differ significantly from 1 (rg = 0.77 [0.48-1.05]; difference from 0: p = 1.8 x 10-7; difference from 1: p = 0.11).

Genetic correlations were calculated between MDD and all available external traits to systematically assess whether genetic relationships with MDD differed in the context of reported trauma exposure. All psychiatric traits included were significantly associated (p < 2.5×10-4) with MDD, but this association did not differ substantially in magnitude between the groups reporting and not reporting trauma exposure (z-test for comparisons of rg - Δrg - ranged from p = 0.10 - 0.99; Figure 1). In contrast, waist circumference was significantly associated with MDD only in individuals reporting trauma exposure (rg = 0.24), and the correlation was significantly larger than that in individuals not reporting trauma exposure (rg = −0.05, jackknife pΔrg = 2.3×10-4). Other correlations between MDD and body composition, reproductive, and socioeconomic phenotypes were larger in the group reporting trauma exposure compared to individuals not reporting trauma exposure, but these differences did not remain significant following multiple testing correction (all jackknife p > 2.5×10-4; Figure 1, Supplementary Table 11).

Polygenic risk scores across strata

We performed polygenic risk score analyses to further explore how stratification by trauma status affects the genetic relationship between MDD and specific correlates of MDD, and to mirror previous analyses in the literature (Figure 2, Table 2; see Supplementary Table 12 for full details of all risk score analyses, including the number of SNPs in each score) 26. Individuals with high genetic risk scores for MDD were more likely to be cases than controls, and a significant additive interaction term was observed from linear regression. Specifically, the combined effect of the MDD risk score and reported trauma exposure on MDD was greater than the sum of the individual effects (beta > 0, Table 2 central panel). However, the multiplicative interaction term was not significant (p > 0.01). The presence of an interaction on the additive scale reflects the greater SNP-based heritability of MDD in individuals reporting trauma exposure (SNP-h2 = 24%) compared to those not reporting trauma exposure (SNP-h2 = 12%), as described above.

Figure 2:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 2:

Association between MDD polygenic risk score (PRS) and MDD. Individuals reporting trauma exposure are shown as orange triangles, and those not reporting trauma exposure as green dots. Panel a shows the relationship on the linear additive scale, and panel b shows the relationship on the multiplicative scale. A significant interaction is observed on the additive scale only, as shown by differing slopes of the two regression lines in panel a.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2:

Main effect and interaction effects for polygenic risk scores (PRS) associated with MDD overall and in stratified analyses. Interaction effects are on he additive scale (Beta) and the multiplicative scale (OR). Bold = significant associations (main analyses: p < 0.000143; interactions: p < 0.01). Base N = ases // Controls. OR/Beta = Increase with 1 SD increase in risk score or trauma exposure. Results are reported at the “best” threshold (that with the lowest p-value in main effect analyses) - results across all thresholds are reported in Supplementary Table 12.

In contrast, although those with higher BMI risk scores were more likely to be cases than controls, this only passed correction for multiple testing in individuals reporting trauma exposure. Both the additive (beta > 0) and the multiplicative (OR > interaction terms were significant, suggesting the combined effect on MDD from BMI risk score and reported trauma exposure together was greater than expected from both the sum of the individual risks and from their product, respectively (OR > 1).

Individuals with high genetic risk scores for SCZ were more likely to be cases than controls, but this did not differ between strata (both interaction terms p >0.01). Individuals with higher BIP risk scores were also more likely to be cases than controls - although this association was not significant in the subset of individuals reporting trauma exposure, no significant interaction term was observed, suggesting the observed difference in results within-strata may be due to differences in power. No significant differences were observed in the negative control analysis with HbA1c.

Sensitivity analyses

Four sets of sensitivity analyses were performed. In the first set, all analyses were repeated using reported trauma exposure as the phenotype, assessed overall and stratified by MDD (as opposed to the primary analysis, where MDD was the phenotype and analyses were stratified by reported trauma exposure). Results from these analyses were broadly similar to the results from the primary analysis (Supplementary Tables 3-12, Supplementary Figures 4-7).

The second set of sensitivity analyses repeated the primary analyses with additional covariates to assess the impact of controlling for age, neighbourhood socioeconomic status, BMI, and education. This did not alter the conclusions drawn from the GWAS and SNP-based heritability analyses, nor from the genetic correlations observed between the internal phenotypes (those assessed in this study; Supplementary Tables 13-18). Genetic correlations between MDD and external phenotypes did not differ significantly from the main analysis (all z-test p < 0.05), but were sufficiently attenuated that the genetic correlations of MDD with waist circumference was no longer significantly different between individuals reporting and not reporting trauma exposure. Differences in the polygenic risk score analyses were limited to analyses involving the BMI risk score. The BMI risk score was no longer associated with MDD in any analysis, and no interactions including the BMI risk score remained significant. This suggests the effects of the BMI risk score in the main analysis likely result from BMI differences between MDD cases and controls, rather than being true effects on MDD.

The third set of sensitivity analyses repeated the genetic correlation analyses, but downsampled the analysed cohort such that each of the four groups (MDD cases/controls reporting/not reporting trauma exposure) had 9,487 participants (the size of the smallest group from the main analysis, cases not reporting trauma exposure). In these analyses, genetic correlations between MDD and external phenotypes were attenuated across most phenotypes, but not significantly (two-sample z-tests, all p > 0.05; Supplementary Table 19). As such, the general pattern of genetic correlations observed in the main analysis was retained, although the genetic correlations of MDD with waist circumference was no longer significantly different between individuals reporting and not reporting trauma exposure.

The final set of sensitivity analyses repeated the SNP-based heritability analyses of MDD in individuals reporting and not reporting trauma exposure, altering the definition of reported trauma exposure in three ways (increasing and decreasing the number of items required to be defined as reporting trauma exposure, and limiting the items considered to only childhood experiences). The purpose of these analyses was to test the robustness of our key finding (greater MDD SNP-based heritability in trauma-exposed individuals compared to those not reporting trauma exposure). Neither increasing nor decreasing the number of MDD-relevant items selected, nor focussing on childhood items, altered our conclusions (Supplementary Table 20).

Full results for all four sensitivity analyses are included in the Supplementary Material.

Discussion

We investigated the relationship between MDD and self-reported trauma exposure in the largest single cohort available to date (N=73,258 with MDD and reported trauma data). We examined individual genetic variants, SNP-based heritability, genetic correlations, and polygenic risk scores. The SNP-based heritability of MDD was higher in individuals reporting trauma exposure than in individuals not reporting trauma exposure. This was not attributable to gene-environment correlation, nor to the transformation of SNP-based heritability from the observed to the liability scale. Despite the significant difference in SNP-based heritability across the two strata, the genetic correlation between MDD in individuals reporting and not reporting trauma exposure was not statistically different from 1. Polygenic risk score-by-reported trauma exposure interaction analyses identified significant interactions for both MDD and BMI risk scores. However, the interactions involving the BMI risk score appear to reflect differences in measured BMI between MDD cases and controls, rather than differences directly related to MDD. Finally, a significant genetic correlation between MDD and waist circumference was observed only in individuals reporting trauma exposure, and was absent from those not reporting trauma exposure.

A number of limitations should be considered when assessing our results. Our simulations suggest that the significantly higher SNP-based heritability of MDD in individuals reporting trauma exposure did not result from gene-environment correlation between MDD and reported trauma exposure, nor from the transformation of the observed scale SNP-based heritability to the liability scale. However, we could not address further sources of potential bias. These could arise from genetic architectures other than those simulated (including non-additive genetic effects), from intrinsic challenges of heritability estimation in case-control data (including ascertainment bias and the effects of covariates not included in the model) 69, 70, or from potential collider bias (the induction of a correlation between two uncorrelated variables by conditioning on a third unknown variable correlated with both) resulting from selection bias 71. We also assumed that the population prevalence of reported trauma exposure can be extrapolated from that observed in this sample (see Supplementary Methods). Although the UK Biobank allows us to integrate genetic and environmental data at scale, and is a reasonably homogeneous cohort, it also has a “healthy volunteer bias”, whereby the participants tend to have better overall health and higher socioeconomic status compared to the equivalent overall population of this age 72. It is possible that the depressive and traumatic experiences reported by these participants may not generalise to the whole population, or to clinically-ascertained cases. Furthermore, we focussed on European ancestry; further studies in non-European populations are required 73.

To obtain further insight into the association of genome-wide genetic variation and reported trauma exposure with MDD (and to enable comparison with previous studies 24–26), we carried out polygenic risk score-by-environment interaction analyses. There are a number of limitations to consider when interpreting such analyses. Polygenic risk score-by-environment interaction analyses test a specific hypothesis, namely that the overall association of common variants with the outcome (modelled as a risk score) varies dependent on the environmental exposure being tested. As such, the absence of an interaction does not preclude the existence of specific variant-by-environment interactions, including those featuring variants contributing to the risk score. Furthermore, we cannot exclude the possibility that the correlation between the MDD risk score and reported trauma exposure may alter the observed interaction. As such, caution is needed before drawing strong conclusions, especially given the small effect sizes, and limited predictive power, of risk scores in this study (Supplementary Table 12).

Throughout this paper, we have referred to our depression phenotype as “MDD” rather than “major depression”. We do this because our definition is based on the CIDI-SF, which has previously been shown to have good concordance with direct clinical assessments of MDD 74, 75. However, it should be noted that direct assessment was not performed, and our probable MDD cases may not have met criteria within a clinical setting. Nonetheless, genetic correlations between studies of MDD and of major depression are high, suggesting there is strong genetic continuity across different methods of assessing depression 15, 64.

Trauma exposure was defined in this study using retrospective self-report. This is not the ideal measure for this phenotype, and precludes robust measurement of the severity and timing of the reported trauma exposure. However, retrospective report is the only feasible option in large cohorts like the UK Biobank. The requirement for cohort sizes large enough to identify the small individual genetic effects typical of complex genetic traits such as MDD makes self-report the most practicable method of data-collection. Retrospectively reported trauma and MDD data are not robust to reverse causation, and our results cannot strongly inform any temporal or causal hypotheses about the relationship between trauma and MDD. Such hypotheses could be tested using longitudinal studies (with the inherent logistical difficulties in obtaining both environmental and genomic data) or through more powerful genomic studies of trauma exposure in a larger cohort. This latter design would enable the identification of sufficient robustly associated genetic variants to inform approaches such as Mendelian randomisation (which we were underpowered to examine in this study). In addition, future work may benefit from assessing the heritability of broader depression phenotypes that lie beyond our binary criteria, including reward sensitivity and negative valence traits 76.

Our findings suggest that the genetic variants associated with MDD are the same in individuals reporting and not reporting trauma exposure, because the genetic correlation between MDD measured in these two groups was not significantly different from 1. However, the SNP-based heritability of MDD was greater in individuals reporting compared to not reporting trauma exposure. This suggests that the combined effect of the variants associated with MDD is greater in people reporting trauma exposure than in those who do not. The mechanism underlying this finding is uncertain. One possibility is that exposure to traumatic events might amplify genetic influences on MDD beyond the magnitude of the effects seen in the absence of trauma (consistent with the stress-diathesis hypothesis 77–79). The concept that genetic variance varies with exposure to different environments is well-recognised in studies of animal populations in the wild 80. However, the opposite may also be true; genetic influences on MDD could increase an individual’s likelihood of experiencing and/or reporting of trauma, and through doing so increase the apparent heritability of MDD by partly incorporating genetic influences related to trauma reporting itself 11. A final possibility relates to the components of variance involved in calculating SNP-based heritability. Phenotypic variance can be attributed either to the SNPs measured in the GWAS, or to environmental sources of variance reflecting all phenotypic variance not explained by common variants. It is possible that the genetic component of variance is constant across the strata, but that the environmental component is smaller in individuals reporting trauma exposure, for example due to the shared (and thus more similar/less variable) exposure of these individuals to MDD-relevant traumatic experiences. This would result in greater heritability in individuals reporting trauma exposure. We provide these explanations as potential interpretations of these findings. However, these are not the only possibilities, and it is also likely that multiple such mechanisms are involved.

In polygenic risk score-by-reported trauma exposure interaction analyses, we identified a significant interaction on the additive scale for the combined effect of the MDD risk score and reported trauma exposure on risk of MDD. These results are also reflected in the larger SNP-based heritability of MDD in exposed compared to unexposed individuals. The simplest explanation for this result is that the effects of the MDD risk score and reported trauma exposure on MDD combine multiplicatively, such that their combined effects are greater than the sum of their individual effects. For the BMI risk score however, the interaction with reported trauma exposure appears to be more complex, combining neither additively nor multiplicatively. In sensitivity analyses controlling for BMI (obtained at recruitment, approximately five years before the mental health questionnaire), the BMI risk score-by-reported trauma exposure interaction was no longer significant, suggesting that the observed interaction can be explained by differences in measured BMI. Further research, with concurrent measurements of BMI, trauma exposure and MDD in a longitudinally-sampled cohort would offer further insight into the relationship between these three variables.

The high genetic correlation between MDD in individuals reporting and not reporting trauma exposure was supported by significant genetic correlations between MDD and other psychiatric disorders regardless of reported trauma exposure. In individuals reporting trauma exposure, a further significant genetic correlation was observed between MDD and waist circumference, which was significantly greater than the equivalent correlation in those not reporting trauma exposure. Although not significant, there was also a general pattern of higher genetic correlations between MDD and several weight-related measures and educational attainment, in individuals reporting trauma exposure. This is consistent with previous literature on traumatic experiences across the life course, and related phenomena such as Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs). This literature has found that such adversities are associated not only with psychiatric risk but also with wide-ranging impairments in social and health outcomes including obesity and education 81–84. However, we stress that causal conclusions cannot be drawn from these data, or that the reported trauma exposure is responsible for the observed differences.

Our estimate of the SNP-based heritability of MDD (20%) is higher than that reported in previous studies of major depression (∼9%) 15. This may be explained by the relative homogeneity of the UK Biobank compared to previous meta-analyses. The UK Biobank is a single-country cohort ascertained using a consistent protocol. The same questionnaire was used to gather symptom data, and the samples were stored, extracted, and genotyped using a single method. In contrast, meta-analyses have needed to combine diverse ascertainment, sampling, and genotyping; SNP-based heritability has been reported to decrease with increasing numbers of meta-analysed samples 85. Previous analyses have assessed alternative depression phenotypes in the UK Biobank 64. Our MDD phenotype (based on DSM criteria for MDD) is most similar to the probable MDD phenotype from Howard et al, rather than the less strictly-defined “broad depression” phenotype, which includes those who seek treatment for depression, anxiety and related phenotypes. More specifically the “broad depression” phenotypes includes many people with MDD, but also many people who may not meet diagnostic criteria for MDD or instead have anxiety disorders or a similar closely related common mental disorder. Our MDD phenotype includes only those people who meet diagnostic criteria (using the CIDI questionnaire). Our estimate from BOLT-LMM (19.9%) is within the bounds of the reported range of GREML estimates by geographic region reported for the probable MDD (0% to 27.5%) phenotype. Our LDSC-based estimate is higher than the equivalent from Howard et al (4-5%). However, the estimate from Howard et al is considerably lower than previous estimates 15, and potentially lacks both the specificity of definitions from clinical practice or structured questionnaires, and the sensitivity of broad phenotyping methods.

Our results also differ in several respects from those of a study of MDD and adversity in Han Chinese women 23. No difference in the SNP-based heritability of MDD between individuals reporting and not reporting trauma exposure was observed in the previous study, and we did not replicate individual variant results. However, this is unsurprising, as there are a number of differences between the studies of which the primary one is sample size (this study: 73,258; CONVERGE: 9,599). Other differences included culture and ethnicity, and the deeper phenotyping methodology applied in CONVERGE, resulting in a severe inpatient MDD phenotype. Notably, the previous study did not report a genetic correlation between MDD and trauma exposure 23.

Sensitivity analyses focussed on trauma found that self-reported traumatic experience was significantly heritable, as has been previously observed 19. We strongly emphasise that this does not necessarily imply that traumatic experiences themselves have a biological component - such experiences may be associated with other significantly heritable traits, and their biology would then be reflected in the observed heritability of trauma exposure. One potential set of heritable traits that may be associated with reporting traumatic experiences are personality traits such as risk-taking, and this might explain the observed genetic correlations with psychiatric traits. A similar phenomenon has been proposed to underlie observed genetic correlations with socioeconomic status 86. Our trauma exposure measure relies on retrospective self-report, which is itself correlated with personality traits and mood at time of report 9. This may also explain the genetic correlations we observe with reported trauma exposure (including in controls, who do not report previous psychiatric illness).

In summary, we find that genetic associations with MDD in UK Biobank vary by context. Specifically, the SNP-based heritability of MDD is larger in individuals reporting trauma exposure compared to those not doing so. Furthermore, the genetic correlation of MDD with waist circumference was significant only in individuals reporting exposure to trauma. Nonetheless, a strong genetic correlation was observed between MDD measured in the two strata. Together, these findings suggest the relative contribution of genetic variants to variance in MDD is greater when additional risk factors are present.

Acknowledgements

We thank the members of the UK Biobank Mental Health Genetics Group for their valuable discussion and feedback on this work. We are also deeply indebted to the scientists involved in the construction of the UK Biobank, and to the investigators who comprise the PGC. Finally, we thank the hundreds of thousands of subjects who have shared their life experiences with investigators in the UK Biobank and the PGC.

This research has been conducted using the UK Biobank Resource, as an approved extension to application 16577 (Dr Breen). This study represents independent research funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care. High performance computing facilities were funded with capital equipment grants from the GSTT Charity (TR130505) and Maudsley Charity (980). WJP was funded by NWO Veni grant 91619152. K.L.P acknowledges funding from the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation. N.R.W acknowledges funding from the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (1078901 and 1087889). The PGC has received major funding from the US National Institute of Mental Health and the US National Institute of Drug Abuse (U01 MH109528 and U01 MH1095320).

Footnotes

  • - Inclusion of additional sensitivity analyses increasing/decreasing the threshold for defining reported trauma exposure, and focussing on traumas experienced in childhood. No qualitative alterations to overall result. - Alteration to multiple-testing correction for genetic correlation analyses to better account for correlated phenotypes - Expanded discussion of potential mechanisms of observed increase in MDD SNP-based heritability when trauma exposure is reported - Substantial rewrite to increase clarity

References

  1. 1.↵
    GBD 2016 Disease and Injury Incidence and Prevalence Collaborators. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 328 diseases and injuries for 195 countries, 1990-2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet 2017; 390: 1211–1259.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. 2.↵
    McManus S, Bebbington P, Jenkins R, Brugha T. Mental Health and Wellbeing in England: Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey 2014: a Survey Carried Out for NHS Digital by NatCen Social Research and the Department of Health Sciences, University of Leicester. NHS Digital, 2016.
  3. 3.↵
    Green JG, McLaughlin KA, Berglund PA, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication I: associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67: 113–123.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  4. 4.
    Nanni V, Uher R, Danese A. Childhood maltreatment predicts unfavorable course of illness and treatment outcome in depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169: 141–151.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. 5.
    Kessler RC. The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annu Rev Psychol 1997; 48: 191–214.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  6. 6.↵
    McLaughlin KA, Conron KJ, Koenen KC, Gilman SE. Childhood adversity, adult stressful life events, and risk of past-year psychiatric disorder: a test of the stress sensitization hypothesis in a population-based sample of adults. Psychol Med 2010; 40: 1647–1658.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  7. 7.
    Kessler RC, Davis CG, Kendler KS. Childhood adversity and adult psychiatric disorder in the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychol Med 1997; 27: 1101–1119.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  8. 8.
    Collishaw S, Pickles A, Messer J, Rutter M, Shearer C, Maughan B. Resilience to adult psychopathology following childhood maltreatment: evidence from a community sample. Child Abuse Negl 2007; 31: 211–229.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  9. 9.↵
    Baldwin JR, Reuben A, Newbury JB, Danese A. Agreement Between Prospective and Retrospective Measures of Childhood Maltreatment: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 2019. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0097.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  10. 10.↵
    Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Prescott CA. Causal relationship between stressful life events and the onset of major depression. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156: 837–841.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  11. 11.↵
    Kendler KS, Karkowski-Shuman L. Stressful life events and genetic liability to major depression: genetic control of exposure to the environment? Psychol Med 1997; 27: 539–547.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  12. 12.↵
    Polderman TJC, Benyamin B, de Leeuw CA, Sullivan PF, van Bochoven A, Visscher PM et al. Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nat Genet 2015; 47: 702–709.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. 13.↵
    Yang J, Zeng J, Goddard ME, Wray NR, Visscher PM. Concepts, estimation and interpretation of SNP heritability. Nat Genet 2017; 49: 1304–1310.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  14. 14.↵
    Hyde CL, Nagle MW, Tian C, Chen X, Paciga SA, Wendland JR et al. Identification of 15 genetic loci associated with risk of major depression in individuals of European descent. Nat Genet 2016; 48: 1031–1036.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. 15.↵
    Wray NR, Ripke S, Mattheisen M, Trzaskowski M, Byrne EM, Abdellaoui A et al. Genome-wide association analyses identify 44 risk variants and refine the genetic architecture of major depression. Nat Genet 2018; 50: 668–681.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. 16.↵
    Jang KL, Vernon PA, Livesley WJ, Stein MB, Wolf H. Intra- and extra-familial influences on alcohol and drug misuse: a twin study of gene-environment correlation. Addiction 2001; 96: 1307–1318.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  17. 17.
    Stein MB, Jang KL, Taylor S, Vernon PA, Livesley WJ. Genetic and environmental influences on trauma exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: a twin study. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159: 1675–1681.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  18. 18.↵
    Lyons MJ, Goldberg J, Eisen SA, True W, Tsuang MT, Meyer JM et al. Do genes influence exposure to trauma? A twin study of combat. Am J Med Genet 1993; 48: 22–27.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  19. 19.↵
    Power RA, Wingenbach T, Cohen-Woods S, Uher R, Ng MY, Butler AW et al. Estimating the heritability of reporting stressful life events captured by common genetic variants. Psychol Med 2013; 43: 1965–1971.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. 20.↵
    Dunn EC, Brown RC, Dai Y, Rosand J, Nugent NR, Amstadter AB et al. Genetic determinants of depression: recent findings and future directions. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2015; 23: 1–18.
    OpenUrl
  21. 21.↵
    Schraedley PK, Turner RJ, Gotlib IH. Stability of retrospective reports in depression: traumatic events, past depressive episodes, and parental psychopathology. J Health Soc Behav 2002; 43: 307–316.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  22. 22.↵
    Dunn EC, Wiste A, Radmanesh F, Almli LM, Gogarten SM, Sofer T et al. GENOME-WIDE ASSOCIATION STUDY (GWAS) AND GENOME-WIDE BY ENVIRONMENT INTERACTION STUDY (GWEIS) OF DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS IN AFRICAN AMERICAN AND HISPANIC/LATINA WOMEN. Depress Anxiety 2016; 33: 265–280.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. 23.↵
    Peterson RE, Cai N, Dahl AW, Bigdeli TB, Edwards AC, Webb BT et al. Molecular Genetic Analysis Subdivided by Adversity Exposure Suggests Etiologic Heterogeneity in Major Depression. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175: 545–554.
    OpenUrl
  24. 24.↵
    Peyrot WJ, Milaneschi Y, Abdellaoui A, Sullivan PF, Hottenga JJ, Boomsma DI et al. Effect of polygenic risk scores on depression in childhood trauma. Br J Psychiatry 2014; 205: 113–119.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. 25.↵
    Mullins N, Power RA, Fisher HL, Hanscombe KB, Euesden J, Iniesta R et al. Polygenic interactions with environmental adversity in the aetiology of major depressive disorder. Psychol Med 2016; 46: 759–770.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. 26.↵
    Peyrot WJ, Van der Auwera S, Milaneschi Y, Dolan CV, Madden PAF, Sullivan PF et al. Does Childhood Trauma Moderate Polygenic Risk for Depression? A Meta-analysis of 5765 Subjects From the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Biol Psychiatry 2017. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.09.009.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  27. 27.↵
    Jaffee SR, Price TS. Gene-environment correlations: a review of the evidence and implications for prevention of mental illness. Mol Psychiatry 2007; 12: 432–442.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  28. 28.↵
    Lau JYF, Eley TC. Disentangling gene-environment correlations and interactions on adolescent depressive symptoms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008; 49: 142–150.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  29. 29.
    Thapar A, Harold G, McGuffin P. Life events and depressive symptoms in childhood--shared genes or shared adversity? A research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1998; 39: 1153–1158.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  30. 30.↵
    Boardman JD, Alexander KB, Stallings MC. Stressful life events and depression among adolescent twin pairs. Biodemography Soc Biol 2011; 57: 53–66.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. 31.↵
    Allen NE, Sudlow C, Peakman T, Collins R, UK Biobank. UK biobank data: come and get it. Sci Transl Med 2014; 6: 224ed4.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  32. 32.↵
    Davis KAS, Coleman JRI, Adams M, Allen N, Breen G, Cullen B et al. Mental health in UK Biobank: development, implementation and results from an online questionnaire completed by 157 366 participants. BJPsych Open 2018; 4: 83–90.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  33. 33.↵
    Smith DJ, Nicholl BI, Cullen B, Martin D, Ul-Haq Z, Evans J et al. Prevalence and characteristics of probable major depression and bipolar disorder within UK biobank: cross-sectional study of 172,751 participants. PLoS One 2013; 8: e75362.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. 34.↵
    Bellis MA, Hughes K, Leckenby N, Perkins C, Lowey H. National household survey of adverse childhood experiences and their relationship with resilience to health-harming behaviors in England. BMC Med 2014; 12: 72.
  35. 35.
    Bernstein DP, Fink L, Handelsman L, Foote J, Lovejoy M, Wenzel K et al. Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151: 1132–1136.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  36. 36.↵
    Grabe HJ, Schulz A, Schmidt CO, Appel K, Driessen M, Wingenfeld K et al. [A brief instrument for the assessment of childhood abuse and neglect: the childhood trauma screener (CTS)]. Psychiatr Prax 2012; 39: 109–115.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  37. 37.↵
    Townsend P, Phillimore P, Beattie A. Health and Deprivation: Inequality and the North. Croom Helm, 1988.
  38. 38.↵
    Bycroft C, Freeman C, Petkova D, Band G, Elliott LT, Sharp K et al. The UK Biobank resource with deep phenotyping and genomic data. Nature 2018; 562: 203–209.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  39. 39.↵
    McCarthy S, Das S, Kretzschmar W, Delaneau O, Wood AR, Teumer A et al. A reference panel of 64,976 haplotypes for genotype imputation. Nat Genet 2016; 48: 1279–1283.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. 40.↵
    UK10K Consortium, Walter K, Min JL, Huang J, Crooks L, Memari Y et al. The UK10K project identifies rare variants in health and disease. Nature 2015; 526: 82–90.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. 41.↵
    Manichaikul A, Mychaleckyj JC, Rich SS, Daly K, Sale M, Chen W-M. Robust relationship inference in genome-wide association studies. Bioinformatics 2010; 26: 2867–2873.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  42. 42.↵
    Warren HR, Evangelou E, Cabrera CP, Gao H, Ren M, Mifsud B et al. Genome-wide association analysis identifies novel blood pressure loci and offers biological insights into cardiovascular risk. Nat Genet 2017; 49: 403– 415.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. 43.↵
    Abraham G, Qiu Y, Inouye M. FlashPCA2: principal component analysis of Biobank-scale genotype datasets. Bioinformatics 2017; 33: 2776–2778.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. 44.↵
    Dudbridge F, Gusnanto A. Estimation of significance thresholds for genomewide association scans. Genet Epidemiol 2008; 32: 227–234.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  45. 45.↵
    Chang CC, Chow CC, Tellier LC, Vattikuti S, Purcell SM, Lee JJ. Second-generation PLINK: rising to the challenge of larger and richer datasets. Gigascience 2015; 4: 7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. 46.↵
    Lloyd-Jones LR, Robinson MR, Yang J, Visscher PM. Transformation of Summary Statistics from Linear Mixed Model Association on All-or-None Traits to Odds Ratio. Genetics 2018. doi:10.1534/genetics.117.300360.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. 47.↵
    Marioni RE, Harris SE, Zhang Q, McRae AF, Hagenaars SP, Hill WD et al. GWAS on family history of Alzheimer’s disease. Transl Psychiatry 2018; 8: 99.
    OpenUrl
  48. 48.↵
    Team RC. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014. 2014.
  49. 49.↵
    Loh P-R, Kichaev G, Gazal S, Schoech AP, Price AL. Mixed-model association for biobank-scale datasets. Nat Genet 2018; 50: 906–908.
    OpenUrl
  50. 50.↵
    Lee SH, Goddard ME, Wray NR, Visscher PM. A better coefficient of determination for genetic profile analysis. Genet Epidemiol 2012; 36: 214– 224.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. 51.↵
    Bulik-Sullivan BK, Loh P-R, Finucane HK, Ripke S, Yang J, Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium et al. LD Score regression distinguishes confounding from polygenicity in genome-wide association studies. Nat Genet 2015; 47: 291–295.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. 52.↵
    Bulik-Sullivan B, Finucane HK, Anttila V, Gusev A, Day FR, Loh P-R et al. An atlas of genetic correlations across human diseases and traits. Nat Genet 2015; 47: 1236–1241.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. 53.↵
    Daniel. Biostatistics: A Foundation For Analysis In Health Sciences, 7Th Ed. Wiley India Pvt. Limited, 2006.
  54. 54.↵
    Tukey WJ. Bias and confidence in not-quite large samples. Ann Math Stat 1958; 29: 614.
    OpenUrl
  55. 55.↵
    Quenouille MH. Notes on Bias in Estimation. Biometrika 1956; 43: 353–360.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  56. 56.↵
    Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature 2014; 511: 421–427.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  57. 57.↵
    Psychiatric GWAS Consortium Bipolar Disorder Working Group. Large-scale genome-wide association analysis of bipolar disorder identifies a new susceptibility locus near ODZ4. Nat Genet 2011; 43: 977–983.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. 58.↵
    Locke AE, Kahali B, Berndt SI, Justice AE, Pers TH, Day FR et al. Genetic studies of body mass index yield new insights for obesity biology. Nature 2015; 518: 197–206.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. 59.↵
    Soranzo N, Sanna S, Wheeler E, Gieger C, Radke D, Dupuis J et al. Common variants at 10 genomic loci influence hemoglobin A1(C) levels via glycemic and nonglycemic pathways. Diabetes 2010; 59: 3229–3239.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. 60.↵
    Euesden J, Lewis CM, O’Reilly PF. PRSice: Polygenic Risk Score software. Bioinformatics 2015; 31: 1466–1468.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. 61.↵
    Keller MC. Gene × environment interaction studies have not properly controlled for potential confounders: the problem and the (simple) solution. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 75: 18–24.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  62. 62.↵
    Yzerbyt VY, Muller D, Judd CM. Adjusting researchers’ approach to adjustment: On the use of covariates when testing interactions. J Exp Soc Psychol 2004; 40: 424–431.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  63. 63.↵
    Turley P, Walters RK, Maghzian O, Okbay A, Lee JJ, Fontana MA et al. Multi-trait analysis of genome-wide association summary statistics using MTAG. Nat Genet 2018;: 118810.
  64. 64.↵
    Howard DM, Adams MJ, Shirali M, Clarke T-K, Marioni RE, Davies G et al. Genome-wide association study of depression phenotypes in UK Biobank identifies variants in excitatory synaptic pathways. Nat Commun 2018; 9: 1470.
    OpenUrl
  65. 65.↵
    Smith DJ, Escott-Price V, Davies G, Bailey MES, Colodro-Conde L, Ward J et al. Genome-wide analysis of over 106 000 individuals identifies 9 neuroticism-associated loci. Mol Psychiatry 2016; 21: 749–757.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. 66.
    Luciano M, Hagenaars SP, Davies G, Hill WD, Clarke T-K, Shirali M et al. Association analysis in over 329,000 individuals identifies 116 independent variants influencing neuroticism. Nat Genet 2018; 50: 6–11.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. 67.
    Okbay A, Baselmans BML, De Neve J-E, Turley P, Nivard MG, Fontana MA et al. Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Nat Genet 2016; 48: 624–633.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. 68.↵
    Nagel M, Jansen PR, Stringer S, Watanabe K, de Leeuw CA, Bryois J et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for neuroticism in 449,484 individuals identifies novel genetic loci and pathways. Nat Genet 2018; 50: 920–927.
    OpenUrl
  69. 69.↵
    Weissbrod O, Flint J, Rosset S. Estimating SNP Heritability and Genetic Correlation in Case-Control Studies Directly and with Summary Statistics. Am J Hum Genet 2018; 103: 89–99.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  70. 70.↵
    Golan D, Lander ES, Rosset S. Measuring missing heritability: inferring the contribution of common variants. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2014; 111: E5272–81.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  71. 71.↵
    Munafò MR, Tilling K, Taylor AE, Evans DM, Davey Smith G. Collider scope: when selection bias can substantially influence observed associations. Int J Epidemiol 2018; 47: 226–235.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. 72.↵
    Fry A, Littlejohns TJ, Sudlow C, Doherty N, Allen NE. OP41 The representativeness of the UK Biobank cohort on a range of sociodemographic, physical, lifestyle and health-related characteristics. J Epidemiol Community Health 2016; 70: A26–A26.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  73. 73.↵
    Martin AR, Gignoux CR, Walters RK, Wojcik GL, Neale BM, Gravel S et al. Human Demographic History Impacts Genetic Risk Prediction across Diverse Populations. Am J Hum Genet 2017; 100: 635–649.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  74. 74.↵
    Haro JM, Arbabzadeh-Bouchez S, Brugha TS, de Girolamo G, Guyer ME, Jin R et al. Concordance of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview Version 3.0 (CIDI 3.0) with standardized clinical assessments in the WHO World Mental Health surveys. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 2006; 15: 167– 180.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  75. 75.↵
    Kessler RC, Wittchen H-U, Abelson JM, Mcgonagle K, Schwarz N, Kendler KS et al. Methodological studies of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) in the US national comorbidity survey (NCS). Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 1998; 7: 33–55.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  76. 76.↵
    Nusslock R, Alloy LB. Reward processing and mood-related symptoms: An RDoC and translational neuroscience perspective. J Affect Disord 2017; 216: 3–16.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  77. 77.↵
    Meehl PE. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. Am Psychol 1962; 17: 827–838.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  78. 78.
    Bleuler M. Conception of schizophrenia within the last fifty years and today [abridged]. Proc R Soc Med 1963; 56: 945.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  79. 79.↵
    1. Rosenthal D
    Rosenthal D. A suggested conceptual framework. In: Rosenthal D (ed). The Genain quadruplets: A case study and theoretical analysis of heredity and environment in schizophrenia, (pp. Basic Books, xiv: New York, NY, US, 1963, pp 505–511.
  80. 80.↵
    Nussey DH, Wilson AJ, Brommer JE. The evolutionary ecology of individual phenotypic plasticity in wild populations. J Evol Biol 2007; 20: 831–844.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  81. 81.↵
    Fuemmeler BF, Dedert E, McClernon FJ, Beckham JC. Adverse childhood events are associated with obesity and disordered eating: results from a U.S. population-based survey of young adults. J Trauma Stress 2009; 22: 329– 333.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  82. 82.
    Metzler M, Merrick MT, Klevens J, Ports KA, Ford DC. Adverse childhood experiences and life opportunities: Shifting the narrative. Child Youth Serv Rev 2017; 72: 141–149.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  83. 83.
    Jaffee SR, Ambler A, Merrick M, Goldman-Mellor S, Odgers CL, Fisher HL et al. Childhood Maltreatment Predicts Poor Economic and Educational Outcomes in the Transition to Adulthood. Am J Public Health 2018; 108: 1142–1147.
    OpenUrl
  84. 84.↵
    Danese A, Tan M. Childhood maltreatment and obesity: systematic review and meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry 2014; 19: 544–554.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  85. 85.↵
    Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet 2013; 381: 1371.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  86. 86.↵
    Hill WD, Hagenaars SP, Marioni RE, Harris SE, Liewald DCM, Davies G et al. Molecular Genetic Contributions to Social Deprivation and Household Income in UK Biobank. Curr Biol 2016; 26: 3083–3089.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
View Abstract
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted May 13, 2019.
Download PDF

Supplementary Material

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Genome-wide gene-environment analyses of major depressive disorder and reported lifetime traumatic experiences in UK Biobank
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Genome-wide gene-environment analyses of major depressive disorder and reported lifetime traumatic experiences in UK Biobank
Jonathan R.I. Coleman, Wouter J. Peyrot, Kirstin L. Purves, Katrina A.S. Davis, Christopher Rayner, Shing Wan Choi, Christopher Hübel, Héléna A. Gaspar, Carol Kan, Sandra Van der Auwera, Mark James Adams, Donald M. Lyall, Karmel W. Choi, Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, Erin C. Dunn, Evangelos Vassos, Andrea Danese, Barbara Maughan, Hans J. Grabe, Cathryn M. Lewis, Paul F. O’Reilly, Andrew M. McIntosh, Daniel J. Smith, Naomi R. Wray, Matthew Hotopf, Thalia C. Eley, Gerome Breen
bioRxiv 247353; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/247353
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Genome-wide gene-environment analyses of major depressive disorder and reported lifetime traumatic experiences in UK Biobank
Jonathan R.I. Coleman, Wouter J. Peyrot, Kirstin L. Purves, Katrina A.S. Davis, Christopher Rayner, Shing Wan Choi, Christopher Hübel, Héléna A. Gaspar, Carol Kan, Sandra Van der Auwera, Mark James Adams, Donald M. Lyall, Karmel W. Choi, Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, Erin C. Dunn, Evangelos Vassos, Andrea Danese, Barbara Maughan, Hans J. Grabe, Cathryn M. Lewis, Paul F. O’Reilly, Andrew M. McIntosh, Daniel J. Smith, Naomi R. Wray, Matthew Hotopf, Thalia C. Eley, Gerome Breen
bioRxiv 247353; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/247353

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Genomics
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (2430)
  • Biochemistry (4791)
  • Bioengineering (3333)
  • Bioinformatics (14683)
  • Biophysics (6640)
  • Cancer Biology (5171)
  • Cell Biology (7428)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (4366)
  • Ecology (6873)
  • Epidemiology (2057)
  • Evolutionary Biology (9925)
  • Genetics (7346)
  • Genomics (9532)
  • Immunology (4557)
  • Microbiology (12686)
  • Molecular Biology (4948)
  • Neuroscience (28344)
  • Paleontology (199)
  • Pathology (809)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (1392)
  • Physiology (2024)
  • Plant Biology (4499)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (977)
  • Synthetic Biology (1299)
  • Systems Biology (3916)
  • Zoology (726)