ABSTRACT
OBJECTIVE: Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is extensively used in basic and clinical neuroscience. Previous work has shown substantial residual variability in TMS effects even despite use of on-line visual feedback monitoring of coil position. Here, we aimed to evaluate if off-line modeling of coil position and orientation deviations can enhance detection of TMS effects. METHODS: Retrospective modeling was used to denoise the impact of common coil position and rotation deviations during TMS experimental sessions on motor evoked potentials (MEP) to single pulse TMS. RESULTS: Offline denoising led to a 26.19% improvement in the signal to noise ratio (SNR) of corticospinal excitability measurements. CONCLUSIONS: Offline modeling enhanced detection of TMS effects by removing variability introduced by coil deviations. SIGNIFICANCE: This approach could allow more accurate determination of TMS effects in cognitive and interventional neuroscience.
HIGHLIGHTS
Coil deviations impact TMS effects despite use of on-line neuronavigation feedback.
Offline denoising of coil deviation impacts on TMS effects reduced variability.
Offline denoising also significantly improved overall SNR of TMS effects.
1. INTRODUCTION
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is widely used in cognitive and interventional neuroscience (Dayan et al., 2013). TMS effects are variable within and between individuals (Herrmann et al., 2006, Wassermann, 2008, Pasley et al., 2009, Nicolo et al., 2015), as shown by the stochastic nature of motor evoked potentials (MEP) to single TMS pulses (Kiers et al., 1993, Zarkowski et al., 2006, Goldsworthy et al., 2016). Sources of TMS effect variability include changes in coil position and orientation relative to the stimulation target occurring within and between experimental sessions (Mills et al., 1992). When poorly controlled, these coil deviations are a problematic source of experimental error or noise that limits the ability to accurately measure TMS effects of interest. Stereotactic neuronavigation systems developed to address this problem provide real-time information about the position and orientation of the coil relative to the individual subject’s scalp‐ or brain-defined target (Ruohonen et al., 2010). To date, this information has been predominantly used to provide online visual feedback to experimenters, thus assisting in manual correction of TMS targeting errors due to coil or head movement at the time stimulation is delivered. This approach has partially improved variability in measurement of TMS effects (Lioumis et al., 2009, Cincotta et al., 2010, Jung et al., 2010, Richter et al., 2013). However, coil deviations and variable TMS effects remain present even when on-line visual feedback monitoring of coil position is implemented (Cincotta et al., 2010, Richter et al., 2013).
Here, we propose an offline denoising method that uses information on deviations in coil position and orientation, typically acquired by neuronavigation systems to retrospectively model and remove its impact on TMS effects. Our aim is to remove outcome measure variance explained only by coil position and orientation deviations not resolved even despite the use of online monitoring.
2. METHODS
2.1 Participants
19 healthy adults participated in this study (13M, 6F; age=30±7.1yrs, range=22–48yrs). Subjects provided written informed consent and the study was approved by the NIH Combined Neuroscience IRB. Healthy status was verified prior to study participation via neurological examination and brain MRI performed by trained clinicians.
2.2 TMS and recording
Subjects were seated in an armchair during the study. Monophasic TMS was delivered with a hand-held figure-of-eight coil (oriented approximately 45° relative to the mid-sagittal line) attached to a Magstim 2002 unit (Magstim, Inc). Electromyography (EMG) was recorded at 5kHz using Signal software (CED, Cambridge UK) from the left first dorsal interosseous (FDI) using disposable adhesive electrodes arranged in a belly-tendon montage. The left FDI hotspot was defined as the scalp position most reliably eliciting the largest MEPs following suprathreshold stimulation. The resting motor threshold (RMT) was defined using an adaptive threshold-hunting algorithm (Awiszus, 2003). A total of 600 suprathreshold TMS stimuli (120% RMT; interstimulus interval=5±0.75s) were then delivered to the FDI hotspot.
2.3 Neuronavigation
Frameless stereotactic neuronavigation (Brainsight 2, Rogue Research) was used to localize subject head, FDI hotspot target, and TMS coil position and orientation within the same spatial reference frame. Real-time visual feedback of the TMS coil position and orientation relative to the FDI hotspot target was used to guide hand-held TMS coil positioning over the course of the experimental session. The coil position and orientation deviations (i.e. – spatial and rotational disparity between coil and FDI hotspot target location) were acquired at the time of TMS delivery for each trial (Fig.1).
2.4 Data Analysis
2.4.1 MEP amplitudes
Peak-to-peak motor evoked potentials (MEP) amplitudes were calculated as the difference between the maximum and minimum voltage deflections between 20–40ms post-stimulus. Subjects whose mean MEP amplitudes were within one standard deviation of the grand mean were included in the analysis to avoid outlier-driven model fits (17 of 19 subjects). For a given subject, we excluded trials deemed to have excessive background EMG activity (average pre-stimulus power exceeding 75th percentile + 3*IQR over all trials). Finally, three trials with extraordinarily high x-axis coil deviations (x>35 mm compared with the grand mean, x=-0.28mm) were discovered via manual inspection and discarded.
Each 6-D coil position (x, y and z) and orientation (yaw, pitch and roll) deviation coordinate was first normalized by the maximum value across trials. Next, to increase the density of MEP amplitudes per deviation coordinate value, we independently re-quantized each coordinate into a number of bins by using a brute-force optimization process over a range of possible number of bins (1 to 1000) that maximized detection of significant changes in MEP amplitudes with respect to the bin that included coordinate 0 (no deviation; Sign test, p<0.05, Bonferroni-corrected).
2.4.2 Denoising
The relationship between the coil deviations (relative to the FDI hotspot target) and MEP amplitude was modeled on a trial-by-trial basis using a mixed effects model with random intercepts at the subject level (Model 1): where YTx1is the matrix with all T trials, XTx6 is the design matrix of fixed effects with each row consisting of all 6-D coil deviations for each trial. We used a piecewise linear model centered at 0 to account for directional contributions of coil deviations to MEP amplitudes, represented as the fixed-effect slopes, βneg and βpos. Vector, W, is the individual subject mean MEP with respect to the grand mean MEP, β0. Residuals, ε, are assumed to be Gaussian-distributed with zero-mean and unknown variance.
Since W reflects any between-subjects difference and not just those differences explained by coil deviations, a second piecewise multiple linear regression was used to obtain the fraction of between-subjects differences accounted for by only coil deviations (Model 2): where W* represents the between-subjects difference explained by coil deviations. X* represents the individual subject median coil deviations reduced to K dimensions (full 6-D coil deviation model is too high-dimensional given current sample size, N=17), via singular value decomposition (SVD): where X̄ represents the demeaned subject median 6-D coil deviations across trials. X* is obtained by projecting X̄ onto the linear subspace spanned by the first K singular vectors of V that explained at least 90% of the original variance in X̄ (for these data, K=3). Thus, this procedure reduced dimensionality of the acquired data by half.
Residuals εw are assumed to be normal with zero-mean and unknown variance. Slopes β[wneg, wpos] represent the contribution of individual subject median coil deviations to the between-subject MEP difference at negative and positive values of these angles, relative to the intercept βw0.
The impact of coil deviations on MEP amplitude variability was hierarchically decomposed into terms expressing MEP variability explained by both within-subject/trial-to-trial (lower level) and between-subject (higher level) factors, by substituting w in Eq.1 with w* (Eq.2).
Finally, denoised MEP amplitude estimates, Ŷ, were obtained by subtracting the total fraction of MEP variability explained by coil deviations:
2.4.3 Assessment of denoising
We assess the effect of denoising as percent changes in mean, variance and signal-to-noise ratio from raw (Y) to denoised (Ŷ) MEP amplitudes between all trials of all subjects: and as the median change within each subject s: where F is one of the following functions: variance (Var), mean (Mean) or signal-to-noise ratio (SNR). Ys and Ŷs are the individual subject, s, subsets of raw and denoised MEP amplitudes, respectively.
3. RESULTS
The main result was that offline denoising led to a significant improvement in SNR of corticospinal excitability measurements (Fig.2).
Despite online monitoring, the actual coil position and orientation varied substantially with respect to the target hotspot (range of −4.87–3.89mm and −0.27–0.36rad or −15.42°–20.53°, respectively; Fig.1c). Offline modeling of the impact of these residual coil deviations corrected this source of individual-trial, MEP amplitude measurement error. Specifically, denoising significantly increased within-subject mean MEP amplitudes (ΔMeanWS=24.23%, V=140, p=0.0007) without affecting the variance (ΔVarWS=-1.80%, V=53, p=0.142), overall resulting in a significant improvement in signal-to-noise ratio (ΔSNRWS=26.19%, V=141, p=.0005; Fig.2b).
Across-subjects, denoising decreased MEP amplitude variance over trials (random permutation test 20,000 repetitions, ΔVarall=6.99%, p=0.00005) in the absence of mean or signal-to-noise ratios changes (ΔMeanall=18.28%, p=0.366 and ΔSNRall=22.64%, p=0.271 respectively).
4. DISCUSSION
The main finding of this study was that retrospective modeling of coil position and orientation deviations improved MEP amplitude measurement SNR by 26.19%.
MEP amplitudes are widely used as outcome measures or as a strategy to standardize TMS stimulation intensity across individuals in systems, cognitive and clinical neuroscience (Herbsman et al., 2009, Kaminski et al., 2011). Here, we used coil position and orientation deviation information saved after customary online monitoring to subsequently characterize MEP amplitude variability explained only by uncontrolled coil deviations, and then denoise MEP measurements offline. These data are usually available, but commonly unused in statistical analysis pipelines after data is collected.
The improvement in SNR identified here raises the possibility of substantial enhancement of TMS effect detection through offline, coil deviation-based denoising. Theoretically, this approach could be applied to both previously acquired data and future studies as a means to more accurately measure trial-by-trial motor thresholds and other neurophysiological features (i.e., intracortical inhibition or facilitation, recruitment curves, and transcranial evoked potentials)(Hallett, 2007, Lioumis et al., 2009), or behavioral TMS effects (i.e., single pulse TMS effects on reaction times)(Chen et al., 1998, Johansen-Berg et al., 2002). This approach could also be extended to denoise cumulative effects of repetitive TMS neuromodulation sessions (i.e. – 1Hz rTMS) on neurophysiological measures (Chen et al., 1997) or behavior (Censor et al., 2010). Finally, this methodology is likely to be particularly useful for study designs that include pre‐ and post-intervention measurements, multiple sessions for a single subject, or multiple operators across different centers)(Julkunen et al., 2009, Fleming et al., 2012).
5. CONCLUSIONS
Offline denoising may be a simple and feasible approach to enhance detection of TMS effects in systems neuroscience.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT
None of the authors have potential conflicts of interest to be disclosed.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke.
Footnotes
Author email addresses:
leonardo.claudino{at}nih.gov
sara.hussain{at}nih.gov
ethan.buch{at}nih.gov
cohenl{at}ninds.nih.gov