Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

A Polygenic Score for Higher Educational Attainment is Associated with Larger Brains

View ORCID ProfileMaxwell L. Elliott, View ORCID ProfileDaniel W Belsky, Kevin Anderson, David L. Corcoran, Tian Ge, Annchen Knodt, Joseph A. Prinz, Karen Sugden, Benjamin Williams, David Ireland, Richie Poulton, Avshalom Caspi, Avram Holmes, Terrie Moffitt, Ahmad R Hariri
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/287490
Maxwell L. Elliott
1Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University Box 104410, Durham, NC 27708, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Maxwell L. Elliott
  • For correspondence: maxwell.elliott@duke.edu
Daniel W Belsky
2Department of Population Health Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Box 3003, Durham, NC 27710, USA
3Social Science Research Institute, Duke University, Box 90989, Durham, NC 27708, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Daniel W Belsky
Kevin Anderson
4Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06511, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David L. Corcoran
5Center for Genomic and Computational Biology, Duke University Box 90338, Durham, NC 27708, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Tian Ge
6Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA
7Psychiatric and Neurodevelopmental Genetics Unit, Center for Genomic Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA 02114, USA
8Stanley Center for Psychiatric Research, Broad Institute of MIT and Harvard, Cambridge, MA, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Annchen Knodt
1Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University Box 104410, Durham, NC 27708, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Joseph A. Prinz
5Center for Genomic and Computational Biology, Duke University Box 90338, Durham, NC 27708, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Karen Sugden
3Social Science Research Institute, Duke University, Box 90989, Durham, NC 27708, USA
5Center for Genomic and Computational Biology, Duke University Box 90338, Durham, NC 27708, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Benjamin Williams
3Social Science Research Institute, Duke University, Box 90989, Durham, NC 27708, USA
5Center for Genomic and Computational Biology, Duke University Box 90338, Durham, NC 27708, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David Ireland
9Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department of Psychology, University of Otago, 163 Union St E, Dunedin, 9016, NZ
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Richie Poulton
9Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department of Psychology, University of Otago, 163 Union St E, Dunedin, 9016, NZ
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Avshalom Caspi
1Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University Box 104410, Durham, NC 27708, USA
5Center for Genomic and Computational Biology, Duke University Box 90338, Durham, NC 27708, USA
10Social, Genetic, & Developmental Psychiatry Research Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, & Neuroscience, King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK
11Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, NC 27708, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Avram Holmes
4Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06511, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Terrie Moffitt
1Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University Box 104410, Durham, NC 27708, USA
5Center for Genomic and Computational Biology, Duke University Box 90338, Durham, NC 27708, USA
10Social, Genetic, & Developmental Psychiatry Research Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, & Neuroscience, King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK
11Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, NC 27708, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ahmad R Hariri
1Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University Box 104410, Durham, NC 27708, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Supplementary material
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

People who score higher on intelligence tests tend to have larger brains. Twin studies suggest the same genetic factors influence both brain size and intelligence. This has led to the hypothesis that genetics influence intelligence partly by contributing to development of larger brains. We tested this hypothesis with molecular genetic data using discoveries from a genome-wide association study (GWAS) of educational attainment, a correlate of intelligence. We analyzed genetic, brain imaging, and cognitive test data from the UK Biobank, the Dunedin Study, the Brain Genomics Superstruct Project (GSP), and the Duke Neurogenetics Study (DNS) (combined N=8,271). We measured genetics using polygenic scores based on published GWAS. We conducted meta-analysis to test associations among participants’ genetics, total brain volume (i.e., brain size), and cognitive test performance. Consistent with previous findings, participants with higher polygenic scores achieved higher scores on cognitive tests, as did participants with larger brains. Participants with higher polygenic scores also had larger brains. We found some evidence that brain size partly mediated associations between participants’ education polygenic scores and their cognitive test performance. Effect-sizes were larger in the population-based UK Biobank and Dunedin samples than in the GSP and DNS samples. Sensitivity analysis suggested this effect-size difference partly reflected restricted range of cognitive performance in the GSP and DNS samples. Recruitment and retention of population-representative samples should be a priority for neuroscience research. Findings suggest promise for studies integrating GWAS discoveries with brain imaging data to understand neurobiology linking genetics with individual differences in cognitive performance.

Introduction

People who score higher on tests of intelligence tend to have larger brains, as measured by ex-vivo brain weight and in-vivo magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)1–4. Twin studies indicate this relationship partly reflects genetic factors that influence both brain size (i.e., volume) and intelligence5–8. These findings suggest the hypothesis that one path through which genetic differences between people influence individual differences in intelligence is by contributing to the development of larger brains. This hypothesis can now be tested using molecular genetic data.

A recent genome-wide association study (GWAS) of educational attainment identified dozens of genetic variants that showed substantial enrichment for genes expressed during brain development9. Follow-up studies further identified associations between an aggregate measure of GWAS-discovered influences on education, called a polygenic score, and intelligence, including in young children who had not yet entered school10,11. These findings implicate brain development and intelligence in the pathway connecting people’s genetics to their educational outcomes. Further, GWAS research has discovered polygenic variants associated with brain size (inferred through intracranial volume)12 that also overlap with variants associated educational attainment9. Now, studies are needed to test if genetics discovered in GWAS of education are associated with in-vivo intermediate phenotypes, like brain size, that could constitute a biological pathway linking genetic variation to differences in intelligence and educational attainment.

We analyzed data from four imaging genetics studies from the United Kingdom (UK Biobank), New Zealand (Dunedin Study), and the United States (Brain Genomics Superstruct Project (GSP) and Duke Neurogenetics Study), including 8,271 participants, to test associations among a polygenic score for educational attainment, cognitive test performance, and brain size. We hypothesized that, consistent with previous findings, (1) participants with higher education polygenic scores would have higher cognitive test scores; and (2) that participants with larger brains as measured by total brain volume would have higher cognitive test scores. We further posed the novel hypotheses that (3) participants with higher education polygenic scores would have larger brains and that brain size would mediate the association between the education polygenic score and cognitive test performance. We combined results across our four imaging genetics datasets using random-effects meta-analysis. We also examined heterogeneity between the datasets under the hypothesis that effect-sizes might differ between the population-based UK Biobank and Dunedin Study samples and the GSP and DNS samples, for which range in cognitive performance is more restricted.

Methods

Participants

We analyzed data from European-descent participants in the United Kingdom-based UK Biobank13,14 a population-based volunteer sample (N=6117), the New Zealand-based Dunedin Study, a birth cohort (N=476)15, and two studies in the United States consisting primarily of university students, the Brain Genomics Superstruct Project16 (GSP, N=1163), and the Duke Neurogenetics Study17 (DNS, N=515). Sample sizes reflect participants with available structural MRI, cognitive testing, and genetic data (Table 1). Samples are described in detail in the supplement.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1. Samples and measures included in analysis.

Polygenic scores for all samples were computed based on the most recent GWAS of educational attainment9 following established methods.

Education Polygenic Score

We computed our polygenic score based on GWAS of educational attainment rather than GWAS of cognitive performance because educational attainment is a proxy phenotype for cognitive performance18 and the polygenic score for educational attainment is more predictive of cognitive performance than polygenic scores from GWAS of cognitive performance19. Education polygenic scores were computed from genome-wide single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) data based on GWAS results published by the Social Science Genetics Association Consortium9 following methods described by Dudbridge20 according to the procedure used in our previous work10. Briefly, for each study, we matched SNPs in the study’s genetic database with published educational attainment GWAS results9. We then multiplied the education-associated allele of each SNP by the GWAS-estimated effect-size and computed the average of these products across all SNPs. Polygenic scores were standardized within each study to have M=0, SD=1 for analysis.

Cognitive Performance

Cognitive performance was measured in the UK Biobank using 13 reason and logic puzzles21 and in the Dunedin Study, GSP, and DNS studies using intelligence tests (the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV)22 in the Dunedin Study, the Shipley Institute of Living Scale23 in GSP and the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI)24 in the DNS).

Total Brain Volume

Total brain volume was measured from high resolution, T1-weighted MRI images. In the UK Biobank total brain volume was estimated using SIENAX25. In the Dunedin Study, GSP, and DNS studies, images were processed using the Freesurfer processing pipeline26.

Statistical Analyses

We tested associations using linear regression models. Models were adjusted for sex. Models including the polygenic score were adjusted for the first 10 principal components estimated from the genome-wide SNP data to account for any residual population stratification within the European-descent samples analyzed27. Models of UK biobank and GSP data were adjusted for age. (The Dunedin Study is a single-year birth cohort and DNS participants vary in age by only by 1-2 years.). In addition to age, models in the GSP were also adjusted for scanner, console version and head coil (12 versus 32 channel) because the GSP was collected across multiple sites. Analyses of individual studies were conducted in R (version 3.4.0). Linear regressions were performed using the lm function. Mediation analyses were performed using a system of equations approach28 implemented with the mediation package29 in R, using nonparametric bootstrapping with 1000 iterations. We combined estimates across studies using random effects meta-analysis30 implemented using STATA (version 15).

Results

Participants with higher polygenic scores performed better on cognitive tests

As anticipated, participants with higher polygenic scores performed better on cognitive tests. Meta-analysis estimated the cross-study effect size as r=.18 (p<.001; 95% CI [.12, .24]) with evidence of heterogeneity in effect sizes across studies (I-squared 80%, p=.002). Effect sizes were statistically significant in UK Biobank (r=.20, p<.001), Dunedin Study (r=.28, p<.001) and GSP (r=.19, p<.001) but not in the DNS (r=.05, p=.220).

Participants with larger brains had higher cognitive test scores

We next tested if participants with larger brains performed better on cognitive tests. As anticipated, participants with larger brains (i.e., those with higher total brain volume) performed better on cognitive tests. Meta-analysis estimated the cross-study effect size as r=.20 (p<.001; 95% CI [.12, .28]) with evidence of heterogeneity in effect sizes across studies (I-squared=75.8%, p=.006). Effect-sizes were statistically significant in all studies (UK Biobank r=.21, p<.001; Dunedin Study r=.35, p<.001; GSP r=.12, p=.002; DNS r=.16, p=.004).

Participants with higher polygenic scores for educational attainment had larger brains in two samples

Finally, we tested if participants with higher polygenic scores tended to have larger brains. Meta-analysis estimated the cross-study effect-size as r=.05 (p=.002; 95% CI [.02, .09]). The test for evidence of heterogeneity in effect sizes across studies was not statistically significant at the alpha=.05 level (I-squared=51.9%, p=.101). Participants with higher polygenic scores had larger brains in the UK Biobank (r=.08, p<.001) and the Dunedin Study (r=.08, p=.033). Effect-sizes were smaller and not statistically significant in the GSP r=.02, p=.380 and DNS r=.04, p=.288.

Brain size was a weak mediator of the polygenic-score associations with cognitive test scores in two study samples

To test the hypothesis that larger brains mediated the polygenic score association with intelligence, we used the system of equations described by Baron and Kenny31 and the methods described by Preacher et al.28 Meta-analysis estimated the cross-study indirect effect to be b=.01, 95% CI [.00, .02], p=.055, with evidence of heterogeneity in effect sizes across studies (I-squared=79.5%, p=.002). The mediation effect was statistically significant in the UK Biobank (b=.01, 95% CI [.01, .02], p < .001) and the Dunedin Study (b=.02, 95% CI [.00, .05], p=.042). We did not find evidence of a mediation effect in the GSP b=.00, 95% CI [.00, .01], p=.36) or DNS b=.01, 95% CI [-.00, .02], p=.24.

Sensitivity analysis: Associations among polygenic scores, brain size, and cognitive test scores were attenuated in a sample of UK Biobank participants restricted to those with cognitive test scores 1 SD above the sample mean

UK Biobank and Dunedin Study participants’ polygenic scores, brain size, and cognitive test performance were positively correlated, with similar effect-sizes (Dunedin-study effect-sizes for analyses including IQ were somewhat larger, possibly reflecting greater measurement precision of the WAIS as compared to the UK Biobank reason-and-logic-puzzle test). By comparison, effect-sizes for these associations were smaller among GSP and DNS participants. To test if this difference could reflect the relatively restricted range of cognitive test performance in the GSP and DNS samples relative to the population-based UK Biobank and Dunedin samples, we conducted sensitivity analysis.

Cognitive test scores were on average, 1-1.5 SDs higher in the GSP and DNS samples as compared to the general population and 30-50% less variable, indicating restricted range (Table 1). Sensitivity analysis restricted the UK Biobank sample to participants with cognitive test scores 1 SD above the full-sample mean (i.e. scores of 9-13; n=1,401), for which the variance was approximately 45% of the full-sample variance. In the restricted-sample sensitivity analysis, associations among participants’ polygenic scores, brain size, and cognitive test performance were attenuated by roughly 1/3 to 1/2 relative to the full-sample estimates (Supplemental Table S3).

Discussion

We analyzed data from four imaging genetics studies in the UK, NZ, and US to test if genetic associations with cognitive performance were mediated by differences in brain size. As anticipated, we found that participants with higher educational-attainment polygenic scores tended to score higher on tests of cognitive performance, as did those with larger brains. We also found new information, that participants with higher education polygenic scores tended to have larger brains. In mediation analysis, brain size accounted for only a small fraction of the association between participants’ educational attainment polygenic scores and their cognitive performance, and this mediation effect was statistically significant in the population-based UK Biobank and Dunedin samples, but not in the GSP and DNS samples.

Effect-size variation across the samples we analyzed followed a consistent pattern; effect-sizes were larger in the population-based UK Biobank and Dunedin Study samples than in the GSP and DNS samples. One reason for these differences may be the more restricted range of variation in cognitive performance in the GSP and DNS samples arising from, e.g. overrepresentation of university educated individuals. Such range restriction biases association estimates32,33 and has previously been shown to bias brain imaging research34,35. In the these relatively high IQ and restricted range samples, average cognitive performance was 1-1.5 standard deviations above the general-population mean and the variance was reduced by 30-50%. We conducted sensitivity analysis in a UK Biobank subsample selected to have high cognitive performance similar to the GSP and DNS samples. In this sample with restricted range of cognitive test performance, effect-sizes were attenuated by roughly 30-50%. Selective observation of high-cognitive-performance individuals in the GSP and DNS samples may have contributed to the lower effect-size estimates in these samples and to overall heterogeneity across samples in our meta-analysis.

We acknowledge limitations of our current analyses, which can be addressed in future research. First, analyses were restricted to European-descent participants. We focused on European-descent participants to match the population studied in the GWAS of educational attainment8. Application of GWAS results from European-descent samples to compute polygenic scores for samples of different ancestry has uncertain validity36. As GWAS of education and related phenotypes in non-European samples become available, replication in additional populations will be needed. Second, polygenic scores were measured with substantial error. Genetic effect-sizes thus represent lower-bound estimates. As larger-sample GWAS become available, error in polygenic score measurement will decline and effect-sizes can be expected to increase37. Third, total brain volume is only one route through which the genetics linked with educational attainment could affect cognitive performance. We studied this specific phenotype because it is the best-replicated neural correlate of cognitive function2. As more refined neural phenotypes of cognitive function are developed, including measures of cortical thickness, surface area, gyrification, and brain function, it will be important to test their potential mediating role in linking genetics with cognitive performance. Finally, we cannot rule out age differences as a potential explanation for the difference in findings between the population-based UK Biobank and Dunedin Study samples as compared to the GSP and DNS samples. UK Biobank and Dunedin Study participants were measured in midlife, whereas GSP and DNS samples primarily included young adults. Among midlife UK Biobank participants, restricting the range of cognitive performance to be similar to the GSP and DNS samples reduced effect-sizes for associations among polygenic scores, brain size, and cognitive test performance. Population-based samples including both young and midlife individuals with DNA, MRI, and cognitive testing are needed to evaluate whether genetic associations with brain volume and cognitive performance vary with age. A final concern is potential reverse causation between brain size and cognitive function. Higher cognitive ability and related educational and socioeconomic attainments may be protective of age-related decline in brain volume. Longitudinal studies with repeated measures of brain volume and cognition are needed to establish causal direction.

Within the bounds of these limitations, our findings contribute to evidence that genetics discovered in GWAS of educational attainment influence brain development and cognitive function. Bioinformatic analysis of education GWAS results have identified enrichment of variants near genes expressed in brain development, specifically neural proliferation, neural development, and dendrite formation9. Epidemiologic analysis of an education-GWAS-based polygenic score found that children who carried more education-associated genetic variants scored higher on cognitive tests as early as age 5 and that polygenic-score-associated differences in cognitive test scores grew larger from middle childhood through adolescence10. Several studies have reported that an education-GWAS-based polygenic score is predictive of cognitive test performance in adolescents and adults11,19,38. Here, we show that adults with higher education-GWAS-based polygenic scores have larger brains and score higher on cognitive tests as compared to peers with lower polygenic scores. Evidence for larger brains as a statistical mediator of polygenic score associations with cognitive performance was mixed in our analysis. But findings suggest promise for future neuroscientific investigation of education-linked genetics. One design to complement formal mediation analysis is gene-environment interaction analysis to test if exposures that slow brain growth or restrict brain size, e.g., Zika virus39, diminish associations between genetics and cognitive performance.

Our finding that genetics associated with educational and socioeconomic attainments are also related to brain volume has implications for research on effects of poverty on the developing brain. Childhood poverty exposure is associated with smaller brain volumes40,41. Education polygenic scores also tend to be lower in children growing up in poorer families, a gene-environment correlation that presumably reflects effects of education-linked genetics on parents economic attainments, which children inherit along with their genotypes10,30. Studies that include controls for education genetics could complement intervention studies42 to help rule out potential confounding in associations between poverty and brain development.

A challenge facing research on how genetics affect the brain is the lack of population-representative samples with available brain imaging data. Human brain-imaging research has typically been conducted in samples similar to those in the GSP and DNS whose data we analyzed43,44. Our findings illustrate how studies of samples pre-selected for high levels of cognitive functioning and related characteristics impose limitations on analysis of cognition-related neurobiology. Opportunities to understand the brain afforded by 21st Century measurement technologies must still reckon with 20th Century discoveries about selection bias45,46. Efforts to recruit more representative samples that reflect the full range of cognitive functioning in the population are needed.

Individual differences in cognitive performance have a partial genetic etiology19,47. This genetic etiology should be evident in individual differences in brain biology. As GWAS discoveries for intelligence and related traits clarify genetic etiology, follow-up in genetically-informed brain imaging studies can shed light on the neurobiological correlates of this genetic variation. Our findings encourage enthusiasm for this research, but also highlight limitations of existing data resources. Recruiting and retaining samples that are representative of the general population must be a priority in neuroscience research.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 1. Educational attainment polygenic score associations with cognitive test scores.

The figure shows a graph of effect-sizes for analyses of the UK Biobank, Dunedin Study (Dunedin), Brain Genomics Superstruct Project (GSP) and Duke Neurogenetics Study (DNS) samples (solid blue diamonds) and the cross-study effect-size estimated from random-effects meta-analysis (open blue diamond). Gray boxes around the solid-blue diamonds show the weighting of study-specific estimates in the meta-analysis (larger gray boxes indicate higher weights). 95% CIs for estimates are shown as error bars for the study-specific estimates and as the left- and right-extremes of the diamond for the meta-analysis effect-size. The meta-analysis estimate of between-study heterogeneity (I-squared) is listed to the left of the open blue diamond showing the meta-analysis effect-size. The table to the right of the effect-size graph reports values for effect-sizes, 95% CIs, and meta-analysis weights.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 2. Associations between brain size and cognitive test scores.

The figure shows a graph of effect-sizes for analyses of the UK Biobank, Dunedin Study (Dunedin), Brain Genomics Superstruct Project (GSP) and Duke Neurogenetics Study (DNS) samples (solid blue diamonds) and the cross-study effect-size estimated from random-effects meta-analysis (open blue diamond). Gray boxes around the solid-blue diamonds show the weighting of study-specific estimates in the meta-analysis (larger gray boxes indicate higher weights). 95% CIs for estimates are shown as error bars for the study-specific estimates and as the left- and right-extremes of the diamond for the meta-analysis effect-size. The meta-analysis estimate of between-study heterogeneity (I-squared) is listed to the left of the open blue diamond showing the meta-analysis effect-size. The table to the right of the effect-size graph reports values for effect-sizes, 95% CIs, and meta-analysis weights.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 3. Educational attainment polygenic score associations with brain size.

The figure shows a graph of effect-sizes for analyses of the UK Biobank, Dunedin Study (Dunedin), Brain Genomics Superstruct Project (GSP) and Duke Neurogenetics Study (DNS) samples (solid blue diamonds) and the cross-study effect-size estimated from random-effects meta-analysis (open blue diamond). Gray boxes around the solid-blue diamonds show the weighting of study-specific estimates in the meta-analysis (larger gray boxes indicate higher weights). 95% CIs for estimates are shown as error bars for the study-specific estimates and as the left- and right-extremes of the diamond for the meta-analysis effect-size. The meta-analysis estimate of between-study heterogeneity (I-squared) is listed to the left of the open blue diamond showing the meta-analysis effect-size. The table to the right of the effect-size graph reports values for effect-sizes, 95% CIs, and meta-analysis weights.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 4. Mediation effect of brain size on the association between the polygenic score for educational attainment and cognitive test scores.

The figure shows a graph of effect-sizes for analyses of the UK Biobank, Dunedin Study (Dunedin), Brain Genomics Superstruct Project (GSP) and Duke Neurogenetics Study (DNS) samples (solid blue diamonds) and the cross-study effect-size estimated from random-effects meta-analysis (open blue diamond). Gray boxes around the solid-blue diamonds show the weighting of study-specific estimates in the meta-analysis (larger gray boxes indicate higher weights). 95% CIs for estimates are shown as error bars for the study-specific estimates and as the left- and right-extremes of the diamond for the meta-analysis effect-size. The meta-analysis estimate of between-study heterogeneity (I-squared) is listed to the left of the open blue diamond showing the meta-analysis effect-size. The table to the right of the effect-size graph reports values for effect-sizes, 95% CIs, and meta-analysis weights.

Acknowledgement

This research was conducted using the UK Biobank Resource (project ID #28174). The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study is supported by the NZ HRC, NZ MBIE, National Institute on Aging grant R01AG032282, R01AG049789, and UK Medical Research Council grant MR/P005918/1. The Duke Neurogenetics Study received support from Duke University as well as US-National Institutes of Health grants R01DA033369 and R01DA031579. MLE is supported by the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship under Grant No. NSF DGE-1644868. DWB is supported by an early-career fellowship from the Jacobs Foundation. TG is supported by National Institutes of Health grant K99AG054573. AJH is supported by National Institute of Mental Health grant K01MH099232. The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

  1. ↵
    van Valen L. Brain size and intelligence in man. Am J Phys Anthropol 1974; 40: 417–423.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  2. ↵
    Pietschnig J, Penke L, Wicherts JM, Zeiler M, Voracek M. Meta-analysis of associations between human brain volume and intelligence differences: How strong are they and what do they mean? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 57: 411–432.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. Haier RJ, Jung RE, Yeo RA, Head K, Alkire MT. Structural brain variation and general intelligence. Neuroimage 2004; 23: 425–433.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  4. ↵
    McDaniel MA. Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence. Intelligence 2005; 33: 337–346.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  5. ↵
    Posthuma D, Baaré WFC, Pol HEH, Kahn RS, Boomsma DI, Geus EJ C De. Genetic Correlations Between Brain Volumes and the WAIS-III Dimensions of Verbal Comprehension, Working Memory, Perceptual Organization, and Processing Speed. Twin Res Hum Genet 2003; 6: 131–139.
    OpenUrl
  6. Toga AW, Thompson PM. Genetics of Brain Structure and Intelligence. Annu Rev Neurosci 2005; 28: 1–23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  7. Deary IJ, Penke L, Johnson W. The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nat Rev Neurosci 2010; 11. doi:10.1038/nrn2793.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  8. ↵
    Posthuma D, De Geus EJC, Baaré WFC, Hulshoff Pol HE, Kahn RS, Boomsma DI. The association between brain volume and intelligence is of genetic origin. Nat Neurosci 2002; 5: 83–84.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  9. ↵
    Okbay A, Beauchamp JP, Fontana MA, Lee JJ, Pers TH, Rietveld CA et al. Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature 2016; 533: 539–542.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    Belsky DW, Moffitt TE, Corcoran DL, Domingue B, Harrington H, Hogan S et al. The Genetics of Success: How Single-Nucleotide Polymorphisms Associated With Educational Attainment Relate to Life-Course Development. Psychol Sci 2016; 27: 957–972.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    Selzam S, Krapohl E, von Stumm S, O’Reilly PF, Rimfeld K, Kovas Y et al. Predicting educational achievement from DNA. Mol Psychiatry 2017; 22: 267–272.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  12. ↵
    Adams HHH, Hibar DP, Chouraki V, Stein JL, Nyquist PA, Rentería ME et al. Novel genetic loci underlying human intracranial volume identified through genome-wide association. Nat Neurosci 2016; 19: 1569–1582.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  13. ↵
    Sudlow C, Gallacher J, Allen N, Beral V, Burton P, Danesh J et al. UK Biobank: An Open Access Resource for Identifying the Causes of a Wide Range of Complex Diseases of Middle and Old Age. PLoS Med 2015; 12. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1001779.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    Miller KL, Alfaro-Almagro F, Bangerter NK, Thomas DL, Yacoub E, Xu J et al. Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nat Neurosci 2016; 19: 1523.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    Poulton R, Moffitt TE, Silva PA. The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study: overview of the first 40 years, with an eye to the future. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2015; 50: 679–693.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Holmes AJ, Hollinshead MO, O’Keefe TM, Petrov VI, Fariello GR, Wald LL et al. Brain Genomics Superstruct Project initial data release with structural, functional, and behavioral measures. Sci Data 2015; 2. doi:10.1038/sdata.2015.31.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Romer AL, Knodt AR, Houts R, Brigidi BD, Moffitt TE, Caspi A et al. Structural alterations within cerebellar circuitry are associated with general liability for common mental disorders. Mol Psychiatry 2017;: 1–7.
  18. ↵
    Rietveld CA, Esko T, Davies G, Pers TH, Turley P, Benyamin B et al. Common genetic variants associated with cognitive performance identified using the proxy-phenotype method. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2014; 111: 13790–13794.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    Plomin R, von Stumm S. The new genetics of intelligence. Nat Rev Genet 2018. doi:10.1038/nrg.2017.104.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  20. ↵
    Dudbridge F. Power and Predictive Accuracy of Polygenic Risk Scores. PLOS Genet 2013; 9: e1003348.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Lyall DM, Cullen B, Allerhand M, Smith DJ, Mackay D, Evans J et al. Cognitive test scores in UK biobank: Data reduction in 480,416 participants and longitudinal stability in 20,346 participants. PLoS One 2016; 11. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0154222.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  22. ↵
    Wechsler D. WAIS-III administration and scoring manual. 1997 doi:10.1177/1073191102009001003.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  23. ↵
    Zachary RA. Shipley Institute of Living Scale: Revised Manual. Los Angeles East Psychol Serv 1986.
  24. ↵
    Wechsler D. WASI -II: Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence - second edition. J Psychoeduc Assess 2013; 31: 337–41.
    OpenUrl
  25. ↵
    Smith SM, Zhang Y, Jenkinson M, Chen J, Matthews PM, Federico A et al. Accurate, Robust, and Automated Longitudinal and Cross-Sectional Brain Change Analysis. Neuroimage 2002; 17: 479–489.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  26. ↵
    Fischl B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage. 2012; 62: 774–781.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  27. ↵
    Price AL, N.j. patterson, R.m. plenge, M.e. weinblatt, N.a. shadick. Principal components analysis corrects for stratification in genome-wide association studies. Nat Genet 2006; 38: 904–909.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  28. ↵
    Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods 2008; 40: 879–91.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  29. ↵
    Tingley D, Yamamoto T, Hirose K, Keele L, Imai K. mediation: R Package for Causal Mediation Analysis. J Stat Softw 2014; 59: 1–38.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials 1986; 7: 177–88.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  31. ↵
    Baron RM, Kenny DA. The Moderator Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological-Research - Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986; 51: 1173–1182.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  32. ↵
    Mendoza JL, Mumford M. Corrections for Attenuation and Range Restriction on the Predictor. J Educ Stat 1987; 12: 282–293.
    OpenUrl
  33. ↵
    Bland JM, Altman DG. Statistics notes: Correlation in restricted ranges of data. BMJ 2011; 343: 577.
    OpenUrl
  34. ↵
    Falk EB, Hyde LW, Mitchell C, Faul J, Gonzalez R, Heitzeg MM et al. What is a representative brain? Neuroscience meets population science. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2013; 110: 17615–17622.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    Lewinn KZ, Sheridan MA, Keyes KM, Hamilton A, McLaughlin KA. Sample composition alters associations between age and brain structure. Nat Commun 2017; 8. doi:10.1038/s41467-017-00908-7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    Martin AR, Gignoux CR, Walters RK, Wojcik GL, Neale BM, Gravel S et al. Human Demographic History Impacts Genetic Risk Prediction across Diverse Populations. Am J Hum Genet 2017; 100: 635–649.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  37. ↵
    Cesarini D, Visscher PM. Genetics and educational attainment. npj Sci Learn 2017; 2: 4.
    OpenUrl
  38. ↵
    Domingue BW, Belsky DW, Conley D, Harris KM, Boardman JD. Polygenic Influence on Educational Attainment: New Evidence From the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health. AERA Open 2015; 1: 2332858415599972.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  39. ↵
    Calvet G, Aguiar RS, Melo ASO, Sampaio SA, de Filippis I, Fabri A et al. Detection and sequencing of Zika virus from amniotic fluid of fetuses with microcephaly in Brazil: a case study. Lancet Infect Dis 2016; 16: 653–660.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Luby J, Belden A, Botteron K, Marrus N, Harms MP, Babb C et al. The Effects of Poverty on Childhood Brain Development. JAMA Pediatr 2013; 167: 1135.
    OpenUrl
  41. ↵
    Hair NL, Hanson JL, Wolfe BL, Pollak SD. Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatr 2015; 169: 822–829.
    OpenUrl
  42. ↵
    Brody GH, Gray JC, Yu T, Barton AW, Beach SRH, Galvan A et al. Protective prevention effects on the association of poverty with brain development. JAMA Pediatr 2017; 171: 46–52.
    OpenUrl
  43. ↵
    Sears DO. College Sophomores in the Laboratory. Influences of a Narrow Data Base on Social Psychology’s View of Human Nature. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986; 51: 515–530.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  44. ↵
    Peterson RA, Merunka DR. Convenience samples of college students and research reproducibility. J Bus Res 2014; 67: 1035–1041.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  45. ↵
    Berkson J. Limitations of the application of fourfold table analysis to hospital data. Biometrics 1946; 2: 27–53.
    OpenUrl
  46. ↵
    Heckman JJ. Sample Selection Bias as a Specification Error. Econometrica 1979; 47: 153.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  47. ↵
    Plomin R, Deary IJ. Genetics and intelligence differences: five special findings. Mol Psychiatry 2015; 20: 98–108.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted March 23, 2018.
Download PDF

Supplementary Material

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
A Polygenic Score for Higher Educational Attainment is Associated with Larger Brains
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
A Polygenic Score for Higher Educational Attainment is Associated with Larger Brains
Maxwell L. Elliott, Daniel W Belsky, Kevin Anderson, David L. Corcoran, Tian Ge, Annchen Knodt, Joseph A. Prinz, Karen Sugden, Benjamin Williams, David Ireland, Richie Poulton, Avshalom Caspi, Avram Holmes, Terrie Moffitt, Ahmad R Hariri
bioRxiv 287490; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/287490
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo LinkedIn logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
A Polygenic Score for Higher Educational Attainment is Associated with Larger Brains
Maxwell L. Elliott, Daniel W Belsky, Kevin Anderson, David L. Corcoran, Tian Ge, Annchen Knodt, Joseph A. Prinz, Karen Sugden, Benjamin Williams, David Ireland, Richie Poulton, Avshalom Caspi, Avram Holmes, Terrie Moffitt, Ahmad R Hariri
bioRxiv 287490; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/287490

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Neuroscience
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (4234)
  • Biochemistry (9128)
  • Bioengineering (6774)
  • Bioinformatics (23989)
  • Biophysics (12117)
  • Cancer Biology (9523)
  • Cell Biology (13773)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (7627)
  • Ecology (11686)
  • Epidemiology (2066)
  • Evolutionary Biology (15506)
  • Genetics (10638)
  • Genomics (14322)
  • Immunology (9479)
  • Microbiology (22832)
  • Molecular Biology (9089)
  • Neuroscience (48987)
  • Paleontology (355)
  • Pathology (1480)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (2568)
  • Physiology (3844)
  • Plant Biology (8327)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (1471)
  • Synthetic Biology (2296)
  • Systems Biology (6187)
  • Zoology (1300)