Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

Dogs, but not wolves, lose their sensitivity towards novelty with age

View ORCID ProfileChristina Hansen Wheat, View ORCID ProfileWouter van der Bijl, Hans Temrin
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/405480
Christina Hansen Wheat
Department of Zoology, Stockholm University, SE-10691 Stockholm, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Christina Hansen Wheat
Wouter van der Bijl
Department of Zoology, Stockholm University, SE-10691 Stockholm, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Wouter van der Bijl
Hans Temrin
Department of Zoology, Stockholm University, SE-10691 Stockholm, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

Selection on behavioural traits holds a prominent role in the domestication of animals. Specifically, a reduction of the fear response is considered a key component, with domesticated animals expressing lower levels of fear towards novelty than their wild counterparts. Previous work has suggested that this is caused by a delay in the onset of fearful behaviour during early ontogeny in domesticated canids. However, it remains unclear how the developmental timing of initial fear expression affects fearfulness later in development. Here we present the first extended examination of the development of fear behaviour in wolves and dogs, using repeated novel object tests between six and 26 weeks of age. Contrary to expectations, fear of novelty did not change in wolves with age, but dogs expressed decreased latency to approach a novel object with age, resulting in a species difference at the end of the measured period. Our results thereby suggest that differences in fear of novelty between wolves and dogs are not caused by a domestication driven shift in the first onset of fear response. Instead we suggest that differences in fear expression between wolves and dogs are caused by a loss of sensitivity towards novelty with age in dogs.

Introduction

Abundant evidence demonstrates how domesticated plants and animals express dramatically altered phenotypes compared to their wild counterparts (1). For animals, it is now clear that selection on behavioural traits alone had a prominent role in domestication (2-4). Specifically, it has been demonstrated how selection upon decreased fearfulness and aggression can lead to the myriad of morphological and physiological alterations observed in domesticated animals (2,5). In wild populations, fear is a key behaviour, as a timely and proper response to novelty, e.g. flight response versus exploration, can have direct and large fitness consequences (6,7). Appropriate fear responses are formed and modified throughout ontogeny, during which juvenile animals gradually combine individual experience and social information, thereby developing the ability to discriminate between threatening and neutral stimuli (7-9). Domesticated animals express reduced reactivity towards novel stimuli (10) and it is thus likely that altered selection pressures caused by domestication have modified the ontogeny of fear related behaviours.

Ontogeny has been modified in several ways during domestication. Compared to ancestral species, domesticated animals express altered developmental rates, a phenomenon known as heterochrony (11-13), which have resulted in accelerated and/or delayed onsets of various ontogenetic stages, such as earlier sexual maturation and the retention of juvenile traits into adulthood (12,14-16). Heterochrony has been suggested to affect behavioural ontogeny in domesticated animals by prolonging the sensitive period (2,17-19), an important period during behavioural development in which the juvenile animal is particularly sensitive to imprint on and form social bonds with conspecifics (8,20-22). During the sensitive period juvenile animals show increased exploratory behaviour, as they readily approach novel stimuli and thereby learn about and socialize with their environment (23). Importantly, in the context of the ontogeny of fear, the end of the sensitive period manifests by progressive fear-related avoidance behaviour expressed as increased fear and decreased exploration of novelty (2,21). A shift in the sensitive period caused by domestication has been demonstrated in a long-term selection study, in which juvenile foxes (Vulpes vulpes) from domesticated strains expressed a delayed onset of fearful response, at 60-65 days of age, compared to the onset in non-domesticated fox kits, at 40-45 days of age (2,5). While these findings suggest that the basis for the quantifiable difference in fear between domesticated and non-domesticated animals might arise already during early ontogeny, no study has investigated the continued ontogenetic trajectory of fear behaviour after the initial onset of the fearful response.

The domestic dog (Canis familiaris) is an excellent study species when addressing questions about how domestication has affected the ontogeny of behaviour. Domestication of the dog from the grey wolf (Canis lupus) occurred at least 15,000 years ago (1), making the dog the first domesticated species and with the ancestral species extant, the opportunities for comparisons are ideal (24). Studies of behavioural ontogeny in dogs have focused on the sensitive period, and the fear of novelty is reported to manifest and continually increase in the dog puppy from eight weeks of age onward (8,21,25,26). In wolves, consensus on when fear behaviour is established is lacking, with the onset of fearful response reported to occur as varied as four to eight weeks of age across studies (25,27-31). However, the majority of these wolf studies were conducted over a short period of time and/or focused on isolated individuals or single litters, limiting our ability to generalize from these findings. Additionally, a recent study found that juvenile wolves explored novel objects more than dogs at both six and eight weeks of age (32), thereby suggesting that that wolves might not express fear towards novelty at an earlier age than dogs. Thus, while adult wolves (33) and wolf-dog hybrids (34) are more fearful of novelty than dogs, when this difference is established during ontogeny is not well resolved. Therefore, extended studies of the continued development of fear after the onset of fearful response is needed to further understand domestication driven changes in behavioural ontogeny in canids.

To investigate if behavioural differences in fear towards novelty differ between wolves and dogs during the first six months of life, we here examine their behavioural development by using repeated novel object tests. The novel object test is an established method to quantify fear and exploration of novelty and has been used on numerous species (32,33,35-37) We tested three litters of wolves (N = 13) and two litters of dogs (N = 12), hand-raised under similar conditions at six, 10, 14, 18, 22 and 26 weeks of age, i.e. before sexual maturity, and varied the context by using a new novel object in each of the six tests. Our overall goal was to test the hypothesis that the sensitive period is prolonged in dogs, resulting in a delayed onset of fearful response in dogs compared to wolves. Based on studies reporting delayed onset of fear behaviour in domestic compared to ancestral species (2,38), including dogs and wolves (22,31), we expected wolves to be more fearful compared to dogs already at six weeks or, alternatively, at ten weeks of age. Furthermore, we wished to investigate how different timing of the onset of fearful response affects the continued behavioural development of fear in wolves and dogs. We predicted that domestication has lowered the sensitivity to novelty in dogs (33,39), and dogs therefore would express decreased fear towards the novel object compared to wolves throughout the testing period.

Methods

a) Study animals

During 2014 – 2016 two litters of Alaskan huskies (N = 12) and three litters of European grey wolves (N = 13) were hand-raised and extensively socialized under similar conditions from the age of 10 days at Stockholm University’s Zoological Research Station, Tovetorp. This set-up was chosen to minimize environmental bias, including maternal effects, which is well-documented to affect the development of behavioural patterns (40-42). Puppies were raised within litters and socialization involved 24-hour presence of human caregivers for the first two months. From two months of age, caregiver presence was decreased with a few hours a day until three months of age and then further decreased during every other night at four months of age. At six months of age, caregivers spent four to six hours with the puppies a day. All wolf and dog litters were kept separate, but reared under standardized conditions. From the age of 10 days to five weeks, puppies were reared in identical indoor rooms and here after given access to smaller roofed outdoor enclosures. After a week of habituation to the roofed outdoor enclosure, puppies were given access to a larger fenced grass enclosure at six weeks of age. Hereafter the puppies had free access to all three enclosures during the day and access to the indoor room and the roofed enclosure during the night. When the puppies where three months old they were moved to large outdoor enclosures (2,000 square meters), in which they remained for the rest of the study period. We started behavioural observations at 10 days of age and behavioural testing was initiated at 6 weeks of age. Testing procedures and exposure to the new environments were standardized over all three years. As required by Swedish law, all hand-raisers were ethically certified and trained to handle animals. Furthermore, rules were implemented to assure that rearing was standardized across all caregivers. This included that puppies were never disciplined, trained or forced to have contact with their caregivers. From the age of eight weeks, puppies were gradually exposed to strangers through the fence with the support of one or more human caregivers.

The dog litter from 2014 consisted of five males and one female and the 2015 litter of three males and three females. The two dog litters were unrelated. The wolf litters from 2014, three females and two males, and 2015, two males, were full siblings. The third wolf litter from 2016 consisted of four males and two females and was unrelated to the wolf litters from 2014 and 2015.

b) Behavioural sampling

To ensure that the puppies’ senses were fully developed by the time of the first test we conducted the first novel object test at six weeks of age (31). Novel object tests were hereafter performed on a monthly basis at 10, 14, 18, 22 and 26 weeks of age. The puppies were tested in a familiar room (4×4 meters) in which a novel object was present, placed opposite of where the puppy would enter the room, approximately four meters away. Puppies were lead into the room by a caregiver, who quickly left the room and closed the door. The duration of a trial was 10 minutes and trials were always monitored by CHW. Some trials (n = 11) were stopped prematurely because the novel object was destroyed. All test were filmed with two mounted GoPro cameras (model 3-4, GoPro Inc.) on opposite sides of the room.

c) Novel objects

A different novel object was used at each age in order to avoid habituation. Objects were handled as little as possible and always with freshly washed hands to avoid food smells transferring to the objects and possibly affecting the puppy’s behaviour towards the object. Novel objects were chosen based on previous studies with wolves and dogs (Moretti et al. 2015, Marshall-Pescini et al. 2017) and varied on several scales, such as shape, sound and movement, and included at six weeks: a rolled up mattress, 10 weeks: a wheelbarrow (up-side down), 14 weeks: a mirror mounted to the wall, 18 weeks: a stuffed wolverine toy, 22 weeks: a moving mechanical dog and 24 weeks: a moving sheet (attached to a string).

d) Behavioural scoring

Behavioural scoring was carried out using the software BORIS v. 5.1.3. (43) based on an ethogram (Table 1). Behaviours were logged both as frequencies and durations (i.e. state behaviours). Similar to previous studies (Moretti et al., 2015), latency to approach the novel object was measured as the duration from test start to the time the puppy came within 1 meters distance of the novel object (Table S1). Latency to make contact with the novel object was measured as the duration to make physical contact with the novel object for the first time after the latency to approach (Table S1). Behaviours were scored in a non-overlapping way, with prioritization of behaviours related to the novel object, i.e. if the puppy was looking at the novel object while walking around the test room this was scored as looking at novel object.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1. Ethogram.

Behaviours scored during novel object tests. Behaviours were scored in a non-overlapping way, with prioritization of behaviours related to the novel object. Latency times were measured regardless of the behaviour performed.

Because there were cases where the total duration of the test was less than 10 minutes, the total test duration was included for use in further analysis (Table S2 and S3). In cases where a puppy did not approach the novel object, the trial duration was used as a measure of latency and in cases where the puppy did not make contact with the novel object this was recorded as a missing value. Inter-rater reliability was calculated using Cohen’s kappa and was considered good with a value of 87.4%.

e) Statistical methods

We tested for species effects in each behaviour by fitting linear mixed models, with either latency or the time spent on a behaviour as the dependent variable. The fixed effects of interest were species, age, their interaction and sex. Additionally, for the models of time spent, we controlled for variation in the duration of each trial by including duration as a covariate. To account for the repeated measures of individuals and the non-independence of individuals from the same litter, we included random intercepts for both factors. The full model in lme4 syntax: y ∼ species * age + sex + duration + (1|individual) + (1|litter). Models were then reduced by backwards model selection using AIC (cut-off □AIC > 2, Table S4), where the parameters for species, duration and the random effects were always maintained. Both latencies were log10 transformed, and the time spent looking, investigating and manipulating the novel object were log transformed after adding 1, in order to fulfil the assumption of normality in the model residuals. We centred the age variable to aid interpretation of the species effect in case of an interaction. When the interaction was retained in the model, we additionally fitted a model where age was a discrete variable, and used that to perform post-hoc tests for species differences at each age (Table S5). All p-values were obtained using Satterwaithe’s approximation of denominator degrees of freedom. Post-hoc p-values were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Holm method.

All statistical analyses were performed in R (v3.4.3, R Core Team 2016), with mixed effects models fitted using lme4 v. 1.1-15 (44), Satterwaithe’s approximation from lmerTest v. 2.0-36 (45) and post-hoc testing using emmeans v. 1.1.2 (46).

d) Ethical note

Daily care and testing were performed in accordance with ethical legislation under Swedish Law. This study was approved by the Ethical Committee in Uppsala (C72/14), and our facilities and daily care routines were approved by the Swedish Board of Agriculture (5.2.18-12309/13).

Results

Latency measures

We found that wolves and dogs developed differently in latency to approach the novel object within 1 meter, where dogs expressed a larger reduction in latency with age compared to wolves (t = 2.35, df = 120.046, p = 0.02, Table 2, Figure 1a and 2). Dogs significantly decreased their latency with time, while wolves did not (see table S6 for slopes per species), resulting in dogs expressing significantly lowered latency to approach at 26 weeks compared to wolves (t = −3.131, df = 18.666, p = 0.006, padjusted = 0.034, Table S5). At younger ages we failed to detect significant differences in latency to approach the novel object between dogs and wolves (Table S5). For the latency to make contact with the novel object, we found no differences in wolves and dogs (t = 1.931, df = 2.16, p = 0.186, Table 2, Figure 1b and 2), neither did we find evidence of sex differences in either species.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2. Model summary.

Results for the best fitted model of repeated measures, with dogs as the reference, on 1) Latency to approach the novel object, 2) Latency to make contact with the novel object, 3) Looking at novel object (NO), 4) Investigating novel object, 5) Manipulating novel object, 6) Active behaviour and 7) Passive behaviour. Estimate, standard error, degress of freedom, t-value and p-value are given. Significant p-values are marked in bold italic.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 1. Dog – wolf comparisons.

Boxplots shows behavioral scores during a novel object test, comparing dogs and wolves across age. Overlaid are the fits and confidence intervals from the best model, selected by AIC. Boxes indicate the quartiles, and the whiskers reach maximally 1.5 times the interquartile range. Values beyond that are shown as points. Note that panels a and b make use of a log(x) scale, and panels e, f and g use log(x + 1).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 2. Standardized regression coefficients.

Standardized regression coefficients. for the best model for each behavior, selected by AIC. Ranges indicate confidence intervals, computed using the likelihood profile. Missing estimates indicate that the term was not included in the best model.

Behaviours related to the novel object

We found that wolves and dogs developed differently in looking at the novel object from a distance (t = −2.058, df = 120.667, p = 0.042, Table 2, Figure 1e), but no such differences were detected in the post hoc tests (Table S5). While both wolves and dogs increased their time spent looking at the novel object from a distance with age (t = 5.848, df = 117.899, p <0.001, Table 2, Figure 1e), dogs expressed a stronger effect of age than wolves (Figure 1e, Table S6).

Wolves and dogs also showed different developmental trajectories for the time spent investigating the novel object (t = 1.994, df = 139.315, p = 0.048, Table 2, Figure 1f and 2). Post-hoc tests revealed that wolves investigated the novel object for longer at 22 weeks than dogs (t = −2.831, df = 28.029, p = 0.008, padjusted = 0.051, Figure 1f, Table S5). The significant interaction between species and age in investigating the novel object again consisted of stronger effect of age in dogs than in wolves (Figure 1f, Table S6), but with an overall decrease with age in both species (t = −6.384, df = 138.727, p <0.001, Table 2, Figure 1f). We found that wolves and dogs developed similarly in time spent manipulating the novel object (Table 2, Figure 1g). We found no evidence of sex differences.

Behaviours not related to the novel object

We found that both species increased time spent on active behaviour with age (t = 2.2, df = 122.362, p = 0.03), with wolves expressing higher levels of activity than dogs (t = 4.26, df = 2.977, p = 0.024, Table 2, Figure 1c and 2, Table S5). Passive behaviour decreased with age in both wolves and dogs (t = −4.268, df = 121.140, p <0.001, Table 2, Figure 1d), and while dogs appeared more passive than wolves the species differences was not significant. We found no evidence of sex differences.

Discussion

Decreased expression of fear is considered a key behavioural alteration in domesticated animals, and evidence suggests that domestication drives altered developmental rates delaying the initial onset of fear response (2). However, while previous studies in wolves and dogs have focused exclusively on the timing of the initial onset of fear (23,30,31), it has been unclear if and how a developmental shift during early ontogeny, caused by domestication, affects the continued development and expression of fear in either species. Here we present the first extended examination of the development of fear behaviour within the juvenile period in wolves and dogs. Contrary to expectation, we found no evidence in support of a delayed onset of fear response in dogs compared to wolves during early stages of development. Instead we found that dogs strongly reduced their fear response to a novel object in the period between six and 26 weeks of age. Critically, wolves did not differ in their fear response towards novelty over time, and the detected species difference was caused solely by a progressive reduced fear response in dogs. Furthermore, dogs and wolves did on average not differ in their interaction with the novel object. Together our results suggest that species differences in fear of novelty are not caused by a domestication driven shift in the first onset of fear response. Instead, we suggest that a loss of sensitivity towards novelty with increasing age in dogs causes the difference in fear expression towards novelty in wolves and dogs.

Fearfulness has previously been quantified by the latency to approach and explore novelty, and novel stimuli such as objects, arenas and people have been used to detect the timing of the initial onset of fear response in both wolves and dogs (2,8,21,23,25,31) However, while there is a general expectation that domestication has caused a delay in the sensitive period in dogs, resulting in later onset of fear behaviour in dogs compared to wolves (8,22,30,31,47), we detected no species differences in fear expression during early development. This finding is in agreement with a recent study comparing exploration of novelty in six and eight weeks old wolves and dogs, which found no species differences in the latency to make contact with a novel object (32). Yet, it has been reported that adult wolves express increased latency to make contact to a novel object compared to dogs (33), thereby indicating that species differences in fear expression might arise later in development than previously thought. Thus, our finding that a species difference in latency to approach a novel object occurred at 26 weeks of age represents the first indication of when a quantifiable difference in fear towards novelty arises in wolves and dogs. We do, however, caution against an overly strong confidence in the exact timing of species differences occurring at 26 weeks. It is possible that the difference emerges in the weeks prior, but that the current sample size is insufficient for detection. However, it is clear that a difference between species progressively develops towards the later end of the time period measured here, and that we have captured the transition from equal fear towards novelty to a clear species difference. Importantly, the species difference in fear towards novelty did not occur because wolves became more fearful with age, as expected, but rather because dogs decreased their time to approach the novel object. Specifically, we detected no increased expression of fear towards novelty in neither wolves nor dogs throughout the study period, and notably wolves did not change their latency to approach the novel object throughout with age. This indicates that the development of fear response in wolves and dogs follows different trajectories, with dogs, but not wolves, loosing their sensitivity towards novelty with age.

The species difference we found in the latency to approach the novel object is not clearly reflected in differences in interaction with the same novel object. While fear of novelty was expressed immediately, through a delayed approach, once the novel object was approached this initial fearfulness appears to no longer affect behavioural responses, and thus wolves and dogs did not differ in their latency to make contact with or interact with the novel object. While the latency to approach the novel object and the time spent being active and passive while in the test room showed consistent linear development over time in both wolves and dogs, the pattern in looking at, investigating and manipulating the novel object appeared variable across trials. This variability was most likely caused by the different novel objects that were used in the study, i.e. behaviours that are more closely related to the object itself show more variability across tests. For example, the stuffed wolverine toy clearly provided more incentive for manipulation than the upside-down wheelbarrow. It is possible that this increased variance may have prohibited detection of additional species differences in behavioural measures directly related to the novel object, such as increased exploration and manipulation of novel objects as reported in both juvenile and adult wolves compared to dogs (Moretti et al., 2015, Marshall-Pescini et al. 2017)

Wolves develop physically faster than dogs (48), and it has been suggested that wolves express increased activity at an earlier age than dogs due to this difference in developmental pace of motor patterns (32,48). However, while we do find a species difference in how much time is spent on active behaviour during tests, this species difference is consistent across age and not restricted to early ontogeny alone. This indicates that wolves, on a general scale, are more active than dogs. While it cannot be ruled out that active behaviour is affected by the presence of a novel object, it is a less likely explanation for our findings, as we measured behaviours in a non-overlapping way with priority of behaviours related to the novel object. Thus, the measurement of activity does not include looking at, manipulating or approaching the novel object, but only time spent on active behaviour with no attention to the novel object. Instead the higher activity in wolves might reflect an increased reactivity of being separated from littermates and being confined in the test room compared to dogs.

Domestication has caused a general acceleration of sexual maturity in animals (12), and earlier sexual maturation in dogs (11,14) could explain the steeper behavioural change observed in dogs compared to wolves across some of the behaviours related to the novel object in our study. However, while reproduction in wild living wolf packs is restricted to the breeding couple, it is currently unclear if the lack of sexual activity in non-reproducing pack members is caused by delayed sexual maturity, behavioural suppression or restricted access to nutrition (49-51). Nevertheless, it has been demonstrated that captive wolves removed from social constraints sexually mature as early as nine months of age (50). Thus, it is unclear if we should expect behavioural ontogeny to be affected by a shift in developmental pace caused by earlier sexual maturity when comparing wolves and dogs living in captive, non-reproductive groups. Our study was conducted before sexual maturity occurred in either wolves or dogs and as we found no effect of sex on the expression of behaviour, we suggest that the steeper development of some behaviours in dogs are instead related to the loss of sensitivity towards novelty.

In conclusion, our study shows that wolves and dogs do not differ in their fear towards novelty before late in the juvenile phase. Importantly, the species difference does not occur because wolves become more fearful with age, but because dogs become less fearful with age. These findings have general implications for our interpretation of how domestication has shaped behavioural ontogeny. We suggest that future studies quantify fear related behaviour on a long-term scale to increase our understanding of the behavioural trajectory of fear through different developmental stages.

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank the Department of Zoology, Stockholm University, for funding this study, our hand-raisers Patricia Berner, Anna Björk, Marjut Pokela, Linn Larsson, Charles Gent, Åsa Lycke, Erika Grasser, Joanna Schinner, Yrsa Andersson, Christoffer Sernert and Mija Jansson, and the staff at Tovetorp Zoological Research Station.

References

  1. 1.↵
    Driscoll CA, Macdonald DW, O’Brien SJ. (2009). From wild animals to domestic pets, an evolutionary view of domestication. PNAS. 106(1):9971–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. 2.↵
    Belyaev DK, Plyusnina IZ, Trut LN. (1985). Domestication in the silver fox (Vulpes fulvus Desm): Changes in physiological boundaries of the sensitive period of primary socialization. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 13:359–70.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  3. 3.
    Trut L. (1999). Early Canid Domestication: The Farm-Fox Experiment: Foxes bred for tamability in a 40-year experiment exhibit remarkable transformations that suggest an interplay between behavioral genetics and development. American Scientist. 87:160–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  4. 4.↵
    Zeder MA. (2012). The domestication of animals. Journal of Anthropological Research. 68:161–90.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  5. 5.↵
    Trut LN, Plyusnina IZ, Oskina IN. (2004). An Experiment on Fox Domestication and Debatable Issues of Evolution of the Dog. Russian Journal of Genetics. Kluwer Academic Publishers-Plenum Publishers; 40(6):644–55. http://doi.org/10.1023/B:RUGE.0000033312.92773.c1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  6. 6.↵
    Boissy A. (1995). Fear and Fearfulness in Animals. The Quarterly Review of Biology.;70:165–91.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  7. 7.↵
    Weidenmayer CP. (2009). Plasticity of defensive behavior and fear in early development. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 33(3):1447–57. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.11.004
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  8. 8.↵
    Scott JP, Fuller JL. (1965). Genetics and the social behavior of the dog - the classic study. University of Chicago Press.
  9. 9.↵
    Griffin AS. (2004). Social learning about predators: A review and prospectus. Learning & Behavior. 32(1):131–40. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  10. 10.↵
    Zeder MA. (2015). Core questions in domestication research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 112(11):3191–8. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1501711112
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. 11.↵
    Goodwin D, Bradshaw JWS, Wickens SM. (1997). Paedomorphosis affects agonistic visual signals of domestic dogs. Animal Behaviour. 53:297–304.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  12. 12.↵
    Price EO. (1999). Behavioral development in animals undergoing domestication. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 65:245–71.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  13. 13.↵
    Dobney K, Larson G. (2006). Genetics and animal domestication: new windows on an elusive process. J Zoology. 269(0):261–71. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.2006.00042.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  14. 14.↵
    Morey DF.(1994). The early evolution of the domestic dog. American Scientist. 82(4):336–47.
    OpenUrlGeoRefWeb of Science
  15. 15.
    Coppinger R, Glendinning J, Torop E, Matthay C, Sutherland M, Smith C. (1987). Degree of Behavioural Neoteny Differentiates Canid Polymorphs. Ethology. 75:89–108.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  16. 16.↵
    Crockford SJ. (2002). Animal Domestication and Heterochronic Speciation: The role of Thyroid Hormone. The Johns Hopkins University Press. 122–53.
  17. 17.↵
    Martin JT. (1978). Embryonic Pituitary Adrenal Axis, Behavior Development and Domestication in Birds. American Zoologist. 18:489–99.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  18. 18.
    Gariépy J-L, Bauer DJ, Cairns RB.(2001). Selective breeding for differential aggression in mice provides evidence for heterochrony in social behaviours. Animal Behaviour. 61(5):933–47. http://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1700
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  19. 19.↵
    Wilkins AS, Wrangham RW, Fitch WT. (2014). The “Domestication Syndrome” in Mammals: A Unified Explanation Based on Neural Crest Cell Behavior and Genetics. 197(3):795–808. http://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.114.165423
    OpenUrl
  20. 20.↵
    Scott JP. (1962). Critical periods in behavioural development. Science. 138:949–58.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  21. 21.↵
    Freedman DG, King JA, Elliot O. (1961). Critical Period in the social development of dogs. Science. 31:1016–62.
    OpenUrl
  22. 22.↵
    Coppinger R, Coppinger L. (2002). Dogs: A Startling New Understanding of Canine Origin, Behavior & Evolution. Univeristy of Chicago Press
  23. 23.↵
    Morrow M, Ottobre J, Ottobre A, Neville P, St-Pierre N, Dreschel N, et al. (2015). Breed-dependent differences in the onset of fear-related aviodance behaviour in puppies. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research. 10(4):286–94. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2015.03.002
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. 24.↵
    Price EO. (2002). Animal Domestication and Behavior. CABI Publishing, CAB Int;.
  25. 25.↵
    Scott JP, Marston M. (1950). Critical Periods Affecting the Development of Normal and Mal-Adjustive Social Behavior of Puppies. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology. 77(1):25–60. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1950.tb27330.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  26. 26.↵
    Scott JP. (1958). Critical Periods in the Development of Social Behavior in Puppies. Psychosomatic Medicine. 20:42–54. http://doi.org/10.1097/00006842-195801000-00005
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. 27.↵
    Fentress JC. (1967). Observations on the behavioural developement of a hand-reared male Timber wolf. American Zoologist.7(2):339–51.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. 28.
    Wooply JH, Ginsburg BE. (1967). Wolf socialization: a study of temperament in a wild social species. American Zoologist. 7(2):357–63.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  29. 29.
    Fox MW. (1972). Socio-Ecological Implications of Individual Differences in Wolf Litters: a Developmental and Evolutionary Perspective. Behaviour. 41(3):298–313. http://doi.org/10.1163/156853972X00077
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  30. 30.↵
    1. Frank H
    Zimen E. (1987). Ontogeny of approach and flight behavior towards humans in wolves, poodles and wolf-poodle hybrids. Frank H, editor. Dr. W. Junk Publishers, Dordrecht;
  31. 31.↵
    1. Zeh D
    Lord K. A (2013). Comparison of the Sensory Development of Wolves (Canis lupus lupus) and Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Zeh D, editor. Ethology. 2nd ed. 119(2):110–20. http://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90109-8
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  32. 32.↵
    Marshall-Pescini S, Virányi Z, Kubinyi E, Range F. (2017). Motivational Factors Underlying Problem Solving: Comparing Wolf and Dog Puppies’ Explorative and Neophobic Behaviors at 5, 6, and 8 Weeks of Age. Frontiers in Psychology. 8:e20231. http://doi.org/10.1093/icb/7.2.357
    OpenUrl
  33. 33.↵
    Moretti L, Hentrup M, Kotrschal K, Range F. (2015). The influence of relationships on neophobia and exploration in wolves and dogs. Animal Behaviour. Elsevier Ltd; 107(C):159–73. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.06.008
    OpenUrl
  34. 34.↵
    Hansen Wheat C, Fitzpatrick J, Tapper I, Temrin H. (2018). Wolf (Canis lupus) hybrids highlight the importance of human-directed play behavior during domestication of dogs (Canis familiaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/com0000119
  35. 35.↵
    Bremner-Harrison S, Prodohl PA, Elwood RW. (2004). Behavioural trait assessment as a release criterion: boldness predicts early death in a reintroduction programme of captive-bred swift fox (Vulpes velox). Animal Conservation. (3):313–20. http://doi.org/10.1017/S1367943004001490
  36. 36.
    Boogert NJ, Reader SM, Laland KN. (2004). The relation between social rank, neophobia and individual learning in starlings. Animal Behaviour.
  37. 37.↵
    Mainwaring MC, Beal JL, Hartley IR. (2011). Zebra finches are bolder in an asocial, rather than social, context. Behavioural Processes. 87(2):171–5. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2011.03.005
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  38. 38.↵
    Martin LB II, Fitzgerald L. A taste for novelty in invading house sparrows, Passer domesticus. Behavioral Ecology. 2005 Apr 13;16(4):702–7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  39. 39.↵
    Brust V, Guenther A. (2014). Domestication effects on behavioural traits and learning performance: comparing wild cavies to guinea pigs. Animal Cognition. 18(1):99–109. http://doi.org/10.1016/0169-5347(94)90134-1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  40. 40.↵
    Wilsson E, Sundgren P-E. (1998). Behaviour test for eight-week old puppies—heritabilities of tested behaviour traits and its correspondence to later behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 58:151–62.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  41. 41.
    Bray EE, Sammel MD, Cheney DL, Serpell JA, Seyfarth RM. (2017). Effects of maternal investment, temperament, and cognition on guide dog success. PNAS. 114(34):9128–33. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1704303114
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. 42.↵
    Clark MM, Galef BG Jr. (1982). Environmental Effects on the Ontogey of Exploratory and Escape Behaviors of Mongolian Gerbils. Developmental Psychobiology. 15(2):121–9. http://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420150205
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. 43.↵
    1. Fitzjohn R
    Friard O, Gamba M. (2016). BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Fitzjohn R, editor. Methods Ecol Evol. 7(11):1325–30. http://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1993.1127
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  44. 44.↵
    Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. 67(1). http://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  45. 45.↵
    Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB. (2017). lmerTestPackage: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software. 82(13). http://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13
  46. 46.↵
    Lenth RV. (2016). Least-Squares Means: The RPackage lsmeans. Journal of Statistical Software. 69(1). http://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v069.i01
  47. 47.↵
    Fox MW. (1970). A comparative study of the development of facial expressions in canids; wolf, coyote and foxes. Behaviour. 36(1/2):49–73.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  48. 48.↵
    Frank H, Frank MG. (1982). Comparison of problem-solving performance in six-week-old wolves and dogs. Animal Cognition. 30:95–8.
    OpenUrl
  49. 49.↵
    Packard JM, Seal US, Mech LD, Plotka ED. (1985). Causes of Reproductive Failure in Two Family Groups of Wolves (Canis lupus). Z Tierpsychol. 68(1):24–40. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1985.tb00112.x
    OpenUrl
  50. 50.↵
    Medjo DC, Mech DL. (1976). Reproductive Activity in Nine- and Ten-Month_old Wolves. Journal of Mammalogy. 57:406–8. http://doi.org/10.2307/1379708
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  51. 51.↵
    Mech DL. (1999). Alpha status, dominance, and division of labor in wolf packs. Can J Zool. 77:1196–203.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted August 31, 2018.
Download PDF
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Dogs, but not wolves, lose their sensitivity towards novelty with age
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Dogs, but not wolves, lose their sensitivity towards novelty with age
Christina Hansen Wheat, Wouter van der Bijl, Hans Temrin
bioRxiv 405480; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/405480
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo LinkedIn logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Dogs, but not wolves, lose their sensitivity towards novelty with age
Christina Hansen Wheat, Wouter van der Bijl, Hans Temrin
bioRxiv 405480; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/405480

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Animal Behavior and Cognition
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (3585)
  • Biochemistry (7539)
  • Bioengineering (5494)
  • Bioinformatics (20724)
  • Biophysics (10292)
  • Cancer Biology (7946)
  • Cell Biology (11609)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (6584)
  • Ecology (10161)
  • Epidemiology (2065)
  • Evolutionary Biology (13573)
  • Genetics (9511)
  • Genomics (12811)
  • Immunology (7900)
  • Microbiology (19490)
  • Molecular Biology (7632)
  • Neuroscience (41969)
  • Paleontology (307)
  • Pathology (1254)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (2189)
  • Physiology (3258)
  • Plant Biology (7017)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (1293)
  • Synthetic Biology (1946)
  • Systems Biology (5417)
  • Zoology (1112)