Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

Towards a “Treadmill Test” for Cognition: Reliable Prediction of Intelligence From Whole-Brain Task Activation Patterns

Chandra Sripada, Mike Angstadt, Saige Rutherford
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/412056
Chandra Sripada
1Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: sripada@umich.edu
Mike Angstadt
1Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Saige Rutherford
1Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Supplementary material
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

Identifying brain-based markers of general cognitive ability, i.e., “intelligence”, has been a longstanding goal of cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Previous studies focused on relatively static, enduring features such as gray matter volume and white matter structure. In this report, we investigate prediction of intelligence based on task activation patterns during the N-back working memory task as well as six other tasks in the Human Connectome Project dataset, encompassing 19 task contrasts. We find that whole brain task activation patterns are a highly effective basis for prediction of intelligence, achieving a 0.68 correlation with intelligence scores in an independent sample, which exceeds results reported from other modalities. Additionally, we show that tasks that tap executive processing and that are more cognitively demanding are particularly effective for intelligence prediction. These results suggest a picture analogous to treadmill testing for cardiac function: Placing the brain in an activated task state improves brain-based prediction of intelligence.

1 Introduction

In addition to particular abilities associated with individual cognitive tasks, there is substantial evidence for an overarching general ability involved in performance across a diverse range of tasks.1–5 Intelligence tests composed of multiple subtests can yield accurate estimates of this general ability, usually denoted g, and which we here refer to as “intelligence”.6, 7 Intelligence is a fundamental dimension of individual differences and is a key contributor to a number of important academic, occupational, health, and well-being-related outcomes.8–13 There is thus substantial interest in understanding the neural basis of intelligence and in developing “neuromarkers” of intelligence, objective brain-based means of measuring individual differences.

One potentially promising route is to construct neuromarkers of intelligence from neuroimaging maps, including structural and functional imaging. Existing studies have mainly investigated brain size14, cortical thickness/gray matter volume15, 16, white matter structure17, and resting state functional connectivity18–20 (for reviews, see 21–24). A notable feature of these studies is they mainly examine stable, enduring features of the brain, features that are largely independent of the person’s current cognitive state, and in particular their actual exercise of the cognitive abilities that are relevant to intelligent task performance. An alternative approach for constructing neuromarkers of intelligence, whose rationale resembles that for cardiac treadmill testing, attempts to first place the brain in an activated state that engages these cognitive abilities, rendering brain features associated with these abilities more “visible” to functional imaging (see 25 for a suggestion along these lines).

Interestingly, while task-based neuroimaging studies have been extensively used to investigate the brain-basis of intelligence, they have thus far been used nearly exclusively for the purposes of localization: Activation patterns in higher versus lower intelligence individuals are compared to identify the places in the brain where there are statistically significant differences.26–30 Task-based studies have thus far not been used widely in a prediction framework in which whole-brain activation patterns are harnessed for making predictions of each subject’s intelligence.

In the current study, we address this notable gap. Utilizing the Human Connectome Project’s 1200 release, we construct a highly reliable measure of intelligence from 10 measures from the NIH Toolbox and Penn Neurocognitive Battery. We then examine brain-based prediction of intelligence from contrast maps derived from the 2-back working memory task as well as six other fMRI tasks (19 task contrasts in total), and we establish two things. First, task-based activation patterns allow highly reliable prediction of intelligence, with performance appreciably higher than that reported in other neuroimaging modalities. Second, tasks that tap executive processing and that are more cognitively demanding yield more accurate predictions of intelligence.

2 Methods

2.1 Subjects and Data Acquisition

All subjects and data were from the HCP-1200 release31, 32 and all research was performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Subjects provided informed consent, and recruitment procedures and informed consent forms, including consent to share de-identified data, were approved by the Washington University institutional review board. Subjects completed two runs each of seven scanner tasks across two fMRI sessions, using a 32-channel head coil on a 3T Siemens Skyra scanner (TR = 720ms, TE = 33.1ms, 72 slices, 2mm isotropic voxels, multiband acceleration factor = 8) with right-to-left and left-to-right phase encoding directions. Comprehensive details are available elsewhere on HCP’s overall neuroimaging approach31, 33 and HCP’s task fMRI dataset34.

Subjects were eligible to be included if they had available MSMAll registered task data for both runs of all seven tasks, had full behavioral data, and no more than 25% of their volumes in each run exceeded a framewise displacement threshold of 0.5mm. These exclusions resulted in 958 subjects. This was further reduced to 944 subjects in generating the Train/Test set split (see below).

2.2 Data Preparation

Data was preprocessed through the HCP minimally preprocessed pipeline, which is presented in detail by Glasser et al.35 Briefly, the pipeline includes gradient unwarping, motion correction, fieldmap distortion correction, brain-boundary based linear registration of functional to structural images, non-linear registration to MNI152 space, and grand-mean intensity normalization. Data then entered a surfaced-based preprocessing stream, followed by grayordinate-based processing, which involves data from the cortical ribbon being projected to surface space and combined with subcortical volumetric data.

2.3 FMRI Tasks

We used contrasts from seven HCP tasks, described in brief in Table 1 (detailed descriptions are available elsewhere32, 34).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1: Seven HCP fMRI Tasks.

At the single subject-level, fixed-effects analyses were conducted using FSL’s FEAT to estimate the average effects across runs within-participants, using 2mm surface smoothed data. Some tasks permitted multiple contrasts beyond the standard experimental versus control condition (e.g., N-back allows additional contrasts based on all four stimulus types). To reduce the complexity of the analysis and avoid loss of power from smaller number of trials, we focused on the standard contrasts yielding a total of 19. A full list of filenames of the contrast maps used can be found in the Supplemental Table S1.

2.4 Constructing a General Intelligence Factor

We conducted an exploratory factor analysis utilizing the strategy and associated code supplied by Dubois and colleagues (https://github.com/adolphslab/HCP_MRI-behavior), who recently investigated prediction of intelligence from resting state fMRI in the HCP dataset36. Unadjusted scores from ten cognitive tasks for 1181 HCP subjects were included in the analysis (subjects with missing data or MMSE < 26 were excluded), including seven tasks from the NIH Toolbox (Dimensional Change Cart Sort, Flanker Task, List Sort Test, Picture Sequence Test, Picture Vocabulary Test, Pattern Completion Test, Oral Reading Recognition Test) and three tasks from the Penn Neurocognitive Battery (Penn Progressive Matrices, Penn Word Memory Test, Variable Short Penn Line Orientation Test), with additional details supplied in36.

We applied Dubois and colleagues’ code to this data, which in turn uses the omega function in the psych (v 1.8.4) package37 in R (v3.4.4). In particular, the code performs maximum likelihood-estimated exploratory factor analysis (specifying a bifactor model), oblimin factor rotation, followed by a Schmid-Leiman transformation38 to find general factor loadings.

To assess reliability, in a separate analysis, we re-ran the factor analysis excluding 46 subjects that had Test/Retest sessions available. We then estimated factor scores for both sessions for these subjects and calculated test/retest reliability via intraclass correlation (we used ICC(2,1) in the Shrout and Fleiss scheme39).

2.5 Train/Test Split

The 958 subjects after exclusions were divided into two groups. To generate a test dataset of 100 unrelated subjects we first selected the 86 subjects with no siblings who passed exclusion criteria. Then, we randomly selected 14 families from those families with 2 siblings included and randomly dropped one sibling of each pair. This resulted in 944 total subjects for our analyses, with 844 in the training data and 100 unrelated subjects in the test set.

2.6 Dimensionality Estimation and Principal Component Analysis

In the training dataset, each subject’s unthresholded task data from 19 task contrasts was vectorized and concatenated yielding 19 matrices, each with dimensions of 844 subjects by 91282 grayordinates. We next estimated the number of intrinsic dimensions associated with each task contrast. This was accomplished by submitting each of the 19 subjects x grayordinates contrast matrices to the dimensionality estimation procedure of Levina and Bickel40. This is a maximum likelihood estimation method based on distance between close neighbors, which we previously successfully applied to HCP resting state data41.

Dimensionality estimation found a mean of 72 dimensions across the 19 task contrasts. Because prior studies by our group41 showed small differences in the number of components make little difference in classifier performance, we chose the round number of 75 components for each task. We also verified that using the exact task-specific number of components made no difference to the outcome (see Supplemental Table S2).

Next, each of the 19 subjects x grayordinates contrast matrices was submitted to principal components analysis using the pca function in MATLAB (2015b), yielding, for each contrast, 843 components ordered by descending eigenvalues.

2.7 Brain Basis Set Modeling

Our aim was to predict each subject’s intelligence from their expression scores for n components (where n was typically set to be 75; see above). To accomplish this, we used Brain Basis Set (BBS) modeling, previously described in detail41.

In a training dataset, we calculate the expression scores for each of the n components for each subject by projecting their data onto the 75 principal components. We then fit a linear regression model with these expression scores as predictors and the phenotype of interest (i.e., intelligence) as the outcome, saving B, the n x 1 vector of fitted coefficients, for later use. In a test dataset, we again calculate the expression scores for each of the n components for each subject. Our predicted phenotype for each test subject is the dot product of B learned from the training dataset with the vector of component expression scores for that subject. We assessed performance of BBS-based prediction of intelligence by calculating the correlation between predicted versus actual intelligence in the test sample.

2.8 Addressing Additional Potential Confounds

In an additional analysis, we used multiple regression to remove a number of potential confounds from the intelligence variable (i.e., g). Similar to Dubois et al.36, variables regressed were: age, handedness, gender, brain size, multiband reconstruction algorithm version number (HCP variables: Age_In_Yrs, Handedness, Gender, FS_BrainSeg_Vol, fMRI_3T_ReconVrs), and mean framewise displacement (task-specific values were used). Analyses involving intelligence prediction were then repeated with the confound-cleansed variable. Results were broadly similar to the original analyses and are presented in Supplemental Table S2.

2.9 Consensus Component Maps for Visualization

We used BBS with 75 whole-brain components to make predictions about intelligence. To help convey overall patterns across the entire BBS predictive model, we constructed “consensus” component maps. We first multiplied each component map with its associated beta from the fitted BBS model. Next, we summed across all 75 components yielding a single map, and z scored the entries.

3 Results

3.1. Constructing a g Factor From Ten HCP Behavioral Tasks

As reported by Dubios and colleagues36, a bifactor model with a general factor g and four group factors, fit the data very well (CFI=0.990; RMSEA=0.0311; SRMR=0.0201; BIC=-0.519). The solution is depicted in Figure 1. Following Dubois and colleagues, we interpret the four group factors as: 1) Crystallized Ability; 2) Processing Speed; 3) Visuospatial Ability; and 4) Memory.

Figure 1:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 1: Bifactor Model Based on Ten Behavioral Tasks from the HCP Dataset with General Factor (“g”) and Four Group Factors.

C=Crystallized Intelligence, S=Processing Speed, V=Visuospatial Ability, M=Memory.

The general factor g, which we refer to throughout as “intelligence” and which is the focus of this report, accounts for 58.5% of the variance (coefficient omega hierarchical ω42), while group factors account for 18.2% of the variance. Based on the 46 subjects in the retest dataset for HCP, test-retest reliability for g was found to be, 0.78, which is conventionally classified as very good (we used ICC(2,1) in the Shrout and Fleiss scheme39).

3.2 Contrasts associated with the N-Back task are highly effective at predicting intelligence in out-of-sample subjects

Because working memory has been strongly and consistently linked with intelligence35, 43, 44, we first investigated prediction of intelligence based on the N-back working memory task. A BBS model utilizing 75 components was fit in the training dataset and then applied to the test dataset. The correlation in the test dataset between predicted intelligence and actual intelligence was 0.68 (p = 8.5 × 10−15).

Figure 2 shows the top three components based on statistical significance displayed so that greater expression of these components predicts greater intelligence. These components include large activations in supplementary motor area (SMA), precuneus, and dlPFC, as well as deactivations in anterior default mode network (DMN). To convey “average” patterns across all 75 components, we constructed consensus component maps (see Methods) and they are displayed in Figure 3. These show additional patterns predictive of intelligence, including deactivation of posterior cingulate cortex and fronto-polar cortex.

Figure 2:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 2: Visualization of the Three Most Intelligence-Predictive Components From the 2-back vs. 0-back Task Contrast.

The three most statistically significant components are shown from a 75-component brain basis set model trained to predict intelligence scores.

Figure 3:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 3: Consensus Component Maps for Seven Task Contrasts Highly Predictive of Intelligence.

Each consensus component map captures aggregate patterns across a 75-component brain basis set model for prediction of intelligence. (top row) N-Back Task; (middle row) Relational Task; (bottom row) Math – Story Contrast.

We next trained additional BBS models on the 2-back vs. baseline and 0-back vs. baseline contrasts. The correlation in the test dataset between predicted intelligence and actual intelligence was 0.58 (p = 2.0×10−10) and 0.43 (p = 6.2×10−6), respectively. The consensus component models in Figure 3 revealed an interesting change in directionality across these contrasts. For example, pre-SMA strongly predicts greater intelligence in the 2-back contrast vs. baseline but the reverse is true in the 0-back vs. baseline contrast. Additionally, less activation (i.e., deactivation) of the anterior DMN predicts higher intelligence in the 2-back vs. 0-back contrast, but the reverse is true in the 0-back vs. baseline contrast.

3.3 Looking across all 19 task contrasts, tasks involving executive processing and higher cognitive demand are more effective in predicting intelligence

We next compared intelligence prediction based on the three N-back task contrasts with the remaining 16 contrasts from the other six HCP tasks. As with the N-back task, we trained BBS models for each task contrast in the train dataset and applied the models to the held out test dataset.

Results are shown in Figure 3. Tasks involving executive processing were top performers, including contrasts from the N-back task and relational reasoning task, as well as the math vs. story contrast of the language processing task. The reward vs. baseline and punishment vs. baseline contrasts of the gambling task, both of which involve making numerical judgments, also performed well.

3.4 FPN/DMN activation, a measure of task demandingness, predicts which tasks are effective for intelligence prediction

A number of studies have observed that tasks that are cognitively demanding produce activation in regions of frontoparietal network (FPN)45–48 and deactivation of regions of default mode network (DMN)49–52. Building on these observations, we hypothesized the more cognitively demanding tasks (operationalized in terms of activation levels of FPN and DMN) should be more effective in predicting intelligence. To ensure comparability across task contrasts, we focused on the 12 task contrasts that compared a task condition versus resting baseline. We performed a regression analysis with accuracy of intelligence prediction as the outcome variable and FPN and DMN activation as predictors. Results, shown in Figure 4, support our hypothesis: The correlation across task contrasts between predicted accuracy, based on FPN and DMN activation, and actual accuracy in predicting intelligence is 0.67 (FPN standardized β = 0.63, DMN standardized β = - 0.58, p = 0.0482).

Figure 4:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 4: Intelligence Prediction Across 19 Task Contrasts.

A. Prediction of Intelligence Using Brain Basis Set Modeling For 19 HCP Task Contrasts. Small Black Dots = results with additional regression of a number of potential confounds (see §2.8). B. FPN/DMN Activation Patterns During a Task Strongly Predict Effectiveness of That Task in Predicting Intelligence.

3.5. Across the 19 task contrasts, activation signatures of intelligence are spatially distributed and task-specific

We next compared the consensus component maps associated with the 19 contrasts (seven maps are shown in Figure 3, and the remaining maps are shown in Supplemental Figure S1). Signatures of intelligence prediction associated with each task were highly distributed, with remarkable variation in these signatures across tasks. Prominently represented regions include: superior parietal cortex (reward vs. baseline, punishment vs. baseline), dlPFC (math vs. story), anterior insula (relational v. match), fronto-polar cortex (math vs. story), pre-SMA (relational vs. match), and visual cortex (relational vs. match, reward vs. baseline, punishment vs. baseline). All 75 components as well as consensus component maps for each of the 19 task contrasts have been shared on BALSA, the Human Connectome Projects’ website for sharing and hosting neuroimaging datasets and can be accessed here: https://balsa.wustl.edu/study/show/MZPv.

4 Discussion

This study is the first to systematically assess neuroimaging-based prediction of intelligence across multiple fMRI task conditions. We find that whole-brain task activation patterns are a highly effective basis for prediction of intelligence, with a model trained on activation during the N-back working memory task achieving a 0.68 correlation with intelligence scores in an independent sample. Additionally, we demonstrate that more cognitively demanding tasks are particularly effective for intelligence prediction. These results highlight the importance of placing the brain in an activated task state for accurate brain-based prediction of intelligence.

Role of executive regions in prediction of intelligence

The importance of fronto-parietal network, as well as related executive regions (e.g., dorsal anterior cingulate), for intelligence has been highlighted in previous work, especially in Jung and Haier’s influential fronto-parietal integration theory21. In a similar vein, Duncan and colleagues proposed that “multiple demand” cortex— regions of the brain that activate across a broad range of cognitively demanding tasks45—are a primary substrate of intelligence27. The present study extends these findings by demonstrating a key role for fronto-parietal regions as a major source of discriminative information for making subject-level predictions of intelligence. This additional role of executive regions was supported in three complementary ways.

First, in looking across the set of 19 contrasts derived from seven HCP tasks, we found that tasks that tap executive processes were more predictive of intelligence (e.g., N-back task contrasts, relational reasoning task contrasts, and math vs. story contrast). Second, we found that FPN activation and DMN deactivation, highly associated with the cognitive demandingness of task conditions45–52, predict which task contrasts will be effective for intelligence prediction. Third, within highly predictive contrasts, such the 2-back vs. 0-back contrast and math vs. story contrast, activation patterns in executive regions were prominent among regions predictive of intelligence.

Interestingly, for certain regions, the directionality of prediction of intelligence exhibited some variability across task contrasts in a way suggestive of moderation by task difficulty (for example, see pre-SMA in 0-back compared to 2-back and in match compared to relational). These observations are consistent with a neural efficiency model of intelligence proposed by Neubauer and Fink53. They propose that higher intelligence is associated with greater processing efficiency in elementary cognitive tasks (leading to less activation in more intelligent individuals) but greater processing capacity in demanding cognitive tasks (leading to greater activation in more intelligent people), thus potentially explaining the flipped directions of activation observed across the easy and hard conditions of the N-back and other tasks.

While activation patterns in executive regions clearly play an important role in explaining the success of our task-based approach to intelligence prediction, there is still clear evidence for substantial discriminative information about intelligence located outside executive regions. This is apparent in looking at the consensus component images in Figure 3 as well Figure S1 in the Supplement. Activations are observed in distributed regions of cortex, including non-executive regions such as visual cortex, lateral temporal cortex, and temporal pole, among other regions.

Comparison of task-based prediction with other modalities

Previous studies using neuroimaging for prediction of intelligence have primarily used structural measures, for example cortical thickness15, 16 or white matter structure17, for reviews see 21–23. In terms of functional fMRI, recent studies have examined resting state connectivity patterns.18–20 In an important study by Dubois and colleagues36 that examined the same HCP 1200 dataset used in the present study, they found resting state connectivity patterns predicted intelligence with a correlation of 0.44 in cross-validated testing (see also a related finding from our group41). While impressive, this is still substantially less than the peak correlation of 0.68 achieved in the present study in out-of-sample testing.

There are two interrelated reasons why task-based fMRI might potentially offer more reliable prediction of intelligence than other modalities. The first appeals to the “treadmill testing” idea already mentioned: actively34 engaging in cognitive tasks has the potential to unmask critical intelligence-relevant features of the brain that are otherwise invisible in other modalities such as structural brain imaging. A second potential advantage of task-based methods is specificity. Tasks are constructed by their designers to target specific psychological processes, often with control conditions that subtract away contributions from auxiliary processes of no interest. This will tend to make classification more accurate as the feature set is culled of a sizable number of uninformative features.

Future Directions

Results from this study open the door to promising additional lines of research. It is notable that we used the set of imaging tasks that were included in the HCP dataset. These imaging tasks, in turn, were selected based on diverse considerations (see 34), but maximizing prediction of intelligence was not among them. Thus, it is plausible that one can do still better: It should be possible to intentionally design and optimize an imaging task battery—based on findings from the literature as well as trial-and-error experimentation—to yield even more accurate task-based prediction of intelligence, and future work should explore this possibility.

In sum, this study firmly establishes the effectiveness of task-based fMRI for prediction of intelligence and demonstrates that tasks that tap executive processing and that are more cognitively demanding are associated with better prediction accuracy.

References

  1. ↵
    Spearman C. ‘General Intelligence,’ objectively determined and measured. Am J Psychol 1904; 15: 201–292.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  2. Neisser U, Boodoo G, Bouchard Jr TJ, Boykin AW, Brody N, Ceci SJ et al. Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. Am Psychol 1996; 51: 77.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  3. Horn JL, Noll J. Human cognitive capabilities: Gf-Gc theory. 1997.
  4. Mackintosh N, Mackintosh NJ. IQ and human intelligence. Oxford University Press, 2011.
  5. ↵
    Carroll JB. The higher-stratum structure of cognitive abilities: Current evidence supports g and about ten broad factors. In: The scientific study of general intelligence. Elsevier, 2003, pp 5–21.
  6. ↵
    Carroll JB. Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. Cambridge University Press, 1993.
  7. ↵
    McGrew KS. CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Elsevier, 2009.
  8. ↵
    Gottfredson LS. Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence 1997; 24: 79–132.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  9. Strenze T. Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence 2007; 35: 401–426.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  10. Ree MJ, Earles JA, Teachout MS. Predicting job performance: Not much more than g. J Appl Psychol 1994; 79: 518.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  11. Whitley E, Batty GD, Gale CR, Deary IJ, Tynelius P, Rasmussen F. Intelligence in early adulthood and subsequent risk of assault: cohort study of 1,120,998 Swedish men. Psychosom Med 2010; 72: 390.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. Gale CR, Batty GD, Tynelius P, Deary IJ, Rasmussen F. Intelligence in early adulthood and subsequent hospitalisation and admission rates for the whole range of mental disorders: longitudinal study of 1,049,663 men. Epidemiol Camb Mass 2010; 21: 70.
    OpenUrl
  13. ↵
    Batty GD, Mortensen EL, Osler M. Childhood IQ in relation to later psychiatric disorder: evidence from a Danish birth cohort study. Br J Psychiatry 2005; 187: 180–181.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    McDaniel MA. Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence. Intelligence 2005; 33: 337–346.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  15. ↵
    Shaw P, Greenstein D, Lerch J, Clasen L, Lenroot R, Gogtay N et al. Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature 2006; 440: 676.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  16. ↵
    Colom R, Haier RJ, Head K, Álvarez-Linera J, Quiroga MÁ, Shih PC et al. Gray matter correlates of fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence: Testing the P-FIT model. Intelligence 2009; 37: 124–135.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  17. ↵
    Turken U, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Bammer R, Baldo JV, Dronkers NF, Gabrieli JD. Cognitive processing speed and the structure of white matter pathways: convergent evidence from normal variation and lesion studies. Neuroimage 2008; 42: 1032–1044.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  18. ↵
    Heuvel MP van den, Stam CJ, Kahn RS, Pol HEH. Efficiency of Functional Brain Networks and Intellectual Performance. J Neurosci 2009; 29: 7619–7624.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. Finn ES, Shen X, Scheinost D, Rosenberg MD, Huang J, Chun MM et al. Functional connectome fingerprinting: identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nat Neurosci 2015; 18: 1664–1671.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Goñi J, Heuvel MP van den, Avena-Koenigsberger A, Mendizabal NV de, Betzel RF, Griffa A et al. Resting-brain functional connectivity predicted by analytic measures of network communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2014; 111: 833–838.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    Jung RE, Haier RJ. The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: converging neuroimaging evidence. Behav Brain Sci 2007; 30: 135–154.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. Deary IJ, Penke L, Johnson W. The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nat Rev Neurosci 2010; 11: 201.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  23. ↵
    Luders E, Narr KL, Thompson PM, Toga AW. Neuroanatomical correlates of intelligence. Intelligence 2009; 37: 156–163.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  24. ↵
    Pietschnig J, Penke L, Wicherts JM, Zeiler M, Voracek M. Meta-analysis of associations between human brain volume and intelligence differences: How strong are they and what do they mean? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 57: 411–432.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Finn ES, Scheinost D, Finn DM, Shen X, Papademetris X, Constable RT. Can brain state be manipulated to emphasize individual differences in functional connectivity? Neuroimage 2017.
  26. ↵
    Gray JR, Chabris CF, Braver TS. Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nat Neurosci 2003; 6: 316–322.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  27. ↵
    Duncan J, Seitz RJ, Kolodny J, Bor D, Herzog H, Ahmed A et al. A neural basis for general intelligence. Science 2000; 289: 457–460.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. Choi YY, Shamosh NA, Cho SH, DeYoung CG, Lee MJ, Lee J-M et al. Multiple bases of human intelligence revealed by cortical thickness and neural activation. J Neurosci 2008; 28: 10323–10329.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. Basten U, Stelzel C, Fiebach CJ. Intelligence is differentially related to neural effort in the task-positive and the task-negative brain network. Intelligence 2013; 41: 517–528.
    OpenUrl
  30. ↵
    Basten U, Hilger K, Fiebach CJ. Where smart brains are different: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional and structural brain imaging studies on intelligence. Intelligence 2015; 51: 10–27.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  31. ↵
    Van Essen DC, Smith SM, Barch DM, Behrens TEJ, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K. The WU- Minn Human Connectome Project: An overview. NeuroImage 2013; 80: 62–79.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  32. ↵
    WU-Minn HCP. 1200 Subjects Data Release Reference Manual. 2017.
  33. ↵
    Glasser MF, Sotiropoulos SN, Wilson JA, Coalson TS, Fischl B, Andersson JL et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage 2013; 80: 105–124.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  34. ↵
    Barch DM, Burgess GC, Harms MP, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL, Corbetta M et al. Function in the human connectome: task-fMRI and individual differences in behavior. Neuroimage 2013; 80: 169–189.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  35. ↵
    Engle RW, Kane MJ, Tuholski SW. Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. 1999.
  36. ↵
    Dubois J, Galdi P, Paul LK, Adolphs R. A distributed brain network predicts general intelligence from resting-state human neuroimaging data. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 2018; 373. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0284.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  37. ↵
    Revelle W. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. 2016.
  38. ↵
    Schmid J, Leiman JM. The development of hierarchical factor solutions. Psychometrika 1957; 22: 53–61.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  39. ↵
    Shrout PE, Fleiss JL. Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol Bull 1979; 86: 420–428.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  40. ↵
    Levina E, Bickel PJ. Maximum Likelihood estimation of intrinsic dimension. In: Proceedings of the 17th International Conference on Neural Information Processing Systems. MIT Press: Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, 2004, pp 777–784.
  41. ↵
    Sripada C, Angstadt M, Rutherford S, Kessler D, Kim Y, Yee M et al. Fundamental Differences: A Basis Set for Characterizing Inter-Individual Variation in Resting State Connectomes. bioRxiv 2018;: 326082.
  42. ↵
    Zinbarg RE, Revelle W, Yovel I, Li W. Cronbach’s α, Revelle’s β, and McDonald’s ω H: Their relations with each other and two alternative conceptualizations of reliability. psychometrika 2005; 70: 123–133.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  43. ↵
    Engle RW, Kane MJ. Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. In: The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. Elsevier, 2004.
  44. ↵
    Duncan J, Schramm M, Thompson R, Dumontheil I. Task rules, working memory, and fluid intelligence. Psychon Bull Rev 2012; 19: 864–870.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    Duncan J, Owen AM. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci 2000; 23: 475–483.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  46. Cabeza R, Nyberg L. Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J Cogn Neurosci 2000; 12: 1–47.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  47. Cole MW, Schneider W. The cognitive control network: Integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions. NeuroImage 2007; 37: 343–360.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  48. ↵
    Niendam TA, Laird AR, Ray KL, Dean YM, Glahn DC, Carter CS. Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2012; 12: 241–268.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    McKiernan KA, Kaufman JN, Kucera-Thompson J, Binder JR. A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. J Cogn Neurosci 2003; 15: 394–408.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  50. Anticevic A, Repovs G, Shulman GL, Barch DM. When less is more: TPJ and default network deactivation during encoding predicts working memory performance. NeuroImage 2010; 49: 2638–2648.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  51. Anticevic A, Cole MW, Murray JD, Corlett PR, Wang X-J, Krystal JH. The role of default network deactivation in cognition and disease. Trends Cogn Sci 2012; 16: 584–592.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  52. ↵
    Esposito F, Bertolino A, Scarabino T, Latorre V, Blasi G, Popolizio T et al. Independent component model of the default-mode brain function: Assessing the impact of active thinking. Brain Res Bull 2006; 70: 263–269.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  53. ↵
    Neubauer AC, Fink A. Intelligence and neural efficiency. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2009; 33: 1004–1023.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted September 09, 2018.
Download PDF

Supplementary Material

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Towards a “Treadmill Test” for Cognition: Reliable Prediction of Intelligence From Whole-Brain Task Activation Patterns
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Towards a “Treadmill Test” for Cognition: Reliable Prediction of Intelligence From Whole-Brain Task Activation Patterns
Chandra Sripada, Mike Angstadt, Saige Rutherford
bioRxiv 412056; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/412056
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo LinkedIn logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Towards a “Treadmill Test” for Cognition: Reliable Prediction of Intelligence From Whole-Brain Task Activation Patterns
Chandra Sripada, Mike Angstadt, Saige Rutherford
bioRxiv 412056; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/412056

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Neuroscience
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (4231)
  • Biochemistry (9124)
  • Bioengineering (6774)
  • Bioinformatics (23981)
  • Biophysics (12115)
  • Cancer Biology (9518)
  • Cell Biology (13772)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (7625)
  • Ecology (11682)
  • Epidemiology (2066)
  • Evolutionary Biology (15500)
  • Genetics (10637)
  • Genomics (14317)
  • Immunology (9476)
  • Microbiology (22825)
  • Molecular Biology (9087)
  • Neuroscience (48943)
  • Paleontology (355)
  • Pathology (1480)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (2567)
  • Physiology (3844)
  • Plant Biology (8324)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (1471)
  • Synthetic Biology (2295)
  • Systems Biology (6184)
  • Zoology (1300)