Abstract
Inhibitory neurons play critical roles in regulating and shaping olfactory responses in vertebrates and invertebrates. In insects, these roles are performed by relatively few neurons, which can be interrogated efficiently, revealing fundamental principles of olfactory coding. Here, with electrophysiological recordings from the locust and a large-scale biophysical model, we analyzed the properties and functions of GGN, a unique giant GABAergic neuron that plays a central role in structuring olfactory codes in the locust mushroom body. Analysis of our in vivo recordings and simulations of our model of the olfactory network suggests that GGN extends the dynamic range of KCs, and leads us to predict the existence of a yet undiscovered olfactory pathway. Our analysis of GGN’s intrinsic properties, inputs, and outputs, in vivo and in silico, reveals basic new features of this critical neuron and the olfactory network that surrounds it.
Introduction
Olfactory information is transformed dramatically as it travels from the periphery to higher order brain centers. Multiple types of olfactory receptor neurons may respond to a given odor with vigorous bursts of action potentials, while neurons deeper in the brain, in the pyriform cortex (vertebrates) or mushroom body (insects), may respond to the same odor with only a spike or two (Laurent & Naraghi, 1994; Friedrich & Laurent, 2001; Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Cang & Isaacson, 2003; Bathellier et al., 2008; Poo & Isaacson, 2009). In these higher order neurons, information about odors is represented sparsely by the identities of the active neurons (population coding) and in the timing of the few spikes elicited in those neurons (temporal coding)(Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Poo & Isaacson, 2009; Stettler & Axel, 2009; Gupta & Stopfer, 2014). Many studies in vertebrates and invertebrates suggest that multiple mechanisms interact to mediate these transformations, including important inhibitory contributions from GABAergic neurons(Luna & Pettit, 2010; Papadopoulou et al., 2011; Lin et al., 2014; Palmer & Harvey, 2014; Large et al., 2016; Large et al., 2018). Here, with intracellular recordings and a new large-scale biophysical model that includes tens of thousands of neurons and spans multiple layers of olfactory processing, we focus on a singularly important inhibitory neuron to investigate the roles of input activity and feedback inhibition in creating a sparse spatio-temporal odor representation in a higher order brain center. Together, our recordings and models point to new functions, neural connectivity patterns, and mechanisms that underlie transformations in the format of olfactory information.
The locust olfactory system is tractable owing to its relative simplicity and is well studied. At rest, olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) are spontaneously active, evoking activity in the antennal lobe’s projection neurons (PNs; Figure 1a)(Joseph et al., 2012). Odorants can elicit increases or decreases in the firing rates of ORNs, and patterns of spikes that can include periods of excitation and inhibition that vary with the odor. The heterogeneous responses of ORNs drive firing patterns in PNs that are further shaped by inhibition from the antennal lobe’s local interneurons (LN)(Raman et al., 2010). Spikes in PNs are also coaxed into rhythmic waves by fast reciprocal interactions between excitatory PNs and inhibitory LNs(MacLeod & Laurent, 1996; Bazhenov et al., 2001). Odor-elicited firing patterns distributed across the population of PNs are informative about the identity, concentration, and timing of the odor(Laurent et al., 1996; Stopfer et al., 2003; Brown et al., 2005). This information is carried by PNs to the mushroom body and the lateral horn. Within the mushroom body, the primary neurons are Kenyon cells (KCs). Unlike the volubly spiking PNs, the KCs are nearly silent at rest and respond very selectively to odors with very few spikes(Laurent & Naraghi, 1994; Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Joseph et al., 2012). Thus, any given odor evokes responses in a small fraction of the KC population, and any KC responds to a small set of odors(Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Stopfer et al., 2003). This sparseness of activity in KCs is thought to arise mainly from two factors: specialized membrane conductances that imbue them with high firing thresholds; and a feedback circuit that tamps down their spiking with cyclic inhibition(Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Demmer & Kloppenburg, 2009; Papadopoulou et al., 2011; Lin et al., 2014). In the locust the main sources of this inhibition are the giant GABAergic neurons (GGNs), one on each side of the brain(Leitch & Laurent, 1996; Papadopoulou et al., 2011; Gupta & Stopfer, 2012).
GGN spans much of each brain hemisphere and branches very widely (Figure 1a). It is reported to receive excitatory input from all 50,000 KCs at synapses within the mushroom body’s α lobe and, in turn, provide inhibitory feedback to all KCs 400-500 μm away within the calyx. In addition, GGN receives inhibitory input from a spiking neuron aptly named “Inhibitor of GGN” (IG) which itself receives inhibition from GGN (Figure 1a, right)(Papadopoulou et al., 2011). GGN is a non-spiking interneuron. Odor presentations, spiking in KCs, and intracellular current injections have all been shown to depolarize GGN, but none of these stimuli causes GGN to generate spikes; even large depolarizations induced by strong intracellular current injections lead only to passive depolarizing responses(Leitch & Laurent, 1996; Papadopoulou et al., 2011); and our own observations).
GGN’s structure is likely an important factor in its function. GGN is very large, and along its path from the α lobe to the calyx, its initially thick processes divide at myriad branch points into vanishingly thin fibers. Cable theory applied to neurons(Rall, 1964) predicts that a passive electrical signal within such a structure will attenuate dramatically as it encounters cytosolic resistance along the neurites, will attenuate further as it divides at the neuronal arbor’s branch points, and will leak out through ionic channels in the cell membrane. Prior studies in invertebrates have shown that 2-5 mV depolarizations in nonspiking interneurons can evoke changes in the membrane potentials of their post-synaptic neurons (Burrows & Siegler, 1978; Manor et al., 1997). If signals originating in GGN’s α lobe attenuate to the extent that they cannot effectively hyperpolarize KCs, then GGN must be exerting its influence on KCs through a different mechanism, perhaps, for example, by inhibiting KCs through local interactions entirely within the mushroom body calyx. To test these ideas, we developed a realistic computational model of GGN to characterize signal attenuation throughout its structure. Our model showed that, although electrical signals undergo substantial attenuation as they travel through GGN, signals arising in its α lobe branches appear to remain strong enough to provide global inhibition to KCs in the calyx.
To further understand the network determinants of GGN’s responses to odors, we recorded from it in vivo while delivering a variety of odors to the animal, and then used our large-scale model to investigate the types of network activity needed to generate these patterns. We identified two novel features in the olfactory network. First, to generate the types of membrane potential patterns we observed in GGN, the synaptic connection strengths onto KCs must be heterogeneous. Second, our in vivo recordings of GGN revealed novel, complex response patterns not previously documented, including periods of hyperpolarization, that vary with the odorant. Although GGN receives reciprocal feedback from IG(Papadopoulou et al., 2011), the periods of hyperpolarization could not be explained by disinhibition of IG from GGN. Instead, our model predicts that this behavior could arise if, in addition to receiving input from GGN, IG also receives direct excitation from another, unknown odor-activated pathway. Additionally, our model replicated emergent features of the olfactory network not explicitly programmed into it, such as the appearance of a small portion of KCs spiking at relatively high rates.
Together, the results of our in vivo recordings and large-scale realistic computational modeling provide a more complete understanding of how different parts of the olfactory system interact. To generate odor-specific temporally patterned responses in GGN and in the mushroom body, temporally-patterned odor evoked excitation from PNs, feedback inhibition from GGN, and inhibition of GGN by odor-driven IG must all cooperate.
Results
GGN morphology
A valuable use of computational modeling is to answer questions about biological systems when those systems are too large, complex, or difficult to study with the tools of physiology alone. Earlier computational studies of the insect olfactory system used relatively simple models of neurons such as integrate and fire or map-based models that collapse entire neuronal structures into a single point(Perez-Orive et al., 2004; Papadopoulou et al., 2011; Arena et al., 2015; Kee et al., 2015; Peng & Chittka, 2017). However, GGN’s giant size, elaborate branching, and passive membrane properties raised questions about its function that could only be addressed by considering properties determined by its morphology. Thus, to understand how the size and shape of GGN affects electrical signal propagation, we constructed a detailed morphological model (available at neuromorpho.org).
To reconstruct the morphology of GGN, we first made intracellular recordings from it in vivo, filled it with dye, and obtained 3D confocal images of the dye-filled cell (Figure 1a, left; Video S1). As previously shown, GGN has a reliable and unique location and morphology(Leitch & Laurent, 1996; Papadopoulou et al., 2011; Gupta & Stopfer, 2012). On each side of the brain, its soma is located ventrally, just anterior to the optic nerve. A single neurite emerges from GGN’s soma, travels toward the posterior and dorsal side of the brain, and splits there into two branches, one innervating the α lobe and the other the mushroom body. Extending outward, these branching neurites expand in width, becoming much thicker than the primary neurite. The mushroom body branch further divides into two thick processes that innervate the medial and the lateral calyx. A thin neurite emerging from the lateral calyceal branch loops back to the lateral horn, close to the soma, and splits there into many branches. We further observed, for the first time, myriad thin fibers that emerge from the stems of the calyceal branches, splitting into very fine feather-like neurites that wrap densely around the peduncle, with some investing the peduncle core (Figure 1b). The neurites in the calyx and lateral horn are dotted with many irregular bouton-like structures (Figure 1c) whereas the branches in α lobe are relatively smooth (Figure 1d).
In two animals we traced and reconstructed the morphology of GGN from confocal image stacks (Figure 1e). Analyzing these traces, we found that the maximum path length of the neuronal trees (i.e., the maximum distance between any two points on the neuronal tree when traversed along the neurites) was on the order of 2mm, and the maximum physical length (i.e., Euclidean distance between any two points on the neuron in three-dimensions) was on the order of 1mm. Some neurites at their thickest were nearly 20μm in diameter. Total traced branch length (i.e., the sum of the lengths of all the neurites) was about 65mm (although many vanishingly thin branches were too fine to trace). Compared to 96,831 vertebrate and invertebrate neurons cataloged in the neuromorpho.org database, GGN fell into the 99.75th percentile for total branch length, and the 99.95th percentile for number of branch points. It is a really big neuron.
Signals travelling through GGN’s arbors attenuate significantly
To investigate GGN’s electrical properties we constructed passive electrical model cells by transferring the morphology tracings of the two GGNs into the NEURON simulator(Carnevale & Hines, 2006). Simulation of both models produced qualitatively similar results; the model we describe here was derived from the second neuron we traced because it was imaged at higher resolution. To account for branches and changes in the diameters of processes that affect electrotonic distance, we segmented the model GGN into 5,283 compartments. We set the membrane resistivity in the model to 33 kΩ-cm2 based on published data obtained from other non-spiking neurons in the locust(Laurent, 1991), the specific membrane capacitance to 1 μF/cm2, the approximate value for cell membranes(Curtis & Cole, 1938; Hodgkin & Huxley, 1952; Gentet et al., 2000), and the cytoplasmic resistivity to 100 Ω-cm, a typical order-of-magnitude value for neurons(Hodgkin & Rushton, 1946; Stuart & Spruston, 1998; Roth & Häusser, 2001).
Feedback signals are thought to travel passively from GGN’s α lobe branch to its calyceal branches. To test the extent of passive signal attenuation through GGN’s structure, we first simulated voltage clamping the base of the α lobe branch of the GGN model (Figure 2a). Intracellular recordings((Papadopoulou et al., 2011); and our own) show GGN’s membrane potential rests at about −51 mV. Because strong odor stimuli depolarize GGN by about 10 mV in recordings made near the base of the α lobe branch(Papadopoulou et al., 2011) (also see Figure 4) we first stepped the clamp voltage to −40 mV, and, after holding it there for 450 ms, measured the resulting depolarizations throughout the model GGN (Figure 2a inset). Notably, the extent of signal attenuation was substantial and varied throughout the calyx with depolarizations ranging from ∼5 - 9 mV. The signal decreased with branch distance from the α lobe, leaving the least amount of signal at the medial portion of the lateral calyceal branch (Figure 2b, c, Video S2).
Since excitatory input typically arrives in GGN not at one point, but rather from many KCs, we then tested a more realistic form of simulated input to the α lobe arbor of GGN by providing nonhomogeneous Poisson spike trains through 500 excitatory model synapses; each synapse had a maximum rate of 20 spikes/s that ramped down linearly to 0 over a 500 ms interval (Figure 2d, e). This stimulus set was calibrated to generate a peak depolarization in the thick branches of GGN in the same range we observed in vivo. This test also revealed significant attenuation of voltage in the neuron’s distant branches (Figure 2e, f) as well as delays in signal propagation (Figure 2 – figure supplement 1).
For GGN, neither membrane resistivity (RM) nor cytoplasmic (axial) resistivity (RA) has been measured definitively; yet, for a given morphology, these two parameters determine signal attenuation. Thus, we explored a range of values for these two parameters with the voltage clamp simulation approach shown in Figure 2a. We based the RM value range on data obtained from many types of neurons provided by the neuroelectro.org database. For RA, neurophysiological data is sparse, so we explored broadly around the range of published values(Hodgkin & Rushton, 1946; Stuart & Spruston, 1998; Roth & Häusser, 2001). As expected, higher RA yielded greater signal attenuation, whereas higher RM yielded less signal attenuation (Figure 2g). This analysis showed that signal transmission in GGN is robust: except for the most extreme values of this parameter range, signals from the α lobe remained strong enough to support synaptic transmission in the calyx. The extent of depolarization throughout GGN’s calyceal arbor varied with location, as quantified in the extended lower lobe in the violin plots in Figure 2c, f and g.
Branches of GGN receiving weaker signals would be expected to provide less inhibition to their postsynaptic KCs. In a simplified model in which all KCs were strongly stimulated by identical input from PNs, the amount of KC spiking was indeed negatively correlated with local GGN voltage deflections (Figure 2 – figure supplement 2). However, in a more realistic model of the mushroom body network including variable excitatory input from PNs and variable strengths of inhibitory synapses between GGN and KCs, we found the negative correlation between depolarizations measured at presynaptic locations throughout GGN and postsynaptic KC activity was small, and likely negligible (Figure 2 – figure supplement 3). This suggests GGN’s inhibitory output has a surprisingly uniform influence across the population of KCs regardless of location.
Feedback inhibition expands the dynamic range of KCs
We used our model to test other basic properties of GGN and the olfactory network surrounding it. In vivo, feedback inhibition from GGN sparsens the odor-elicited responses of KCs by increasing the KC spiking threshold and by restricting KC spiking to brief temporal windows defined by the oscillatory cycle established in the AL(Papadopoulou et al., 2011; Gupta & Stopfer, 2014). Large-scale feedforward inhibition has previously been shown to expand the dynamic range of cortical neurons(Pouille et al., 2009), and in Drosophila, feedback inhibition from APL, the analog of GGN, expands the dynamic range of KCs (Inada, Tsuchimoto, & Kazama, 2017). Whether feedback inhibition from GGN has a similar effect on KCs is unknown. To test this, we extended our model to include, for simplicity, a single KC receiving feedback inhibition from GGN (Figure 3a). To simulate the KC in this test we used a single compartmental model with Hodgkin-Huxley type ion channels(Wüstenberg et al., 2004). Since a single KC would have negligible effect on GGN, we applied its spiking output to GGN’s α lobe branch via 50,000 synapses. Moreover, to avoid unrealistic, strong synchronous input to GGN, we jittered the incoming spike times by applying random synaptic delays between 0 and 60ms. Thus, after each spike generated by the model KC, GGN received 50,000 EPSPs spread over a 60ms time window. We drove the KC model with a range of tonic current injections and compared its responses to those of an isolated KC model receiving the same input without feedback inhibition. As expected, baseline inhibition from spontaneous activity in GGN increased the KC’s threshold for spiking. Notably, though, the GGN-coupled KC continued to spike over a much larger range of current injection than the isolated KC, which quickly saturated to a level where it could no longer spike (Figure 3b, c). A similar result was obtained when we tested the KC by applying simulated GGN inhibition from a model of the olfactory network described later (Figure 3 – figure supplement 1). These results suggested that feedback inhibition from GGN allows an individual KC to function effectively over a larger dynamic range of inputs. In control system analysis, dynamic range is often characterized by the slope of the input-response curve; expanding the dynamic range makes the slope shallower, as we observed in our model (Figures 3c and d; see Methods for the slope calculation).
To test this result in vivo, we made simultaneous intracellular recordings from GGN and KCs. We counted the number of spikes elicited in single KCs by small or large amplitude intracellular current pulses in the presence or absence of a simultaneous small or large amplitude depolarizing current injection to GGN (Figure 3e). In 5 of 5 tested KCs (p<0.033, binomial test), the slope of the input-response curve with increasing current injection became shallower when GGN was depolarized, as predicted by the model (Figure 3f-g), demonstrating that, in vivo, inhibition from GGN increases the dynamic range of KCs.
GGN responses can be complex, including hyperpolarization
Our recordings made in vivo from GGN frequently revealed depolarizations lasting throughout an odor presentation, often with additionally depolarizing peaks corresponding to the onset and offset of the odor (Figure 4, Animal 1, hexanol) (see also (Papadopoulou et al., 2011)). Notably, our recordings from GGN also revealed more complex odor-elicited responses than previously reported, including combinations of depolarization and hyperpolarization (Figure 4, Animal 1, hexanal). Moreover, we found that the same GGN could respond differently when different odors were presented; for example, GGNs from Animals 2 and 3 shown in Figure 4 depolarized in response to one odor and hyperpolarized in response to another. Also, GGNs in different animals could respond differently to the same odor (Figure 4, hexanal). Almost a quarter of the odor-GGN pairs in our in vivo recordings showed reliable hyperpolarizations at some point in the odor response (40 out of 169). Earlier computational models(Papadopoulou et al., 2011; Kee et al., 2015) did not attempt to reproduce the variety of GGN responses we observed in vivo, including hyperpolarizations. To better understand the mechanisms underlying GGN’s odor-elicited responses (and by extension, novel features of olfactory circuitry), we used our GGN model as the center of a more extensive mushroom body olfactory network.
Sustained GGN responses require heterogeneous input to KCs
We sought to determine olfactory network characteristics required to generate realistic responses in GGN because such an analysis could reveal gaps in our understanding of network function. We first tested whether simply simulating the average statistics of olfactory input to the mushroom body network would elicit responses from GGN like those observed in vivo. To test this, we extended our detailed GGN model with a full population of 50,000 simulated KCs. KCs are very small, have few dendritic branches, and generate action potentials; thus, in contrast to the large, complex, and passive GGN, the morphologies of individual KCs are unlikely to differentially influence their odor coding properties. Therefore, we used a relatively simple NEURON version of a single compartmental KC model(Wüstenberg et al., 2004). Each model KC was connected to GGN in the α lobe via an excitatory synapse, and each received inhibitory input from a random segment of GGN in the calyx via a graded synapse (Figure 5a). To provide excitatory input to the KCs, the firing patterns of 830 PNs were simulated as spike-trains, and each KC was stimulated by 50% of these spike trains via excitatory synapses(Jortner et al., 2007). The spike trains were designed to follow the statistics of PNs recorded in vivo(Mazor & Laurent, 2005; Jortner et al., 2007). Thus, 77% of the PN spike trains were assigned a spontaneous firing rate of 2.6 spikes/s; during odor stimulation, 20% of these PNs were switched to 20 spikes/s modulated by the 20 Hz oscillations generated in the antennal lobe and reflected in the local field potential (LFP), and 10% were inhibited (no spikes) (Figure 5b). In this model all the synapses between two given cell types had a fixed conductance. Simulations of this model produced a few highly synchronized bouts of spiking in the KC population (Figure 5c), and corresponding isolated peaks in GGN’s membrane potential (Figure 5d). These unrealistic responses were similar to those generated by earlier models(Papadopoulou et al., 2011; Kee et al., 2015).
Since this simplified model did not reproduce the long-lasting depolarization of GGN we had observed in vivo, we introduced more biological detail. It has been shown that synaptic strengths follow a lognormal distribution(Song et al., 2005; Loewenstein et al., 2011; Buzsáki & Mizuseki, 2014). After we adjusted our network model to include this property, some KCs became weakly inhibited, allowing them to fire more volubly. We also adjusted the input to KCs provided by PNs. In our simplified model, input to the KCs emulated a fixed set of PNs all active throughout the duration of the odor stimulus. However, in vivo, spiking patterns of PNs evolve over the course of an odor presentation, and different PNs respond to the same odor in different ways(Laurent & Davidowitz, 1994; Stopfer et al., 2003; Mazor & Laurent, 2005), thus activating changing sets of KCs. To simulate these diverse responses, we divided the model’s PN population into five groups: four groups responsive to the stimulus and one unresponsive. Odor-elicited spiking within each of the responsive groups started in a subset of its member PNs, and then, in each successive LFP cycle, new PNs were activated (Figure 5e). Lacking the heterogeneity in synaptic strengths onto KCs described above, even this complex activity pattern in PN population produced unrealistically synchronized bouts of activity in KCs, resulting in unrealistic isolated peaks in GGN’s simulated membrane potential (Figure 5f, g). Increasing the strength of input from PNs allowed KCs to spike throughout the stimulus duration and generate sustained depolarization of GGN, but, under this condition, the number of spiking KCs became much higher than observed in vivo(Perez-Orive et al., 2002). Increasing the diversity of synaptic delays from PNs to KCs did not alleviate overly-synchronized spiking in the KC population if heterogeneity in synaptic strengths and temporal patterns of PN activity were omitted from the model (Figure 5 – figure supplement 1). However, we found that combining heterogeneous connectivity with structured PN firing patterns gave rise to GGN voltage traces that included sustained depolarization and temporal dynamics more characteristic of responses we had observed in vivo (Figure 5h, i). Moreover, even the steady PN activity shown in Figure 5b could produce sustained depolarization in GGN when the synaptic strengths were diversified (Figure 5 – figure supplement 2). This analysis, centered on GGN’s responses, revealed important constraints for modeling the olfactory system.
Some KCs fire at high rates in a GGN-centered olfactory network model
The distribution of firing rates of the KC population in our simulations showed that, while most KCs produced 0-2 odor-elicited spikes, a few KCs spiked much more (Figure 6a and Figure 6 – figure supplement 1). To compare the firing rate distribution of KCs in the model to that in vivo, we made patch clamp recordings from 147 KCs in 114 animals, obtaining results from 707 KC-odor pairs. On average, the spontaneous firing rates of these real KCs were very low (∼0.09 Hz) and reached only somewhat higher rates during odor presentation and after odor termination (∼0.15 and ∼0.16 Hz, respectively, Figure 6b), as previously observed(Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Gupta & Stopfer, 2014). Some odor-elicited responses in KCs, though, consisted of many more spikes. Figure 6c shows a representative example of a hyperactive KC, in which a 1s odor pulse elicited an average of 7 spikes (Figures 6d and e). Figure 6f shows spike times obtained from 10 other KC-odor pairs. Overall, the distribution of spike counts in KCs we tested in vivo was clustered close to 0 but included a long rightward tail (Figure 6g), in agreement with our model (Figure 6a). KCs generating especially strong responses were not located in any particular part of the calyx, nor did they feature distinguishing morphologies (data not shown).
Although relatively rare KC odor responses with many spikes have been observed before (Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Gupta & Stopfer, 2014), the origin and significance of these responses were unclear. We tested this with a series of simulations in which we systematically reduced the number of high spike-rate KCs by disconnecting them from the network. When KCs producing more than 5 odor-elicited spikes were removed (Figure 7 – figure supplement 1), a different set of KCs emerged to replaced them by becoming more active; however, the replacement high spike-rate KCs were fewer in number than the original high-firing set (Figure 7a). As a result, the population spike time histogram and corresponding GGN voltage deflections became smaller (Figure 7b and c). As we reiterated this process, deleting newly-emerged high spike-rate KCs from the network with each round, the rightward tail of the KC population’s spike count distribution shortened, the number of low-spike KCs increased (Figure 7a), and, overall, the total number of spikes produced by the entire KC population decreased monotonically. Eventually, when no KCs in the reiterative simulation reached 5 odor-elicited spikes, we decreased the spike limit, first to 3 or more spikes, and then, to any spikes (Figure 7 – figure supplement 1). Throughout this process, GGN’s odor-elicited response gradually decreased, shifting from extended depolarization to unrealistic, isolated peaks. Ultimately, no spiking KCs, or responses in GGN, remained (Figure 7c). Our analysis showed two things: first, the high spike-rate KCs in our model are not a static population, but are rather changing, emergent features of the network’s distribution of synaptic conductances, PN input, and GGN feedback; and second, high-spike rate KCs augment the depolarization of GGN to realistic levels.
Odor-evoked hyperpolarization in GGN originates in IG spiking
Our intracellular recordings from GGN revealed extended periods of odor-elicited hyperpolarization (Figure 4), something not previously reported in recordings made in vivo and not explainable by existing models of GGN within its olfactory network. We hypothesized that these periods of hyperpolarization might originate in the activity of IG, a neuron known to share reciprocal inhibition with GGN (Figure 1a, right)(Papadopoulou et al., 2011). Specifically, we hypothesized that an increase in IG activity might underlie the periods of hyperpolarization in GGN, with IG’s activity increase caused by disinhibition from GGN. To test this, we first tried adding a simple version of IG to our model following the reciprocal connectivity plan shown in Figure 1a. We tested a range of synaptic strengths and time constants for GGN→IG, and IG→GGN synapses, and manipulated baseline firing rates of IG, but none of these efforts could generate odor-elicited hyperpolarization in GGN (data not shown). This hinted at a discrepancy in the causal connection between GGN and IG activity. Intuitively, if IG spiking increased solely because inhibition from GGN decreased, then IG spiking could only increase after GGN returned to baseline. However, as evident in Figure 8, sometimes GGN, recorded in vivo, hyperpolarized at the odor’s onset, ruling out the possibility that disinhibition from GGN is the sole driver of spiking in IG.
The location and most properties of IG are unknown, and we were not able to identify it in our recordings. However, a previous report showed spikes in IG correlate one-to-one with IPSPs in GGN (Papadopoulou et al., 2011), suggesting we could infer IG’s activity by examining GGN’s membrane potential. Our recordings made in vivo from GGN revealed IPSPs in the absence of odors, and that odor presentations elicited an increase in the frequency of IPSPs (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, N=198 pairs, W=2328.5, p << 0.001); responses from 2 animals are shown in Figure 8a, and responses from 1257 trials with several odors from 47 GGNs are shown as a peri-stimulus time histogram in Figures 8b and c. Assuming these IPSPs originate as spontaneous and odor-elicited spikes in IG (Papadopoulou et al., 2011), these results show that IG’s responses to an odor pulse are delayed and lengthy. Notably, as evident in Figure 8b, IG’s firing rate begins to increase before GGN’s membrane potential returns to baseline, suggesting that IG’s odor response cannot be driven solely by disinhibition from GGN. It has been hypothesized that IG receives input from KCs (Papadopoulou et al., 2011). Using our model, we could indeed reproduce realistic hyperpolarization in GGN’s membrane potential by adding excitatory synapses to IG from either the PNs or the KCs (Figures 9a-e). Depending on the PN activity pattern, our simulations produced GGN membrane potentials with hyperpolarization and depolarization (Figure 9f-i).
A remaining question concerned the source of excitation driving spontaneous activity in IG (Figure 9j, top black traces). KCs are nearly silent at rest(Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Gupta & Stopfer, 2014), making it unlikely that they provide the sole source of excitation to IG. PNs, though, spike spontaneously because they receive direct, powerful input from spontaneously active ORNs(Joseph et al., 2012), suggesting a PN-driven pathway might be responsible for spontaneous activity in IG. To test this in vivo, we completely silenced PNs and KCs by bilaterally cutting the animal’s antennal and labral nerves (Joseph et al., 2012) and then recorded intracellularly from GGN. We found that spontaneous IPSPs in GGN persisted in preparations with silenced PNs and KCs (Figure 9j, bottom red traces), demonstrating that IG’s spontaneous spiking either arises intrinsically or is driven by a source other than PNs or KCs.
Discussion
Inhibitory neurons play critical roles in regulating and shaping olfactory responses in vertebrates and invertebrates(Kay & Stopfer, 2006). In insects, these roles are performed by relatively few neurons that can be interrogated efficiently, revealing fundamental principles of olfactory coding. The unique giant GABAergic neuron GGN plays a central role in structuring olfactory codes in the locust mushroom body by regulating the excitability of KCs and parsing their responses into rhythmic bursts. We combined intracellular recordings from GGN and KCs, and developed a new morphologically detailed model of GGN as a focus of analysis to investigate GGN’s properties, inputs, and outputs. Further, we used a broader model of the olfactory system built around GGN to explore several basic properties of the olfactory network. Our new electrophysiological recordings and computational model successfully reproduced the sparse activity of KCs and the membrane dynamics of GGN in the locust brain while providing concrete hypotheses about how the mushroom body circuit processes odor information.
It has been proposed that signals generated within GGN’s α lobe branch propagate to its calyceal branch, where they provide global inhibition to all KCs(Papadopoulou et al., 2011). It is clear that GGN inhibits KCs. But, the enormous size, extensive branching, and passive conduction characterizing GGN raised the hypothesis that signals arising in GGN’s α lobe branch would attenuate to such an extent as they travel to the calyx that they would be unable to effectively inhibit KCs. If this were the case, GGN would presumably inhibit KCs by a different mechanism, such as through local inhibition entirely within the mushroom body calyx. Non-spiking interneurons in insects are often large with complex splays of neurites in separate brain areas, suggesting their far-flung branches may be functionally isolated, serving separate local computations(Burrows, 1981), and a variety of complex, local processing has been proposed to occur within the locust mushroom body calyx (Leitch & Laurent, 1996). Our simulation of GGN’s morphological and electrical properties shows that realistic, odor-elicited levels of depolarizations of 10 mV in GGN’s α lobe branch do indeed attenuate greatly with distance to amplitudes as low as 5 mV in parts of the calyx. However, earlier studies of non-spiking neurons in invertebrates showed depolarizations of this amplitude should suffice to evoke neurotransmitter release. For example, (Burrows & Siegler, 1978) showed that depolarizations of only about 2 mV in a non-spiking interneuron in the metathoracic ganglion of the locust can change the firing rate of its postsynaptic motor neuron. Similarly, (Manor et al., 1997) showed in a graded synapse in the lobster stomatogastric ganglion that voltage steps from −50 mV to −45 mV can reliably evoke postsynaptic effects. Thus, we conclude that GGN has the biophysical capacity to convey signals from KCs at GGN’s α lobe branches to the calyx, providing effective global inhibition to all KCs.
GGN’s arborizations in the calyx extend to different lengths, suggesting signals arising in the α lobe could attenuate more in some of its calyceal branches than in others. Our simulations indeed showed the amount of depolarization reaching GGN’s distant branches varied with their locations, but only by a few millivolts (Figure 2b), within the range of variation caused by other factors. Consistent with this, APL, the Drosophila analog of GGN(Lin et al., 2014) shows different levels of Ca2+ activity in different spatial regions of its arbor when the animal is stimulated with low concentrations of odors (Inada et al., 2017). Inada et al. also found that some KCs are more effective than others at activating APL, and that APL, in turn, provides diverse levels of inhibition to different subsets of KCs in the same brain. By optogenetically activating a small subset of PNs, Inada et al. showed that APL could provide local inhibition to a subset of KCs, likely through dendrodendritic connections between KCs and APL within the calyx. Indeed, reconstructions of APL based on serial section electron microscope images show that it receives synaptic connections from KCs throughout its arbor, including within the calyx(Eichler et al., 2017; Zheng et al., 2018).
In the presence of strong odor stimuli, both GGN and APL appear to provide global inhibition to all KCs(Papadopoulou et al., 2011; Lin et al., 2014), and, as we found (Figure 2), depolarizations evoked by strong input can remain above threshold to inhibit KCs throughout GGN’s arbor. We speculate that, in the locust, weaker stimuli might evoke lower levels of depolarization in GGN’s α lobe branch, leading to lower-amplitude signals that attenuate below the threshold in GGN’s distal branches needed to inhibit KCs. If, GGN, like APL, forms reciprocal synapses with KCs within the calyx, it might be able to inhibit KCs there locally even in the absence of a global inhibitory signal. In this scenario, a KC driven to spike by a weak odor stimulus would effectively inhibit only its close neighbors via local interactions with GGN. Assuming randomly-connected input from PNs to KCs(Jortner et al., 2007; Caron et al., 2013; Eichler et al., 2017), we can speculate that this circuitry could result in a winner-take-all, center-surround-type of contrast enhancement in which only the most strongly driven KCs in each region of GGN’s arbor can respond to an odor. Given stronger odor stimuli (or in the absence of local KC-GGN reciprocal connections in the calyx), signals originating in the α lobe would produce inhibitory output throughout GGN’s arbor, dominate local inhibition, and allow KCs receiving inhibition from the same region of GGN to fire together, reducing the contrast among their responses.
Inhibition from GGN is known to sparsen the firing of KCs and to impose rhythmic time windows on their responses(Papadopoulou et al., 2011; Gupta & Stopfer, 2012). Notably, our model also revealed that feedback inhibition from GGN can expand the dynamic range of inputs able to activate KCs, a result we confirmed in vivo with simultaneous recordings from GGN and KCs (Figure 3). This result is consistent with observations made in Drosophila where blocking inhibitory inputs to KCs narrowed the dynamic range of their odor responses(Inada et al., 2017). Why might expanding the dynamic range of KCs benefit olfactory coding? In the brain, a wide range of synaptic strengths exists even within a given type of neuron, and synaptic strength can change over time and with experience (reviewed in(Barbour et al., 2007)). As in other systems, homeostatic mechanisms might precisely and constantly tune conductances in KCs to match their inputs, enabling them to respond appropriately to a broad and changing range of inputs (reviewed in(Marder & Goaillard, 2006)). Our results suggest an additional mechanism: feedback inhibition from GGN that helps KCs function robustly by expanding their sensitivities to a wider range of inputs, enabling them to generate consistent responses.
Although KCs generally respond very sparsely to any given odor, with very few cells firing just one or a few spikes(Laurent & Naraghi, 1994; Perez-Orive et al., 2004; Mazor & Laurent, 2005), examples of KCs that fire substantially more spikes, spontaneously and in response to odors, have been reported (Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Gupta & Stopfer, 2014). Notably, simulations that gave rise to realistic, sustained responses in GGN always included some high-spike rate KCs. When we removed these high-firing rate KCs from the network, new high-firing rate KCs arose to replace them (Figure 7). This analysis showed that high-firing rate KCs are an emergent property of the olfactory network, determined by the balance of input KCs receive: distributed patterns of excitation from PNs; and rhythmic inhibitory feedback from GGN. Eliminating all high spike-rate KCs from a network had a modest effect on GGN, slightly reducing the amplitude of its sustained, odor-elicited depolarization. Eliminating KCs generating 3-6 spikes per stimulus reduced GGN’s depolarization to unrealistic levels, leaving only widely-separated peaks of activity (Figure 7).
Our intracellular recordings from GGN revealed more elaborate membrane potential temporal dynamics than previously reported, including reliable and prolonged stimulus-dependent periods of hyperpolarization, that varied with odor and animal. In our model, simple reciprocal inhibition of GGN by the inhibitory neuron IG could not reproduce these features. But we found introducing odor-elicited excitatory input to the inhibitory neuron IG could give rise to hyperpolarizations of GGN’s membrane potential (Figure 10). The direct source of odor-elicited excitatory drive to IG is unknown, but could, in principle, be traced to KCs. Indeed, a version of our model in which all KCs synapse upon IG reliably reproduced fairly realistic odor-elicited hyperpolarizations in GGN. This result leads us to hypothesize that KCs, which require strong and coincident input from PNs to spike (Perez-Orive et al., 2002; Jortner et al., 2007) may serve as thresholding elements in the odor pathway from the antennal lobe to IG, transferring odor-evoked activity, but not lower levels of spontaneous activity, from PNs. The identity of this proposed odor pathway is not known.
Why are hyperpolarizations of GGN elicited by only some odors in some animals, as we saw in vivo (Figure 4)? Our simulations suggest that odor-specific patterns of spiking across the PN population (Stopfer et al., 2003) may underlie this variability (Figure 9b-i). Another possibility, not tested here, is that different sets of KCs could drive GGN and IG differentially such that odor-driven KCs strongly activate IG, but only weakly activate GGN, causing GGN to hyperpolarize. Different classes of KCs have been reported to differentially activate APL in Drosophila (Inada et al., 2017), but this possibility remains unexplored in locust.
Our results indirectly suggest IG is spontaneously active (Figures 8, 9). IG may generate this activity on its own or may inherit it from other olfactory or non-olfactory neurons. KCs are nearly silent at rest, making them unlikely direct sources of this activity, but it is possible that even extremely sparse spontaneous activity distributed over the population of 50,000 KCs could suffice to drive spontaneous spiking in IG. This possibility would be difficult to test in vivo.
In summary, we used biophysically detailed simulations in combination with electrophysiology performed in vivo to explore the olfactory circuit of the locust mushroom body. Our intracellular and patch recordings revealed new features of GGN, a neuron that plays a central role in shaping olfactory responses, and of the KC population. These results extend our understanding of the olfactory system, highlighting ways different components interact, and provide new constraints and predictions for additional research.
Materials and Methods
Dissection and electrophysiology
Newly eclosed adult locusts of both sexes picked randomly from our crowded colony were immobilized and the brain was exposed, desheathed and superfused with locust saline as described before(Brown et al., 2005). Our dissection included cutting the labral nerves and removing the suboesophageal ganglion (SOG), precluding stimulation via olfactory receptors in the palps. We usually obtained recordings from one GGN per animal and observed no systematic variation in results over the course of about a year. GGN is not visible from the brain’s surface. To record from GGN, a sharp glass micropipette filled with 2 or 3M potassium acetate with 5% neurobiotin was inserted into the peduncle region of the mushroom body; when impaled, GGN could be identified by its characteristic pattern of IPSPs in the voltage record (Figure 9j). At the end of the recording session, neurobiotin was injected into the cell iontophoretically using 0.2 nA current pulses at 3 Hz for 10 to 20 minutes, allowing the cell’s identity to be confirmed by subsequent imaging. To analyze how the dynamic range of KCs is influenced by GGN, we calculated firing rate slopes, using the following method. After impaling GGN with a microelectrode, KCs were impaled with sharp microelectrodes filled with 1% neurobiotin in 0.5 M potassium acetate. In 10 s trials we applied a 1 s current pulse to the KC at 2 s (A) and 7 s (B) and another current pulse to GGN only at 7 s (Figure 3e). Two amplitudes of current were tested (i1 and i2). Currents injected into KCs were 0.02, 0.05 or 0.1 nA, and into GGN were 0.2, 0.3, 0.5, 0.9 or 1.0 nA, based on pretests to determine currents that effectively depolarized each cell. We counted the number of spikes in the KC over multiple trials and calculated the average firing rate f during current injection under each condition. The slope of firing rate without GGN stimulation was computed as , and the slope with GGN stimulation was .
To test whether IPSPs in GGN originate from spontaneous activity in PNs, we first silenced the PNs by cutting both antennal nerves at the base(Joseph et al., 2012) and then searched for GGN, which could be identified by its ongoing unique pattern of IPSPs and confirmed by its morphology, revealed by subsequent filling with neurobiotin and imaging.
For patch clamp recordings from KCs, the initial dissection was performed as described above. Patch pipettes were pulled to between 7 and 12 MΩ, filled with locust internal solution(Laurent et al., 1993) as well as a neural tracer for subsequent histology (either 12 mM neurobiotin for later conjugation with an Avidin-Alexa complex, or 20 μM Alexa Fluor tracer with absorption wavelengths of 488, 568 or 633). Patch recordings were made in current clamp mode, and data was only analyzed if the observed membrane potential was within the previously reported range for KCs (−55 to −65 mV) and if either LFP or membrane potential oscillations were observed in response to odor stimulation. Firing rates were obtained by smoothing the PSTH with a 100 ms SD Gaussian window.
Stimulus delivery
For GGN recordings, odor pulses were delivered to the ipsilateral antenna as described in (Gupta & Stopfer, 2012).The odorants included 1-hexanol at the dilution of 1% v/v, 1-hexanol, hexanal, methyl benzoate, benzaldehyde, and cyclohexanone mixed at the dilution of 10% v/v in mineral oil, and 100% mineral oil.
For KC recordings, the following odors were delivered similarly: 1-hexanol, hexanal, cyclohexanol, 1-octananol, citral, geraniol, ethyl butyrate, 1-butanol, benzaldehyde, eugenol, 3-methyl-2-butenol, methyl jasmonate, decanal, methyl salicylate, linalool, limonene, pentyl acetate, (all 10% v/v in mineral oil, except methyl salicylate and limonene, at 40% each) and 100% mineral oil.
Histology and immunostaining
Brains were dissected from the head capsule and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde overnight, then conjugated with Avidin-Alexa 568 or Avidin-Alexa 488. Some brains (Figure 1a) were first immunostained with mouse nc82 primary antibody (DSHB Cat# nc82, RRID:AB_2314866, deposited to the DSHB by Buchner, E.) and Alexa 568 conjugated anti-mouse IgG secondary antibody(Shimizu & Stopfer, 2017).
Imaging and neuronal tracing
Brains were dehydrated in an ethanol series and cleared with methyl salicylate or CUBIC(Susaki et al., 2014), mounted in methyl salicylate or mineral oil respectively, and imaged with a Zeiss LSM 710 confocal microscope. Two GGNs were traced in detail from 3D image stacks using NeuroLucida software (MBF Bioscience, Williston, Vermont). The traces were converted to SWC format for further processing and cleanup using NLMorphologyConverter (www.neuronland.org). The two traces were very similar, and models based on both gave similar results. Here, we show results obtained from the GGN imaged at higher resolution.
Statistics
For each of 198 GGN-odor pairs, average IPSP rates were calculated over 5 trials in windows beginning 2 s before, and 2 s after, odor presentations. Wilcoxon-signed rank tests from the scipy package were used to compute the statistic and the two-sided p-value.
Computational model
GGN morphologies in SWC format were imported into NEURON and converted into passive models in NEURON’s hoc format. The resting membrane potential was set to −51 mV, as we have observed in vivo. The single compartmental KC model reported by (Wüstenberg et al., 2004) was translated manually into a NEURON model. This Hodgkin-Huxley type model included fast and slow Na+ channels, and delayed rectifier, transient A type, and slow transient outward K+ channels. The passive reversal potential of the KCs was set to −70 mV. Custom Python scripts were written to set up network models and simulation experiments using NEURON’s Python interface. The simulations were run on NIH’s Biowulf supercomputer cluster (http://hpc.nih.gov) and simulation results were saved in HDF5 based NSDF format(Ray et al., 2015) and later analyzed with custom Python scripts.
PN activity model with a fixed responsive population
Modeled PN spike train rates were based on firing statistics reported in Figure 2c-e in (Mazor & Laurent, 2005)from which we infer 77% of PNs are spontaneously active. Odor presentations were set to evoke spiking in ∼20% of PNs, each spiking at an average rate of 20 Hz(Mazor & Laurent, 2005; Jortner et al., 2007). A 20 Hz sinusoid with amplitude 0.4 times the average spiking rate was further superimposed on odor-elicited spiking to model oscillatory activity generated in the antennal lobe. Based on our own observations in vivo we set 10% of spontaneously active PNs to respond with inhibition to odor presentations. We used a non-homogeneous Poisson generator to create the spike trains based on these rates.
PN activity model with a shifting responsive population
To test the significance of varying temporal structure in PN firing patterns, we assumed ∼30% of the PNs were unresponsive to odors, and divided the other 70% into 4 equally sized groups with the following odor-elicited sequences of excitation (E) and inhibition (I): EEI, EIE, IEI and IIE, with the last epoch occurring upon stimulus offset(Laurent & Davidowitz, 1994). Within each group, excitation epochs featured shifting sets of active PNs, with new PNs recruited during each LFP cycle(Mazor & Laurent, 2005). Each group started its excitatory epoch with activation of 70% of its members and 10% were recruited in each of the next three LFP cycles. In this scheme at most ∼30% of PNs were active at any given time during odor presentations.
Connectivity from GGN to KCs
Each KC received one graded inhibitory synaptic input from a randomly assigned point on GGN’s calyceal branches. The strength of each synapse was adjusted to keep the KC’s membrane potential close to −60 mV when bombarded by spontaneous activity from PNs, as observed in vivo(Joseph et al., 2012). The graded synapse was modeled as a NEURON mechanism based on published descriptions(Manor et al., 1997; Papadopoulou et al., 2011). In some simulations the individual synaptic conductances were selected from a lognormal distribution with the mean adjusted to produce realistic KC activity, i.e., to evoke spiking in about 10% of the KCs with most of them responding with fewer than 5 spikes to odor stimuli. The standard deviation of the lognormal distribution had the same value as the mean, based roughly on (Song et al., 2005). In network simulations we considered KC activity to be realistic if ∼10% or fewer KCs spiked in response to odor stimulus. Also, we considered KC activity to be unrealistically low if it failed to depolarize GGN.
Connectivity from PNs to KCs
Half of the PN population was randomly and independently selected as presynaptic partners for each KC. If two subsets of size m and n are randomly and independently selected from a set of size q, the expected size of their intersection is s = m * n / q. Thus, with 800 PNs and each KC receiving input from 400 PNs, the expected number of PNs shared by any two KCs is 400 * 400 / 800 = 200, i.e., they share about 50% of their presynaptic PNs, as shown in vivo(Jortner et al., 2007). We carried out a parameter search to obtain a preliminary estimate of a PN→KC synaptic conductance sufficient to evoke a few spikes in a single KC model when driven with the expected combined PN firing rate during odor stimuli. Later, in the full network model, we tested values around this conductance along with different GGN→KC conductances. For heterogeneous conductance models, the conductances of individual synapses were selected from a lognormal distribution with the mean adjusted to produce realistic KC activity, as described above.
To test whether diverse synaptic delays could alleviate synchronized spiking in KC population, we created a set of networks where PN→KC synapses were assigned a range of delays from a normal distribution with mean 7.5 ms and standard deviation 3 ms. We chose these values with the animal’s physiology in mind, by assuming a conservative speed of 0.2 m/s = 200 μm/ms for signal propagation through dendrites(Larkum et al., 1996). The length of PN axon is about 1500 μm, so the total time delay from PN to KCs should be under 1500 / 200 = 7.5 ms, consistent with the delay of 6 ms observed in vivo by (Jortner et al., 2007). The maximum diameter of a large KC dendritic arbor is about 600 μm in the calyx and thus, the longest delay between two KCs receiving spikes from the same PN in their soma should be about 3 ms.
IG model and connectivity
To simulate IG, we used a single compartmental Izhikevich-type model of a regular spiking (RS) pyramidal cell from the Model DB repository [https://github.com/ModelDBRepository/39948] modified to include graded synaptic input from GGN. To model IG’s spontaneous firing, sufficient current was injected into it to generate ∼7 spikes/s. A single inhibitory synapse with −80 mV reversal potential connected IG to one of GGN’s basal dendrite segments. The strength and time constant for this synapse were adjusted to produce IPSP amplitudes matching those we observed in vivo. The same GGN segment was connected to IG via a graded synapse. In some simulations either the output of all PNs or the output of all KCs were connected to IG via excitatory synapses. The synaptic weights from KCs to IG were selected from a lognormal distribution.
Data Analysis
Most analyses and 3D visualizations were carried out with custom Python scripts using published modules including numpy, scipy, networkx, matplotlib, h5py, pandas and scikitslearn.
Analyses of patch clamp recordings from KCs were carried out with custom MATLAB scripts.
Data and Software Availability
The morphological reconstruction of GGN will be made publicly available in neuromorpho repository (http://neuromorpho.org/).
The code for setting up and simulating the model is available on request and will be made publicly available in ModelDB repository (https://senselab.med.yale.edu/ModelDB/). Scripts for data analysis are available at github: https://github.com/subhacom/mbnet_analysis.git.
Author contributions
S.R. and M.S. designed the study. Z.A. carried out KC electrophysiology and analyzed data. S.R. carried out GGN and KC electrophysiology, developed computational models and analyzed data. S.R. and M.S. wrote the manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Supplemental Information
Video S1. Related to Figure 1: 3D view of the confocal stack of the dye filled GGN in Figure 1a.
Video S2. Related to Figure 2: 3D view of depolarizations (color coded spheres) at various locations in GGN arbor in simulation described in Figure 2a.
Acknowledgements
We thank Tianming Li and Ian McBain for help with tracing GGN morphology from confocal image stacks, Nitin Gupta (IIT Kanpur, India) for providing a confocal image stack of GGN for a pilot model and helpful advice for making recordings from GGN. We thank the members of the Stopfer lab for feedback and suggestions and Diantao Sun and Kui Sun for excellent animal care. We also thank Vincent Schram and Lynn Holzclaw of the NICHD Microscopy and Imaging Core for help with confocal imaging, George Dold and Bruce Pritchard of NIMH Section on Instrumentation for help with electrophysiology data acquisition setup, and Theodor Usdin, NIMH/NIH, for suggestions on tissue clearing and for providing initial reagents for the CUBIC protocol. We thank the Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank for the nc82 antibody. This work utilized the computational resources of the NIH HPC Biowulf cluster (http://hpc.nih.gov). This work was funded by an intramural grant from NIH-NICHD to M.S.
Footnotes
New experiments and simulations have been added; text has been revised.