Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

Attentional alterations in migraine: a behavioral and M/EEG study

View ORCID ProfileRémy Masson, Yohana Lévêque, Geneviève Demarquay, Hesham ElShafei, Lesly Fornoni, Françoise Lecaignard, Dominique Morlet, Aurélie Bidet-Caulet, Anne Caclin
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/661413
Rémy Masson
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), INSERM UMRS 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Rémy Masson
  • For correspondence: remy.masson@ens-lyon.fr
Yohana Lévêque
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), INSERM UMRS 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Geneviève Demarquay
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), INSERM UMRS 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, FranceNeurological Hospital Pierre Wertheimer, Functional Neurology and Epilepsy Department, Hospices Civils de Lyon and Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hesham ElShafei
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), INSERM UMRS 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lesly Fornoni
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), INSERM UMRS 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Françoise Lecaignard
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), INSERM UMRS 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Dominique Morlet
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), INSERM UMRS 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Aurélie Bidet-Caulet
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), INSERM UMRS 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Anne Caclin
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), INSERM UMRS 1028, CNRS UMR 5292, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Université de Lyon, Lyon, France
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Preview PDF
Loading

ABSTRACT

Migraine is characterized by a hypersensitivity to environmental stimulation which climaxes during attacks but persists interictally. This multisensory disturbance may arise from a dysfunction of top-down and/or bottom-up attention which would lead to the inability to filter out irrelevant information and a state of sensory overload. We used a recent paradigm to evaluate jointly top-down and bottom-up attention among migraineurs and healthy controls using visually-cued target sounds and unexpected task-irrelevant distracting sounds. Behavioral responses and MEG/EEG were recorded. At the behavioral level, neither top-down nor bottom-up attentional processes appeared to be altered in migraine. However, migraineurs presented heightened evoked responses following distracting sounds (orienting component of the N1 and Re-Orienting Negativity, RON) and following target sounds (orienting component of the N1), concomitant to an increased recruitment of the right temporo-parietal junction. They also displayed an increased effect of the cue informational value on target processing resulting in the elicitation of a negative difference (Nd). Based on these results, migraineurs appear to present an increased bottom-up orienting response to all incoming sounds, and an enhanced recruitment of top-down attention. We propose that the interictal state in migraine is characterized by a dysfunction of bottom-up attention and that the hyperfunction of top-down attention acts as a compensatory mechanism enabling them to maintain adequate task-efficiency. These attentional alterations might participate to the disruptions of sensory processing in migraine.

1 Introduction

Migraine is the most common neurological disorder with a prevalence around 10% in the worldwide population [66]. Migraine is mainly characterized by recurrent headache attacks often accompanied by nausea and vomiting, all of which can be disabling and have a vast impact on quality of life. Migraine attacks are strongly associated with photophobia, phonophobia, osmophobia (aversion to visual, auditory and olfactory stimuli, respectively), and allodynia (pain sensitization to non-painful somatosensory stimuli) [24]. These “phobias” encompass both a heightened sensitivity to external stimulation and an exacerbation of pain by those same stimulations. Sensory alterations persist, to a smaller extent, during the attack-free period. Interictally, thresholds for light-induced discomfort or pain [36,67] were found decreased in migraine while intensity of light-induced pain was found exacerbated [14]. Similar results were reported in the auditory modality [36,70] and migraineurs describe a general over-responsiveness to everyday non-noxious stimuli in subjective questionnaires [23,35].

During the last two decades, numerous electrophysiological studies have investigated sensory processing in migraine. They reported a lack of habituation of brain responses to repeated visual stimulations (for a review, see [9]). Deficits of habituation in migraine were described for various event-related potentials (ERPs): sensory components such as the visual P1 and N1 [1,50,62] or later cognitive ERPs such as the P3b [17,64] and the contingent negative variation (CNV) [32,34,61]. Interestingly, those habituation impairments normalize before and during migraine attacks [17,28,31], even though hypersensitivity climaxes during attacks. Impairment of habituation in migraineurs is considered a hallmark of migraine neurophysiology and a biomarker of the interictal state in migraine. However, these results have suffered from a replication crisis [48,49,60]. In the auditory modality, studies investigating habituation deficits in migraine are much scarcer and produced negative results [43,60,71], while other electrophysiological patterns were affected.

During a passive auditory oddball task, enhanced amplitudes of the N1 orienting component [43] and of the P3a [13] have been reported among migraineurs. These two ERPs have been associated with the involuntary orienting of attention [47,54]. In the visual modality, migraineurs were also found to present a heightened involuntary attentional orienting, a decreased ability to suppress unattended stimuli in the periphery, and abnormalities in top-down attentional processes [39]. Overall, these results suggest that migraineurs present alterations in both top-down (voluntary, goal-driven) and bottom-up (involuntary, stimulus-driven) attention. We hypothesize that migraine is associated with a state of sensory overload stemming from exacerbated bottom-up and/or deficient top-down attention processes, resulting in the inability to filter out irrelevant information.

The present study aims to test directly if migraine is associated with attentional abnormalities during the headache-free state and to characterize these potential alterations. Migraineurs and control participants were recruited to perform an adapted version of the Competitive Attention Task [6] while brain activity was monitored using EEG and MEG. This paradigm enables to conjointly evaluate top-down and bottom-up attention, using visually-cued target sounds and unexpected task-irrelevant distracting sounds. Analyses of behavioral performances, event-related potentials, and event-related fields both at the sensor and source levels were conducted to detect any attentional alterations in migraine.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

25 migraine patients (17 female, 8 male) suffering from migraine without aura were included in this study. Inclusion criteria were age between 18 to 60 years old and have a diagnosis of migraine with a reported migraine frequency between 2 to 5 days per month. Exclusion criteria comprised migraine with aura, chronic migraine and migraine preventive medication. Every patient was examined by a neurologist (GD, Hospices Civils de Lyon). Migraine patients filled out the Hospital Depression and Anxiety scale [73], the HIT-6, a short questionnaire aiming to evaluate headache impact on everyday life [30] and the Migraine Disability Assessment Questionnaire (MIDAS) [65]. As we were interested in studying attention during the interictal state, if the patient had a migraine attack during the 72 hours before the testing session, the session was postponed to an ulterior date. If the patient had a migraine attack during the 72 hours after the session, collected data were not used in the analyses. Data from 19 patients (13 female, 6 male) were usable in this study: data from 5 patients were discarded because a migraine attack happened in the 72 hours following the recording session and data from 1 patient because the patient failed to accomplish the task correctly.

19 control participants free of migraine and matched to the patients for sex, age, laterality, education level, and musical education level were included in this study. Exclusion criteria for all subjects included a medical history of psychological or neurological disorders, ongoing background medical treatment other than contraceptive medication, pregnancy and hearing disability. All subjects gave written informed consent and received a monetary compensation for their participation. The study was approved by the local ethical committee (Comité de Protection des Personnes SUD EST III) and registered under the ID number NCT02791997.

2.2 Task and procedure

75 % of the trials consisted in a visual cue (200 ms duration) followed after a 1000 ms delay by an auditory target (100 ms duration with 5 ms rise-time and 5 ms fall-time) (Figure 1a). The cue was centrally presented on a screen (gray background) and could be a green arrow pointing to the left, to the right, or to both sides. The target sounds were monaural pure sounds presented at 25 dB SL. The low-pitched target sound had a fundamental frequency of 512 Hz, the high-pitched target was 2 semi-tones higher than the low-pitched sound (574 Hz). If during training the subject was unable to discriminate the two sounds, the pitch difference could be increased up to 3 semi-tones by steps of half a semi-tone prior to starting EEG/MEG recordings.

Figure 1:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 1:

Protocol. The task was to discriminate between a low- and a high-pitched sound, presented monaurally. A visual cue initiated the trial, and was either informative (50%) or non-informative (50%) about the target ear. 25% of the trials included a distracting sound. (a) Example of an informative trial with no distracting sound: a one-sided visual cue (200 ms duration) indicates in which ear (left or right) the target sound (100 ms duration) will be played after a fixed 1000 ms delay. (b) Example of an uninformative trial with a distracting sound: a two-sided visual cue (200 ms duration) does not provide any indication in which ear (left or right) the target sound will be played. The target sound can be a high- or low-pitched sound indifferently of the cue informational value. In 25% of all trials (with informative or uninformative cues), a loud binaural distracting sound (300 ms duration), such as a clock ring, is played during the cue-target interval at a random delay after the cue offset: the DIS1 condition corresponds to early distracting sounds (starting 50–350 ms after cue offset), the DIS2 condition corresponds to late distracting sounds (starting 350–650 ms after cue offset).

In the other 25 %, the same trial structure was used, but a binaural distracting sound (300 ms duration, 55 dB SL) was played at some point between the cue offset and the target onset (Fig.1b). Trials with a distracting sound starting between 50 ms and 350 ms after the cue offset were classified as DIS1 (early distracting sound), those with a distracting sound starting between 350 ms and 650 ms after the cue offset were classified as DIS2 (late distracting sound), and those with no distracting sound were classified as NoDIS. A total of 40 different ringing sounds were used as distracting sounds (clock-alarm, door-bell, phone ring, etc.) in each participant.

The cue and target categories (i.e., informative vs. uninformative cue, low vs. high-pitched tone) were presented in the same proportion for trials with and without distracting sound. In 25 % of the trials, the cue was indicating left and the target was presented on the left side. In 25 % of the trials, the cue was indicating right and the target was presented on the right side. This leads to a total of 50 % of informative trials. In the last 50 % of the trials, the cue was uninformative, indicating both directions and the target was presented equiprobably on the left or right side. The target type (high or low) was presented in the same proportion (50% each) in all conditions. To compare brain responses to acoustically matched sounds, the same distracting sounds were played in each combination of cue category (informative, uninformative) and distractor condition (DIS1 or DIS2). Each distracting sound was thus played 4 times during the whole experiment, but no more than once during each single block to limit habituation.

Participants were instructed to perform a discrimination task and to respond as fast as possible by pushing or pulling a joystick. The mapping between the targets (low or high) and the responses (pull or push) was counterbalanced across participants, but did not change across the blocks, for each participant. They were asked to allocate their attention to the cued side in the case of informative cues. Participants were informed that informative cues were 100 % predictive and that a distracting sound could be sometimes played. Participants had a 3.4 second response window. At any time in the absence of the visual cue, a blue fixation cross was presented at the center of the screen. Participants were instructed to keep their eyes fixating on the cross and to minimize eye movements while performing the task.

Participants were in a seating position. All stimuli were delivered using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems). Auditory stimuli were delivered through air-conducting plastic ear tubes. First, the auditory threshold was determined for the low-pitched target sound, in each ear, for each participant using the Bekesy tracking method. Second, participants were trained with a short sequence of the task (task difficulty was adjusted if needed, see above). Finally, participants performed 10 blocks of 64 trials of the task (640 trials in total): the whole session lasted around 80 minutes.

2.3 MEG and EEG recording and preprocessing

Simultaneous EEG and MEG data were recorded with a sampling rate of 600Hz during task performance. A 275-channel whole-head axial gradiometer system (CTF-275 by VSM Medtech Inc., Vancouver, Canada) was used to record electromagnetic brain activity (0.016–150Hz filter bandwidth and first-order spatial gradient noise cancellation). Head movements were continuously monitored using 3 coils placed at the nasion and the two preauricular points. EEG was recorded continuously from 7 scalp electrodes placed at frontal (Fz, FC1, FC2), central (Cz), and parietal (Pz) sites, and at the two mastoids (TP9, TP10). The reference electrode was placed on the tip of the nose, the ground electrode on the forehead. One bipolar EOG derivation was recorded from 2 electrodes placed on the supra-orbital ridge of the left eye and infra-orbital ridge of the right eye.

For each participant, a 3D MRI was obtained using a 3T Siemens Magnetom whole-body scanner (Erlangen, Germany), locations of the nasion and the two preauricular points were marked using fiducials markers. These images were used for reconstruction of individual head shapes to create forward models for the source reconstruction procedures (see part 2.6).

MEG and EEG data were processed offline using the software package for electrophysiological analysis (ELAN Pack) developed at the Lyon Neuroscience Research Center [2]. Continuous MEG and EEG data were bandstop-filtered between 47 and 53 Hz, 97 and 103 Hz, and 147 and 153 Hz (zero-phase shift Butterworth filter, order 3) to remove power-line artifacts. An independent component analysis (ICA) was performed on the 0.1-40 Hz band-pass filtered MEG signal to remove eye-movements and heartbeat artifacts. Component topographies and time courses were visually inspected to determine which ones were to be removed through an ICA inverse transformation. 2 to 5 components were removed on the “bandstop-filtered” MEG signal in each participant. Eye artifacts were removed from the EEG signal by applying a linear regression based on the EOG signal.

Only trials for which the participant had answered correctly were retained. Trials contaminated with muscular activity or any other remaining artifacts were excluded automatically using a threshold of 2200 femtoTesla for MEG channels or 150 microvolts for EEG channels. Trials for which the head position differed for more than 10 mm from the median position during the 10 blocks were also excluded from the analyses. For all participants, more than 80 % of trials remained in the analyses after rejection. Finally, both MEG and EEG data were band-pass filtered between 0.2 and 40 Hz (zero-phase shift Butterworth filter, order 3).

2.4 Event-related responses in the sensor space

Event-related fields (ERFs) and potentials (ERPs) were obtained by averaging filtered MEG and EEG data locked to each stimulus event: cue-related responses were locked to cue onset, target-related responses were locked to target onset, and distractor-related responses were locked to distractor onset. A baseline correction was applied based on the mean amplitude of the −100 to 0 ms period before the event. To analyze ERFs/ERPs to distracting sounds, for each distractor onset time-range, surrogate distractor ERFs/ERPs were created in the NoDIS trials and subtracted from the actual distractor ERFs/ERPs. The obtained distractor ERFs/ERPs were thus free of cue-related activity. Time-courses and topographies of ERFs/ERPs were plotted using ELAN software. Please note that regarding distractor-related responses, only responses to early distracting sounds (DIS1) were considered here in order to analyze late components unaffected by target-related responses.

2.5 Source localization of event-related fields

Conventional source reconstruction of MEG data was performed using the Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12) toolbox (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Previously processed ERF data were converted in a SPM-compatible format. Regarding forward modelling, we considered a three-layer realistic Boundary Element Model (BEM), using canonical meshes provided with SPM12 (scalp, inner skull and cortical sheet) and warped to individual MRI to account for each participant anatomy [38]. Forward models were computed with the software OpenMEEG (OpenMEEG Software, https://openmeeg.github.io/, [22]). The estimation of sources was subsequently computed separately for each participant using a LORETA method [52], as implemented in SPM12. We performed inversions on the time-windows of interest defined using the time-courses of ERFs for each studied event (see Supplementary Fig 1). Regarding cue-related responses, we reconstructed the contingent magnetic variation (CMV, 650 to 1200 ms post-cue onset). Regarding distractor-related responses, we reconstructed the magnetic N1 (N1m, 80 to 130 ms), the magnetic early-P3 (early-P3m, 200 to 250 ms), the magnetic late-P3 (late-P3m, 290 to 340 ms) and the magnetic reorienting negativity (RONm, 350 to 500 ms). Regarding target-related responses, we reconstructed the magnetic N1 (N1m, 70 to 150 ms) and the magnetic P300 (P3m, 250 to 400 ms).

Supplementary Figure 1:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Supplementary Figure 1:

ERFs in response to the cue (a), the distracting sound (b) and the target (c) as a function of the cue category (informative or uninformative, plain vs. dashed lines) and the group (control or migraine, blue vs. red lines). Time-courses are presented for two selected MEG sensors. Scalp topographies of the main event-related responses are presented under the time-courses: time-windows chosen for scalp topographies matched those used for source reconstruction of MEG data in the article. For all events, the first vertical bar corresponds to the onset of the stimulus; for the cue, the second vertical bar corresponds to the target onset.

2.6 Statistical analyses

a. Behavioral data

Trials with response before target (false alarm, FA), trials with incorrect responses and trials with no response after target onset and before the next cue onset (miss) were discarded. Percentages of correct responses and median reaction-times (RTs) in the correct trials were computed for each participant and were submitted to three-way repeated-measures ANOVA (rmANOVAs) with CUE category (2 levels: uninformative, informative) and DISTRACTOR condition (3 levels: NoDIS, DIS1, DIS2) as within-subject factors and GROUP category (2 levels: controls, migraineurs) as a between-subject factor. Post-hoc comparisons were conducted using t-tests followed by a Bonferroni correction. For all statistical effects involving more than one degree of freedom in the numerator of the F value, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied to correct for possible violations of the sphericity assumption. We report the uncorrected degree of freedom and the corrected probabilities. Statistical analyses were conducted using the software JASP (version 0.9).

b. ERP – Sensor-level data

For each ERPs, every sample in each electrode within a time-window of interest (650 to 1200 ms for cue-related ERPs, 0 to 650 ms for distractor-related ERPs, and 0 to 500 ms for target-related ERPs) were submitted to a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA (rmANOVAs) with CUE category (2 levels: uninformative, informative) as a within-subject factor and GROUP category (2 levels: controls, migraineurs) as a between-subject factor. Effects were considered significant if p-values remained lower than 0.05 over a 15 ms interval (corresponding to 9 consecutive samples, see [21]).

In case of a GROUP by CUE interaction, post-hoc unpaired t-tests were performed to assess group difference on the ERP difference informative minus uninformative, for every sample within the time-windows that had been found significant with the rmANOVA. Again, effects were considered significant if p-values remained lower than 0.05 over a 15 ms interval (corresponding to 9 consecutive samples).

c. ERF – Source-level data

All statistical analyses regarding the activity of cortical sources were conducted using built-in statistical tools in SPM12. To investigate the GROUP and CUE main effects and the CUE by GROUP interaction, a two-way repeated-measure ANOVA was conducted on the value of source activity for each and every cortical vertex. Significance threshold was 0.05 at the cluster level (p-values corrected for family-wise error, cluster forming threshold=0.05). In order to correct for multiple testing (as several time-windows are inspected, see 2.5 above), a subsequent Bonferroni correction has been applied.

3 Results

Demographics and results of the HAD, HIT-6 and MIDAS questionnaires are displayed in Table 1. The control and migraine group did not significantly differ in terms of age, education, musical education, anxiety and depression scores (all p>0.3).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1:

Demographics and headache profile of the control and migraine groups. Two control participants did not filled the HAD scale. Mean and standard deviation are provided. Group differences are tested using a non-parametric Mann-Whitney U test. NA: not applicable.

3.1 Behavior

Behavioral data are depicted Figure 2. The two groups did not significantly differ in terms of percentage of correct responses (Migraineurs: 91.2 ± 8.2%, Controls: 88.9 ± 10.2%, F1,36=0.92, p=0.34). The percentage of correct responses was not found significantly modulated by the CUE category (F1,36=1.9, p=0.18). The DIS category significantly modulated the percentage of correct responses (F2,72=5.2, ε=0.99, p=0.008), with a significant decrease (p=0.011) in the DIS2 condition (88.1% ± 1.2%) compared to the NoDIS condition (91.5% ± 0.8%) (other post-hoc comparisons p>0.05). No interaction effect was found significant (all p>0.3).

Figure 2:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 2:

Behavioral results. Mean correct responses rate (top) and mean reaction times in milliseconds (bottom) to the target as a function of the GROUP (migraineurs or controls), and as a function of (left) the CUE category (informative, uninformative) or (right) the DISTRACTOR category (NoDIS, DIS1 and DIS2). ***: p<0.001, *: p<0.05, error bars represent the standard error of the mean.

Concerning the median reaction times, both groups did not significantly differ in their performances (Migraineurs: 515 ± 11 ms, Controls: 520 ± 11 ms, F1,36=0.013, p=0.91). A significant main effect of CUE (F1,36=16.1, p<0.001) was observed with participants responding faster in the informative condition than in the uninformative condition. A significant main effect of DISTRACTOR (F2,72=43.8, ε=0.69, p<0.001) was observed with participants responding faster in trials with an early distracting sound (DIS1) (p<0.001) and slower in trials with a late distracting sound (DIS2) (p=0.001) compared to trials without distracting sound (NoDIS) (for information, DIS1 vs. DIS2, p<0.001). No interaction effect was found significant (all p>0.5).

3.2 Event-related responses

Regarding source reconstruction, for every time-window of interest, inversions resulted in an explained variance superior to 95% (group average across the 38 subjects).

a. Cue-related responses

In response to visual cues (Figure 3), participants presented occipital ERPs (obligatory visual ERPs) followed by a fronto-central slow negative wave, the contingent negative variation (CNV), which slowly builds up from around 650 ms to 1200 ms post-cue (corresponding to the target onset). The magnetic counterpart of the CNV, the CMV, was visible at the same latencies (Supplementary Figure 1). The time-window of interest for subsequent analyses was 650-1200 ms post-cue onset.

Figure 3:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 3:

(a) ERPs in response to the visual cues as a function of the cue category (informative or uninformative, plain vs. dashed lines) and the group (control or migraine, blue vs. red lines). Time-courses are presented for all EEG sensors. Scalp topographies of the main cue-related responses are presented on the right. The first vertical bar corresponds to the cue onset, the second to the target onset. Statistical analysis of the ERPs during the CNV time-window (650-1200 ms after cue onset) showed no significant effect. (b) P-value map (masked for corrected p<0.05, the whiter the more significant) of the pattern of increased cueing effect on brain activation in the migraine group during the CMV time-window (650-1200 ms).

In EEG sensor-level data, neither GROUP nor CUE main effect nor CUE by GROUP interaction were found significant during the time-window of interest.

In MEG source-related data, no GROUP main effect was found significant during the CMV (650-1200 ms). Regarding the CUE main effect, a larger activation of the left occipital, motor and frontal cortices, the bilateral temporo-parietal junctions, and the right parietal and temporal cortices (Brodmann area (BA) 6, 19, 22, 39, 44) was found for informative trials compared to uninformative trials (Supplemental Figure 2). Regarding the GROUP by CUE interaction effect, the effect of the cue information (informative – uninformative) was stronger among migraineurs in a cluster including right associative visual areas (BA 7, 19).

Supplementary Figure 2:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Supplementary Figure 2:

P-value map (masked for corrected p<0.05, the whiter the more significant) of the pattern of increased brain activation (a) in informative trials during the CMV in response to the cue (650-1200 ms) and (b) in uninformative trials during the P3m in response to the target (250-400 ms).

b. Distractor-related responses

In response to distracting sounds (Figure 4), participants presented an expected sequence of ERPs. It includes the fronto-central N1, the fronto-central early-P3 (∼270 ms), the fronto-parietal late-P3 (∼330 ms) and the frontal reorienting negativity (RON, ∼410 ms). The fronto-central N1 comprises two subcomponents: the sensory component of N1 (∼95 ms, with polarity inversion at the mastoids) and the orienting component of the N1 (∼130 ms, with no polarity inversion at the mastoids). Their magnetic counterparts, respectively labelled in the following as N1m, early-P3m, late-P3m and RONm, were visible at similar latencies (Supplementary Figure 1).

Figure 4:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 4:

(a) ERPs in response to the distracting sounds as a function of the cue category (informative or uninformative, plain vs. dashed lines) and the group (control or migraine, blue vs. red lines). Time-courses are presented for all EEG sensors. Scalp topographies of the main distractor-related responses are presented below time-courses. GROUP by CUE rmANOVA was applied to ERPs: significant effects (p<0.05 over 15 consecutive ms) correspond to the colored boxes. (b) P-value map (masked for corrected p<0.05, the whiter the more significant) of the pattern of increased brain activation in the migraine group during the RONm time-window (350-500 ms) and the patterns of increased cueing effect on brain activation in the migraine group during the early-P3m (200-250 ms) and the RONm time-windows.

In EEG data, the orienting component of the N1 (138-153 ms) and the RON (440-487 ms then 572-590 ms) were found significantly larger in migraineurs than in controls at Fz. A non-significant trend towards a decreased early-P3 in migraine could be observed. The GROUP by CUE interaction was significant on FC1 in the P50 latency range, prior to the N1 (38-60 ms). Post-hoc analyses confirmed that migraineurs show an increased cueing effect (informative – uninformative) during those latencies, with a more positive deflection in uninformative trials compared to the control group. Regarding the CUE main effect, during the first 150 ms and during the RON from 380 to 550 ms, responses were found significantly more negative in informative trials than in uninformative trials at fronto-central electrodes.

In MEG source-related data, at the latencies of the early-P3m (200-250 ms), migraineurs presented an increased cueing effect (informative – uninformative) in the left superior and middle temporal gyri (BA 21, 22). At the latencies of the RONm (350-500 ms), migraineurs presented a greater activation of the right angular gyrus (BA 39) which is part of the right temporo-parietal junction (rTPJ), and an increased cueing effect (informative – uninformative) in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 9), right frontal eyes fields (Frontal eye fields, BA 8) and right superior parietal lobule and motor cortex (BA 4, 7).

c. Target-related responses

In response to target sounds (Figures 5, 6), in terms of ERPs, participants presented a fronto-central N1 composed of the sensory component of N1 (∼95 ms) and the orienting component of the N1 (∼130 ms), followed by a parietal P300 (after 250 ms). Their magnetic counterparts, respectively labelled in the following as N1m and P3m, were observed at similar latencies (Supplementary Figure 1).

Figure 5:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 5:

(a) ERPs in response to the target sounds as a function of the cue category (informative or uninformative, plain vs. dashed lines) and the group (control or migraine, blue vs. red lines). All EEG sensors are presented. Scalp topographies of the main target-related responses are presented below time-courses. GROUP by CUE rmANOVA was applied to ERPs: significant effects (p<0.05 over 15 consecutive ms) correspond to the colored boxes. (b) P-value map (masked for corrected p<0.05, the whiter the more significant) of the pattern of increased brain activation in the migraine group during the N1m and P3m time-window (respectively 70-150 ms and 250-400 ms).

Figure 6:
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 6:

Difference ERPs in response to the target (informative minus uninformative trials), only Fz is presented here. Significant group effects (p<0.05 over 15 consecutive ms) correspond to the brown boxes. Please note the two peaks of the negative difference (Nd) present in the migraine group but absent for the control group.

In EEG data, the orienting component of the N1 on frontal electrodes (Fz, FC1, and FC2) was found larger in migraineurs than in controls (around 130 ms). The GROUP by CUE interaction was significant on fronto-central electrodes around 125 ms and 300 ms (with a significant CUE main effect between 278 and 317 ms at FC2). Difference ERPs (informative – uninformative, see Figure 6) showed that contrary to controls participants, migraineurs displayed a frontal negative wave (Negative difference, Nd) comprising two mains peaks (∼130 ms and ∼300 ms). Post-hoc analyses showed that these two negatives peaks were significantly more negative among migraineurs on frontal electrodes (Fz, FC1, and FC2).

In MEG source-related data, at the latencies of the N1m (70-150 ms), migraineurs presented a larger activation of the right operculum (BA 40). At the latencies of the P3m (250-400 ms), migraineurs presented a larger activation of the right TPJ. Moreover, at the same latencies, a larger activation of the right frontal cortex (BA 9, 47) and of a cluster comprising the right angular gyrus and right occipital gyri (BA 7, 39) was found significant in uninformative trials compared to informative trials.

4 Discussion

Attention in migraine was investigated here using complementary methods. While behavioral data showed no group differences, EEG data helped to identify the precise bottom-up and top-down attentional processes altered in migraine and MEG source data allowed to pinpoint the cortical correlates underlying those alterations.

4.1 Exacerbated bottom-up attentional effects in migraine

In both participant groups, distracting sounds had opposite behavioral effects depending on the distractor-target interval. Early distracting sounds (DIS1) decreased reaction times compared to the condition without distractor (NoDIS). This facilitation effect has been previously interpreted as an increase in phasic arousal which improves readiness to respond to any incoming stimulus [6,37]. However, late distracting sounds (DIS2) resulted in a deterioration of performances (increase of reaction times) compared to early distracting sounds (DIS1). This has been previously interpreted as the transient effect of attentional capture by the distracting sound [6,37].

There is no observable evidence that the attentional capture and arousal effects of the distracting sounds were different among migraineurs compared to control participants at the behavioral level. This result is in line with a previous study finding no increased impact over performance of visual distractors during a visual cueing task in migraine [40].

However, at the cortical level, migraineurs presented an increased orienting component of the N1 to the distracting sound while the sensory component remained unaltered. The orienting component of the N1 corresponds to the orienting component III described by Näätänen and Picton [47] and is only elicited by infrequent stimuli [4]. It follows the obligatory sensory component of the N1 and it is considered to be linked to the orienting response to unexpected incoming stimuli[4]. Increased N1 has been previously reported in migraine interictally [56] and also specifically its orienting component [13,43]. These results suggest that the orienting response to distractors is increased in migraine. Unaltered sensory component of the N1 (or earlier responses such as the P50) to the distractor or the target sound argues against an early dysfunctional sensory gating in migraine.

The reorienting negativity (RON) was also increased among migraineurs. The RON is considered to reflect the reorienting of attention towards task-relevant stimuli after distraction [45,63] but the exact cognitive function of this response is still a matter of debate [27]. Source reconstruction of MEG data during the RONm time-window revealed an increased activation of the right temporo-parietal junction (rTPJ) in migraineurs. The rTPJ is part of the ventral attentional network considered to be implicated in stimulus-driven attentional control (for a review, see [11]) and is activated by salient unexpected sounds [59]. Therefore, enhanced rTPJ activation could reflect an exacerbated bottom-up attentional capture by the distracting sounds in migraine. The rTPJ has also been proposed to play a crucial role in both voluntary and involuntary shifts of attention [11]. In this line, its increased recruitment could also be the necessary consequence of a disproportionate orienting response towards the distracting sound which calls for a more powerful reorientation process towards the task.

Migraineurs also presented an increased orienting component of the N1 to target sounds compared to control participants. Target sounds appear to induce strong orientation responses in migraineurs despite their predictability and low salience. This is consistent with a previous auditory oddball study which reported increased orienting component of the N1 in migraine even for standard sounds [43]. Moreover, an increased activation of the rTPJ in migraine could be observed during the P300m time-window, confirming the exacerbation of the orienting response towards target sounds among migraineurs.

These results suggest that migraineurs present an increased orienting response towards both expected relevant and unexpected irrelevant sounds, indicating exacerbated bottom-up attentional processes in migraine. This effect would be mediated, at least in part, by the increased recruitment of the rTPJ, a major node of the ventral attention network [12].

4.2 Increased top-down attentional effects in migraineurs

Participants responded faster when the visual cue was informative of the auditory target location, in agreement with previous studies using the Competitive Attention Task [6,16]. This effect has been considered to reflect enhanced anticipatory attention.

The effect of the cue informational value on reaction times was not significantly different between the migraine and the control groups, suggesting no difference in top-down attention at the behavioral level in migraine using this paradigm. To our knowledge, three publications have investigated top-down attention in migraine using visual cueing tasks. None of them observed that migraineurs had a greater top-down attentional enhancement in valid cue trials, which is consistent with our results [39–41]

However, at the cortical level, differences in top-down attentional processes were observed between control participants and migraineurs. During target-related responses, the migraineurs presented a frontal slow negative wave in informative trials compared to uninformative trials, unlike control participants. This resembles the negative difference (Nd), also referred as processing negativity (PN). First described by Hillyard and colleagues [25] using a dichotic listening paradigm, it is observable by subtracting responses from unattended stimuli to responses to attended stimuli and comprises two components generated by auditory cortices (at ∼80 ms and ∼150 ms) and a late frontal component (>300 ms) emerging from non-specific areas [3]. The Nd has been associated with the active selection of relevant information [19,46], suggesting enhanced voluntary attention in migraineurs.

Moreover, the effect of the cue information was found more pronounced among migraineurs in visual association areas during the CMV preceding targets and in temporal areas during the early-P3m to distracting sounds. Interestingly, a similar effect was found during the RONm to distractors in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the superior parietal lobule, two major nodes of the dorsal attentional network implicated in voluntary top-down attention [10,12].

However, no clear evidence of an increased CNV/CMV in migraine could be found using this paradigm. The CNV reflects both attentional anticipation and motor preparation to an imperative stimulus (for a review on the CNV, see [7], for a discussion specific to its magnetic counterpart, the CMV, see [15,21]). These results are inconsistent with previous studies which considered that a wider CNV is a clinical marker of migraine [31,32,34,61], which correlates with disease duration [33,34] and fails to habituate [34,64]. This discrepancy could result from differences in the methods. Previous studies used a simple protocol with a warning signal and an imperative stimulus, separated by a 3-second inter-stimulus interval (while we used here only a one second delay), and the tasks only required motor preparation (while here also attentional processes were at play during the anticipation period).

These results suggest that migraineurs engaged more top-down attention during target processing and anticipation, but also during distractor processing, than control participants.

4.3 Towards a new cognitive model of sensory processing in migraine

We hypothesized that migraine is associated with exacerbated bottom-up and/or deficient top-down attention processes, resulting in increased responsiveness to irrelevant information. In consideration of the present data, the reality appears more complex than our hypothesis:

(1) Increased brain responses to target and distracting sounds do suggest that the orienting response to attended and unattended sounds is exacerbated in migraine. This is quite consistent with anecdotal reports from migraineurs where they mention being easily distracted by their environment [57]. This has been confirmed by a questionnaire study in which migraineurs disclosed a higher level of attention difficulty than healthy controls [35]. It is noteworthy that there exists a comorbidity of migraine with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [18,51,58].

(2) However, at the behavioral level, contrary to our hypothesis, distracting sounds did not have a more pronounced effect on performance in migraine, nor did informative cues have a weaker effect in migraineurs. Literature about cognition and attention in migraine is quite contrasted. Neuropsychological evaluations of migraine patients in the literature did not report any major cognitive impairment during the interictal period [20,53] but some psychometric tests have linked migraine with diverse minor cognitive alterations [5,8,26,42,72]. Few studies have investigated selective attention in migraine: some neuropsychological tests did not find any interictal attentional alterations in adults [29,44]; while children have been reported to perform worse in visual attention tests [55,68,69].

(3) Finally, top-down effects were found increased in migraine as evidenced by event-related potentials and source reconstruction. To our knowledge, increased top-down attention in migraine has never been described in the literature.

How can this be interpreted in term of attentional functions? A good balance between top-down and bottom-up attention is essential to remain task-efficient while still being aware of one’s own environment. Improved top-down attentional functions may be seen as a compensatory mechanism that migraineurs have developed to cope with heightened bottom-up orienting responses for each and every incoming sound. A more efficient top-down attention would maintain the top-down/bottom-up balance at an operational state, preventing any behavioral impairment. However, it is likely that maintaining such an equilibrium in migraine would be costlier in terms of cognitive resources.

Increased bottom-up attention in migraine might be related to sensory overload, as inputs from the environment trigger an orienting response regardless of their actual relevance. This is corroborated by a questionnaire study correlating attentional difficulties and interictal hypersensitivity in migraine [35]. Future studies should aim at exploring links between attention, cognitive load and hypersensitivity in migraine, at cortical and sub-cortical levels.

5 Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the French National Research Agency (ANR) Grant ANR-14-CE30-0001-01 (to Aurélie Bidet-Caulet). This work was performed within the framework of the LABEX CORTEX (ANR-11-LABX-0042) and the LABEX CeLyA (ANR-10-LABX-0060) of Université de Lyon, within the program “Investissements d’Avenir” (ANR-16-IDEX-0005) operated by the French ANR. The acquisition of electrophysiological and imaging data was performed at the CERMEP imaging center, we thank Sebastien Daligault and Claude Delpuech from the MEG department for their technical assistance. The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest regarding the subject matter of this article.

Footnotes

  • ↵* Both last authors

References

  1. [1].↵
    Áfra J, Proietti Cecchini A, Sándor PS, Schoenen J. Comparison of visual and auditory evoked cortical potentials in migraine patients between attacks. Clin Neurophysiol 2000;111:1124–1129.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  2. [2].↵
    Aguera P-E, Jerbi K, Caclin A, Bertrand O. ELAN: A Software Package for Analysis and Visualization of MEG, EEG, and LFP Signals. Intell Neurosci 2011;2011:5:1–5:11.
    OpenUrl
  3. [3].↵
    Alcaini M, Giard M-H, Echallier J-F, Pernier J. Selective auditory attention effects in tonotopically organized cortical areas: A topographic ERP study. Hum Brain Mapp 1994;2:159–169.
    OpenUrl
  4. [4].↵
    Alcaini M, Giard MH, Thévenet M, Pernier J. Two separate frontal components in the N1 wave of the human auditory evoked response. Psychophysiology 1994;31:611–615.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  5. [5].↵
    Annovazzi P, Colombo B, Bernasconi L, Schiatti E, Comi G, Leocani L. Cortical function abnormalities in migraine: neurophysiological and neuropsychological evidence from reaction times and event-related potentials to the Stroop Test. Neurol Sci 2004;25:s285–s287.
    OpenUrl
  6. [6].↵
    Bidet-Caulet A, Bottemanne L, Fonteneau C, Giard M-H, Bertrand O. Brain dynamics of distractibility: interaction between top-down and bottom-up mechanisms of auditory attention. Brain Topogr 2015;28:423–436.
    OpenUrl
  7. [7].↵
    Brunia CHM, van Boxtel GJM. Wait and see. Int J Psychophysiol 2001;43:59–75.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  8. [8].↵
    Calandre E, Bembibre J, Arnedo M, Becerra D. Cognitive disturbances and regional cerebral blood flow abnormalities in migraine patients: their relationship with the clinical manifestations of the illness. Cephalalgia 2002;22:291–302.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  9. [9].↵
    Coppola G, Pierelli F, Schoenen J. Habituation and migraine. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2009;92:249–259.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  10. [10].↵
    Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Ollinger JM, McAvoy MP, Shulman GL. Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2000;3:292–297.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  11. [11].↵
    Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL. The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind. Neuron 2008;58:306–324.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  12. [12].↵
    Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3:201–215.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  13. [13].↵
    Demarquay G, Caclin A, Brudon F, Fischer C, Morlet D. Exacerbated attention orienting to auditory stimulation in migraine patients. Clin Neurophysiol 2011;122:1755–1763.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. [14].↵
    Drummond PD. A Quantitative Assessment of Photophobia in Migraine and Tension Headache. Headache J Head Face Pain 1986;26:465–469.
    OpenUrl
  15. [15].↵
    Elbert T, Rockstroh B, Hampson S, Pantev C, Hoke M. The magnetic counterpart of the contingent negative variation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Potentials Sect 1994;92:262–272.
    OpenUrl
  16. [16].↵
    ElShafei HA, Bouet R, Bertrand O, Bidet-Caulet A. Two Sides of the Same Coin: Distinct Sub-Bands in the Alpha Rhythm Reflect Facilitation and Suppression Mechanisms during Auditory Anticipatory Attention. eNeuro 2018:ENEURO.0141-18.2018.
  17. [17].↵
    Evers S, Quibeldey F, Grotemeyer KH, Suhr B, Husstedt IW. Dynamic changes of cognitive habituation and serotonin metabolism during the migraine interval. Cephalalgia Int J Headache 1999;19:485–491.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  18. [18].↵
    Fasmer OB, Riise T, Lund A, Dilsaver SC, Hundal O, Oedegaard KJ. Comorbidity of migraine with ADHD. J Atten Disord 2012;16:339–345.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. [19].↵
    Giard MH, Fort A, Mouchetant-Rostaing Y, Pernier J. Neurophysiological mechanisms of auditory selective attention in humans. Front Biosci J Virtual Libr 2000;5:D84–94.
    OpenUrl
  20. [20].↵
    Gil-Gouveia R, Oliveira AG, Martins IP. Sequential brief neuropsychological evaluation of migraineurs is identical to controls. Acta Neurol Scand 2016;134:197–204.
    OpenUrl
  21. [21].↵
    Gómez CM, Fernández A, Maestú F, Amo C, González-Rosa JJ, Vaquero E, Ortiz T. Task-specific sensory and motor preparatory activation revealed by contingent magnetic variation. Cogn Brain Res 2004;21:59–68.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  22. [22].↵
    Gramfort A, Papadopoulo T, Olivi E, Clerc M. OpenMEEG: opensource software for quasistatic bioelectromagnetics. Biomed Eng OnLine 2010;9:45.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. [23].↵
    Granovsky Y, Shor M, Shifrin A, Sprecher E, Yarnitsky D, Bar-Shalita T. Assessment of Responsiveness to Everyday Non-Noxious Stimuli in Pain-Free Migraineurs With Versus Without Aura. J Pain 2018. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2018.03.008.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. [24].↵
    Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society (IHS). The International Classification of Headache Disorders, 3rd edition (beta version). Cephalalgia 2013;33:629–808.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  25. [25].↵
    Hillyard SA, Hink RF, Schwent VL, Picton TW. Electrical Signs of Selective Attention in the Human Brain. Science 1973;182:177–180.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. [26].↵
    Hooker WD, Raskin NH. Neuropsychologic Alterations in Classic and Common Migraine. Arch Neurol 1986;43:709–712.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  27. [27].↵
    Horváth J, Winkler I, Bendixen A. Do N1/MMN, P3a, and RON form a strongly coupled chain reflecting the three stages of auditory distraction? Biol Psychol 2008;79:139–147.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  28. [28].↵
    Judit Á, Sándor PS, Schoenen J. Habituation of Visual and Intensity Dependence of Auditory Evoked Cortical Potentials Tends to Normalize Just Before and During the Migraine Attack. Cephalalgia 2000;20:714–719.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  29. [29].↵
    Koppen H, Palm-Meinders I, Kruit M, Lim V, Nugroho A, Westhof I, Terwindt G, van Buchem M, Ferrari M, Hommel B. The impact of a migraine attack and its after-effects on perceptual organization, attention, and working memory. Cephalalgia 2011;31:1419–1427.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  30. [30].↵
    Kosinski M, Bayliss MS, Bjorner JB, Ware JE, Garber WH, Batenhorst A, Cady R, Dahlöf CGH, Dowson A, Tepper S. A six-item short-form survey for measuring headache impact: The HIT-6TM. Qual Life Res 2003;12:963–974.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  31. [31].↵
    Kropp P, Gerber W-D. Contingent Negative Variation during Migraine Attack and Interval: Evidence for Normalization of Slow Cortical Potentials during the Attack. Cephalalgia 1995;15:123–128.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  32. [32].↵
    Kropp P, Gerber W-D. Is increased amplitude of contingent negative variation in migraine due to cortical hyperactivity or to reduced habituation? Cephalalgia 1993;13:37–41.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  33. [33].↵
    Kropp P, Siniatchkin M, Gerber WD. Contingent negative variation as indicator of duration of migraine disease. Funct Neurol 2000;15 Suppl 3:78–81.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  34. [34].↵
    Kropp P, Wallasch T-M, Müller B, Meyer B, Darabaneanu S, Bosse C, Keller A, Meyer W, Gerber W-D. Disease duration of episodic migraine correlates with modified amplitudes and habituation of contingent negative variation. J Neural Transm 2015;122:877–885.
    OpenUrl
  35. [35].↵
    Lévêque Y, Masson R, Fornoni L, Moulin A, Bidet-Caulet A, Caclin A, Demarquay G. Relationships between sensory sensitivity and attention in migraine (in preparation). n.d.
  36. [36].↵
    Main A, Dowson A, Gross M. Photophobia and Phonophobia in Migraineurs Between Attacks. Headache J Head Face Pain 1997;37:492–495.
    OpenUrl
  37. [37].↵
    Masson R, Bidet-Caulet A. Fronto-central P3a to distracting sounds: An index of their arousing properties. NeuroImage 2019;185:164–180.
    OpenUrl
  38. [38].↵
    Mattout J, Henson RN, Friston KJ. Canonical Source Reconstruction for MEG. Comput Intell Neurosci 2007;2007. doi:10.1155/2007/67613.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. [39].↵
    Mickleborough MJ, Hayward J, Chapman C, Chung J, Handy TC. Reflexive attentional orienting in migraineurs: The behavioral implications of hyperexcitable visual cortex. Cephalalgia 2011;31:1642–1651.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  40. [40].↵
    Mickleborough MJS, Ekstrand C, Gould L, Lorentz EJ, Ellchuk T, Babyn P, Borowsky R. Attentional Network Differences Between Migraineurs and Non-migraine Controls: fMRI Evidence. Brain Topogr 2016;29:419–428.
    OpenUrl
  41. [41].↵
    Mickleborough MJS, Truong G, Handy TC. Top–down control of visual cortex in migraine populations. Neuropsychologia 2011;49:1006–1015.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. [42].↵
    Mongini F, Keller R, Deregibus A, Barbalonga E, Mongini T. Frontal lobe dysfunction in patients with chronic migraine: a clinical–neuropsychological study. Psychiatry Res 2005;133:101–106.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  43. [43].↵
    Morlet D, Demarquay G, Brudon F, Fischer C, Caclin A. Attention orienting dysfunction with preserved automatic auditory change detection in migraine. Clin Neurophysiol 2014;125:500–511.
    OpenUrl
  44. [44].↵
    Mulder E, Linssen W, Passchier J, Orlebeke J, de Geus E. Interictal and postictal cognitive changes in migraine. Cephalalgia 1999;19:557–565.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  45. [45].↵
    Munka L, Berti S. Examining task-dependencies of different attentional processes as reflected in the P3a and reorienting negativity components of the human event-related brain potential. Neurosci Lett 2006;396:177–181.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  46. [46].↵
    Näätänen R. Processing negativity: An evoked-potential reflection. Psychol Bull 1982;92:605–640.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  47. [47].↵
    Näätänen R, Picton T. The N1 Wave of the Human Electric and Magnetic Response to Sound: A Review and an Analysis of the Component Structure. Psychophysiology 1987;24:375–425.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  48. [48].↵
    Omland PM, Nilsen KB, Uglem M, Gravdahl G, Linde M, Hagen K, Sand T. Visual Evoked Potentials in Interictal Migraine: No Confirmation of Abnormal Habituation. Headache J Head Face Pain 2013;53:1071–1086.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  49. [49].↵
    Omland PM, Uglem M, Hagen K, Linde M, Tronvik E, Sand T. Visual evoked potentials in migraine: Is the “neurophysiological hallmark” concept still valid? Clin Neurophysiol 2016;127:810–816.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. [50].↵
    Ozkul Y, Bozlar S. Effects of Fluoxetine on Habituation of Pattern Reversal Visually Evoked Potentials in Migraine Prophylaxis. Headache J Head Face Pain 2002;42:582–587.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  51. [51].↵
    Paolino MC, Ferretti A, Villa MP, Parisi P. Headache and ADHD in Pediatric Age: Possible Physiopathological Links. Curr Pain Headache Rep 2015;19:25.
    OpenUrl
  52. [52].↵
    Pascual-Marqui RD, Esslen M, Kochi K, Lehmann D. Functional imaging with low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA): a review. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol 2002;24 Suppl C:91–95.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  53. [53].↵
    Pearson A, Chronicle E, Maylor E, Bruce L. Cognitive Function is not Impaired in People with a Long History of Migraine: A Blinded Study. Cephalalgia 2006;26:74–80.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  54. [54].↵
    Polich J. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:2128–2148.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  55. [55].↵
    Riva D, Usilla A, Aggio F, Vago C, Treccani C, Bulgheroni S. Attention in Children and Adolescents With Headache. Headache J Head Face Pain 2012;52:374–384.
    OpenUrl
  56. [56].↵
    Sable JJ, Patrick TA, Woody PL, Baker KR, Allen-Winters S, Andrasik F. Auditory Event-Related Potentials in the Interictal Phase of Migraine Indicate Alterations in Automatic Attention. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 2017;42:323–333.
    OpenUrl
  57. [57].↵
    Sacks O. Migraine. Berkeley, 1992 p.
  58. [58].↵
    Salem H, Vivas D, Cao F, Kazimi IF, Teixeira AL, Zeni CP. ADHD is associated with migraine: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2017:1–11.
  59. [59].↵
    Salmi J, Rinne T, Koistinen S, Salonen O, Alho K. Brain networks of bottom-up triggered and top-down controlled shifting of auditory attention. Brain Res 2009;1286:155–164.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  60. [60].↵
    Sand T, Vingen JV. Visual, Long-Latency Auditory and Brainstem Auditory Evoked Potentials in Migraine: Relation to Pattern Size, Stimulus Intensity, Sound and Light Discomfort Thresholds and Pre-Attack State. Cephalalgia 2000;20:804–820.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  61. [61].↵
    Schoenen J, Timsit-Berthier M. Contingent Negative Variation: Methods and Potential Interest in Headache, Contingent Negative Variation: Methods and Potential Interest in Headache. Cephalalgia 1993;13:28–32.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  62. [62].↵
    Schoenen J, Wang W, Albert A, Delwaide P j. Potentiation instead of habituation characterizes visual evoked potentials in migraine patients between attacks. Eur J Neurol 1995;2:115–122.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  63. [63].↵
    Schröger E, Wolff C. Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm. Cogn Brain Res 1998;7:71–87.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. [64].↵
    Siniatchkin M, Kropp P, Gerber W-D. What kind of habituation is impaired in migraine patients? Cephalalgia 2003;23:511–518.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  65. [65].↵
    Stewart WF, Lipton RB, Whyte J, Dowson A, Kolodner K, Liberman JN, Sawyer J. An international study to assess reliability of the Migraine Disability Assessment (MIDAS) score. Neurology 1999;53:988–994.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. [66].↵
    Stovner LJ, Hagen K, Jensen R, Katsarava Z, Lipton RB, Scher AI, Steiner TJ, Zwart J-A. The global burden of headache: a documentation of headache prevalence and disability worldwide. Cephalalgia 2007;27:193–210.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  67. [67].↵
    Vanagaite J, Pareja JA, Støren O, White LR, Sand T, Stovner LJ. Light-induced discomfort and pain in migraine. Cephalalgia Int J Headache 1997;17:733–741.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  68. [68].↵
    Villa TR, Agessi LM, Moutran ARC, Gabbai AA, Carvalho D de S. Visual Attention in Children With Migraine The Importance of Prophylaxis. J Child Neurol 2015:0883073815601498.
  69. [69].↵
    Villa TR, Moutran AC, Diaz LS, Pinto MP, Carvalho FA, Gabbai AA, Carvalho D de S. Visual Attention in Children With Migraine: A Controlled Comparative Study. Cephalalgia 2009;29:631–634.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  70. [70].↵
    Vingen JV, Pareja JA, Støren O, White LR, Stovner LJ. Phonophobia in migraine. Cephalalgia Int J Headache 1998;18:243–249.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  71. [71].↵
    Wang W, Schoenen J. Interictal Potentiation of Passive “Oddball” Auditory Event-Related Potentials in Migraine. Cephalalgia 1998;18:261–265.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  72. [72].↵
    Zeitlin C, Oddy M. Cognitive impairment in patients with severe migraine. Br J Clin Psychol 1984;23:27–35.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  73. [73].↵
    Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983;67:361–370.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
View Abstract
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted June 06, 2019.
Download PDF
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Attentional alterations in migraine: a behavioral and M/EEG study
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
Share
Attentional alterations in migraine: a behavioral and M/EEG study
Rémy Masson, Yohana Lévêque, Geneviève Demarquay, Hesham ElShafei, Lesly Fornoni, Françoise Lecaignard, Dominique Morlet, Aurélie Bidet-Caulet, Anne Caclin
bioRxiv 661413; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/661413
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Attentional alterations in migraine: a behavioral and M/EEG study
Rémy Masson, Yohana Lévêque, Geneviève Demarquay, Hesham ElShafei, Lesly Fornoni, Françoise Lecaignard, Dominique Morlet, Aurélie Bidet-Caulet, Anne Caclin
bioRxiv 661413; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/661413

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Neuroscience
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (1526)
  • Biochemistry (2480)
  • Bioengineering (1739)
  • Bioinformatics (9683)
  • Biophysics (3903)
  • Cancer Biology (2972)
  • Cell Biology (4195)
  • Clinical Trials (135)
  • Developmental Biology (2627)
  • Ecology (4102)
  • Epidemiology (2031)
  • Evolutionary Biology (6898)
  • Genetics (5206)
  • Genomics (6501)
  • Immunology (2184)
  • Microbiology (6945)
  • Molecular Biology (2752)
  • Neuroscience (17281)
  • Paleontology (126)
  • Pathology (427)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (706)
  • Physiology (1057)
  • Plant Biology (2489)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (643)
  • Synthetic Biology (831)
  • Systems Biology (2689)
  • Zoology (430)