Skip to main content
bioRxiv
  • Home
  • About
  • Submit
  • ALERTS / RSS
Advanced Search
New Results

Functional reorganization of brain networks across the human menstrual cycle

Laura Pritschet, Tyler Santander, Evan Layher, Caitlin M. Taylor, Shuying Yu, Michael B. Miller, Scott T. Grafton, Emily G. Jacobs
doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/866913
Laura Pritschet
1Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Tyler Santander
1Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Evan Layher
1Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Caitlin M. Taylor
1Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Shuying Yu
1Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Michael B. Miller
1Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA
2Institute for Collaborative Biotechnologies, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA
3Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Scott T. Grafton
1Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA
2Institute for Collaborative Biotechnologies, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Emily G. Jacobs
1Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA
3Neuroscience Research Institute, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: emily.jacobs@psych.ucsb.edu
  • Abstract
  • Full Text
  • Info/History
  • Metrics
  • Supplementary material
  • Preview PDF
Loading

Abstract

The brain is an endocrine organ, sensitive to the rhythmic changes in sex hormone production that occurs in most mammalian species. In rodents and nonhuman primates, estrogen and progesterone’s impact on the brain is evident across a range of spatiotemporal scales. Yet, the influence of sex hormones on the functional architecture of the human brain is largely unknown. In this dense-sampling, deep phenotyping study, we examine the extent to which endogenous fluctuations in sex hormones alter intrinsic brain networks at rest in a woman who underwent brain imaging and venipuncture for 30 consecutive days. Standardized regression analyses illustrate estrogen and progesterone’s widespread influence on cortical dynamics. Time-lagged analyses examined the directionality of these relationships and reveal estrogen’s ability to drive connectivity across major functional brain networks, including the Default Mode and Dorsal Attention Networks, whose hubs are densely populated with estrogen receptors. These results reveal the rhythmic nature in which brain networks reorganize across the human menstrual cycle. Neuroimaging studies that densely sample the individual connectome have begun to transform our understanding of the brain’s functional organization. As these results indicate, taking endocrine factors into account is critical for fully understanding the intrinsic dynamics of the human brain.

Introduction

The brain is an endocrine organ whose day-to-day function is intimately tied to the action of neuromodulatory hormones1–4. Yet, the study of brain-hormone interactions in human neuroscience has often been woefully myopic in scope: the classical approach of interrogating the brain involves collecting data at a single time point from multiple subjects and averaging across individuals to provide evidence for a hormone-brain-behavior relationship. This cross-sectional approach obscures the rich, rhythmic nature of endogenous hormone production. A promising trend in network neuroscience is to flip the cross-sectional model by tracking small samples of individuals over timescales of weeks, months, or years to provide insight into how biological, behavioral, and state-dependent factors influence intra- and inter-individual variability in the brain’s intrinsic network organization5–7. Neuroimaging studies that densely sample the individual connectome are beginning to transform our understanding of the dynamics of human brain organization. However, these studies commonly overlook sex steroid hormones as a source of variability—a surprising omission given that sex hormones are powerful neuromodulators that display stable circadian, infradian, and circannual rhythms in nearly all mammalian species. In the present study, we illustrate robust, time-dependent interactions between the sex steroid hormones 17β-estradiol and progesterone and the functional network organization of the brain over a complete menstrual cycle, offering compelling evidence that sex hormones drive widespread patterns of connectivity in the human brain.

Converging evidence from rodent1, 2, 8, non-human primate9, 10, and human neuroimaging studies11–16 has established the widespread influence of 17β-estradiol and progesterone on regions of the mammalian brain that support higher level cognitive functions. Estradiol and progesterone signaling are critical components of cell survival and plasticity, exerting excitatory and inhibitory effects that are evident across multiple spatial and temporal scales4, 8. The dense expression of estrogen and progesterone receptors (ER; PR) in cortical and subcortical tissue underscores the widespread nature of hormone action. For example, in non-human primates ∼50% of pyramidal neurons in prefrontal cortex (PFC) express ER10 and estradiol regulates dendritic spine proliferation in this region3. In rodents, fluctuations in estradiol across the estrous cycle enhance spinogenesis in hippocampal CA1 neurons and progesterone inhibits this effect1.

During an average human menstrual cycle, occurring every 25-32 days, women experience a ∼12-fold increase in estradiol and an ∼800-fold increase in progesterone. Despite this striking change in endocrine status, we lack a complete understanding of how the large-scale functional architecture of the human brain responds to rhythmic changes in sex hormone production across the menstrual cycle. Much of our understanding of cycle-dependent changes in brain structure1, 17 and function18–20 comes from rodent studies, since the length of the human menstrual cycle (at least 5× longer than rodents’) presents experimental hurdles that make longitudinal studies challenging. A common solution is to study women a few times throughout their cycle, targeting stages that roughly correspond to peak/trough hormone concentrations. Using this ‘sparse-sampling’ approach, studies have examined resting-state connectivity in discrete stages of the cycle13, 14, 21–23; however, some of these findings are undermined by inconsistencies in cycle staging methods, lack of direct hormone assessments, or limitations in functional connectivity methods.

In this dense-sampling, deep-phenotyping study, we assessed brain-hormone interactions over 30 consecutive days representing a complete menstrual cycle. Our results reveal that intrinsic functional connectivity is influenced by hormone dynamics across the menstrual cycle at multiple spatiotemporal resolutions. Estradiol and progesterone conferred robust time-synchronous and time-lagged effects on the brain, demonstrating that intrinsic fluctuations in sex hormones drive changes in the functional network architecture of the human brain. Together, these findings provide insight into how brain networks reorganize across the human menstrual cycle and suggest that consideration of the hormonal milieu is critical for fully understanding the intrinsic dynamics of the human brain.

Results

A healthy, naturally-cycling female (author L.P.; age 23) underwent venipuncture and MRI scanning for 30 consecutive days. The full dataset consists of daily mood, diet, physical activity, and behavioral assessments; task-based and resting-state fMRI; structural MRI; and serum assessments of pituitary gonadotropins and ovarian sex hormones. Neuroimaging data, analysis code, and daily behavioral assessments will be publicly accessible upon publication.

Endocrine assessments

Analysis of daily sex hormone (by liquid-chromatography mass-spectrometry; LC-MS) and gonadotropin (by chemiluminescent immunoassay) concentrations confirmed the expected rhythmic changes of a typical menstrual cycle, with a total cycle length of 27 days. Serum levels of estradiol and progesterone were lowest during menses (day 1-4) and peaked in late follicular (estradiol) and late luteal (progesterone) phases (Figure 1; Table 1). Progesterone concentrations surpassed 5 ng/mL in the luteal phase, signaling an ovulatory cycle24.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 1. Participant’s hormone concentrations plotted by day of cycle.

17β- estradiol, progesterone, luteinizing hormone (LH), and follicle stimulating hormone (FSH) concentrations fell within standard ranges.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 1.

Gonadal and pituitary hormones by cycle stage.

Time-synchronous associations between sex hormones and whole-brain functional connectivity

To begin, we tested the hypothesis that whole-brain functional connectivity at rest is associated with intrinsic fluctuations in estradiol and progesterone in a time-synchronous (i.e. day-by-day) fashion. Based on the enriched expression of ER in PFC10, we predicted that the Default Mode, Frontoparietal Control, and Dorsal Attention Networks would be most sensitive to hormone fluctuations across the cycle. For each session, the brain was parcellated into 400 cortical regions from the Schaefer atlas25 and 15 subcortical regions from the Harvard-Oxford atlas (Figure 2C). A summary time-course was extracted from each parcel, data were temporally-filtered using a maximal overlap discrete wavelet transform (scales 3–6; ∼0.01–0.17 Hz), and 415 × 415 functional association matrices were constructed via magnitude-squared coherence (FDR-thresholded at q < .05; see Methods and Materials for a full description of preprocessing and connectivity estimation). Next, we specified edgewise regression models, regressing coherence against estradiol and progesterone over the 30 days of the study. All data were Z-scored prior to analysis and models were thresholded against empirical null distributions generated through 10,000 iterations of nonparametric permutation testing. Results reported below survived a conservative threshold of p < .001.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 2. Whole-brain functional connectivity at rest is associated with intrinsic fluctuations in estradiol and progesterone.

(A) Time-synchronous (i.e. day-by-day) associations between estradiol and coherence. Hotter colors indicate increased coherence with higher concentrations of estradiol; cool colors indicate the reverse. Results are empirically-thresholded via 10,000 iterations of nonparametric permutation testing (p < .001). Nodes without significant edges are omitted for clarity. (B) Time-synchronous associations between progesterone and coherence. (C) Cortical parcellations were defined by the 400-node Schaefer atlas (shown here). An additional 15 subcortical nodes were defined from the Harvard-Oxford atlas. (D) Mean nodal association strengths by network and hormone. Error bars give 95% confidence intervals. Abbreviations: DMN, Default Mode Network; DorsAttn, Dorsal Attention Network; SalVentAttn, Salience/Ventral Attention Network; SomMot, SomatoMotor Network; TempPar, Temporal Parietal Network.

We observed robust increases in coherence as a function of increasing estradiol across the brain (Figure 2A). When summarizing across networks (computing the mean association strength across network nodes, where strength was defined per graph theory as the sum of positive and negative edge weights linked to each node, independently), components of the Temporal Parietal Network had the strongest positive associations on average, as well as the most variance (Figure 2D). With the exception of Subcortical nodes, all networks demonstrated some level of significantly positive association strength (95% CIs not intersecting zero). We observed a paucity of edges showing inverse associations (connectivity decreasing while estradiol increased), with no networks demonstrating significantly negative association strengths on average. These findings suggest that edgewise functional connectivity is primarily characterized by increased coupling as estradiol rises over the course of the cycle.

Progesterone, by contrast, yielded a widespread pattern of inverse association across the brain, such that connectivity decreased as progesterone rose (Figure 2B). Most networks (with the exception of the Salience/Ventral Attention and SomatoMotor Networks) still yielded some degree of significantly positive association over time; however, the general strength of negative associations was larger in magnitude and significantly nonzero across all networks (Figure 2D). Together, these results align with animal models suggesting excitatory and inhibitory roles for estradiol and progesterone, respectively, manifested here as predominant increases and decreases in functional connectivity across the cycle.

Time-lagged associations between estradiol and whole-brain functional connectivity

We then employed time-lagged methods from dynamical systems analysis to further elucidate the influence of hormonal fluctuations on intrinsic functional connectivity: specifically, vector autoregression (VAR), which supports more directed, causal inference than standard regression models. Here we chose to focus exclusively on estradiol for two reasons: 1) the highly-bimodal time-course of progesterone confers a considerably longer autocorrelative structure, requiring many more free parameters (i.e. higher-order models, ultimately affording fewer degrees of freedom); and 2) progesterone lacks an appreciable pattern of periodicity in its autocovariance with network timeseries, suggesting less relevance for time-lagged analysis over a single cycle. In contrast, estradiol has a much smoother time-course that is well-suited for temporal-evolution models such as VAR.

In short, VAR solves a simultaneous system of equations that predicts current states of the brain and estradiol from the previous states of each. We report results from second-order VAR models: thus, in order to predict connectivity or hormonal states on a given day of the experiment, we consider their values on both the previous day (hereafter referred to as ‘lag 1’) and two days prior (hereafter referred to as ‘lag 2’). See Methods and Materials for an additional mathematical description. Ultimately, if brain variance over time is attributable to previous states of estradiol, this suggests that temporal dynamics in connectivity may be driven (in part) by fluctuations in hormonal states. Vector autoregressive models were specified for each network edge; as before, all data were Z-scored and models were empirically thresholded against 10,000 iterations of nonparametric permutation testing. Surviving edges were significant at the p < .001 level.

When predicting edgewise connectivity states, a powerful disparity emerged between the brain’s autoregressive effects and the effects of estradiol. We observed vast, whole-brain associations with prior hormonal states, both at lag 1 and lag 2 (Figure 3A). Perhaps most immediately striking, the sign of these brain-hormone associations inverts between lags, such that it is predominantly positive at lag 1 and predominantly negative at lag 2—this holds for all networks when considering their nodal association strengths (Figure 3B). We interpret this as a potential regulatory dance between brain states and hormones over the course of the cycle, with estradiol perhaps playing a role in maintaining both steady states (when estradiol is low) and transiently-high dynamics (when estradiol rises). No such pattern emerged in the brain’s autoregressive effects, with sparse, low-magnitude, and predominantly negative associations at lag 1 and lag 2 (Supplementary Figure 1). The flow of effect between estradiol and edgewise connectivity was partially unidirectional. Previous states of coherence predicted estradiol across a number of edges, intersecting all brain networks. This emerged at both lag 1 and lag 2; however, unlike the lagged effects of estradiol on coherence, association strengths were predominantly negative and low-magnitude (on average) at both lags (Supplementary Figure 2). Moreover—and importantly—none of the edges that predicted estradiol were also significantly predicted by estradiol at either lag (i.e. there was no evidence of mutual modulation at any network edge).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 3. Whole-brain functional connectivity is linearly dependent on previous states of estradiol.

(A) Time-lagged associations between coherence and estradiol at lag 1 (left) and lag 2 (right), derived from edgewise vector autoregression models. Hotter colors indicate a predicted increase in coherence given previous concentrations of estradiol; cool colors indicate the reverse. Results are empirically-thresholded via 10,000 iterations of nonparametric permutation testing (p < .001). Nodes without significant edges are omitted for clarity. (B) Mean nodal association strengths by network and time lag. Error bars give 95% confidence intervals.

Time-lagged associations between estradiol and functional network topologies

Given the findings above, we applied the same time-lagged framework to topological states of brain networks in order to better capture the directionality and extent of brain-hormone interactions at the network level. These states were quantified using common graph theory metrics: namely, the participation coefficient (an estimate of between-network integration) and global efficiency (an estimate of within-network integration). As before, all data were Z-scored prior to VAR estimation, and model parameters/fit were compared against 10,000 iterations of nonparametric permutation testing. We focus on significant network-level effects below, but a full documentation of our findings is available in Supplementary Tables 1 and 2.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 2.

VAR model fit: Between-network participation.

Estradiol and between-network participation

As expected, estradiol demonstrated significant autoregressive effects across all models. Previous states of estradiol also significantly predicted between-network integration across several intrinsic networks; however, overall model fit (variance accounted for, R2, and root mean-squared error, RMSE) was at best marginal compared to empirical null distributions of these statistics. For example, in the Dorsal Attention Network (DAN; Figure 4A-B; Table 2), estradiol was a significant predictor of between-network participation both at lag 1 (b = −0.56, SE = 0.25, t = −2.27, p = .035) and at lag 2 (b = 0.53, SE = 0.24, t = 2.16, p = .042). Overall fit for DAN participation, however, rested at the classical frequentist threshold for significance, relative to empirical nulls (R2 = 0.32, p = .049; RMSE = 0.79, p = .050). We observed a similar pattern of results for the Default Mode Network (DMN) and Limbic Network, where lagged states of estradiol significantly predicted cross-network participation, but model fit as a whole was low (see Supplementary Table 1). Interestingly, for all three of these networks, there were no significant autoregressive effects of brain states—previous states of network participation also did not predict estradiol, suggesting that modulation of network topology likely goes from hormones to brain, not the other way around.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 4. Dorsal Attention Network topology is driven by previous states of estradiol.

Observed data (solid lines) vs. VAR model fits (dotted lines) for between-network participation (B, middle) and within-network efficiency (C, right) in the Dorsal Attention Network (A, left). Timeseries for each network statistic are depicted above in (B,C) and estradiol for each VAR is plotted below. Data are in standardized units and begin at experiment day three, given the second-order VAR (lag of two days).

The single exception to this trend was the Visual Network. Prediction of its between-network participation yielded a significant model fit (R2 = 0.37, p = .024; RMSE = 0.79, p = .044). However, this was primarily driven by autoregressive effects of the network at lag 1 (b = −0.39, SE = 0.17, t = −2.30, p = .027) and lag 2 (b = −0.43, SE = 0.17, t = −2.46, p = .024); estradiol yielded a marginal (but nonsignificant) effect only at lag 2 (b = 0.49, SE = 0.24, t = 2.01, p = .058).

Estradiol and global efficiency

In contrast to between-network integration, estradiol was a strong predictor of within-network integration, both in terms of parameter estimates and overall fit. Here, the Default Mode Network provided the best-fitting model (R2 = 0.50, p = .003; RMSE = 0.70, p = .022; Figure 5A-B). As before, estradiol demonstrated significant autoregressive effects at lag 1 (b = 1.15, SE = 0.19, t = 6.15, p < .0001) and lag 2 (b = −0.48, SE = 0.19, t = −2.50, p = .012). When predicting DMN efficiency, previous states of estradiol remained significant both at lag 1 (b = 0.98, SE = 0.23, t = 3.37, p = .0003) and at lag 2 (b = −0.93, SE = 0.23, t = −4.00, p = .002). Critically, these effects were purely directional: prior states of Default Mode efficiency did not predict estradiol, nor did they have significant autoregressive effects, supporting the conclusion that variance in topological network states (perhaps within-network integration, in particular) is primarily accounted for by estradiol—not the other way around (Table 3).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 5. Default Mode Network topology is driven by previous states of estradiol.

Observed data (solid lines) vs. VAR model fits (dotted lines) for within-network efficiency (B, right) in the Default Mode Network (A, left). The efficiency timeseries is depicted above in (B) and estradiol is plotted below. Data are in standardized units and begin at experiment day three, given the second-order VAR (lag of two days).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
  • Download powerpoint
Table 3.

VAR model fit: Global efficiency.

We observed a similar pattern of results in the Dorsal Attention Network (R2 = 0.37, p = .022; RMSE = 0.77, p = .023; Figure 4C; Table 3). Estradiol again demonstrated significant autoregressive effects at lag 1 (b = 1.17, SE = 0.19, t = 6.30, p < .0001) and lag 2 (b = −0.48, SE = 0.19, t = −2.49, p = .011), along with predicting DAN efficiency both at lag 1 (b = 0.84, SE = 0.25, t = 3.35, p = .002) and at lag 2 (b = −0.67, SE = 0.16, t = −2.57, p = .017). As above, Dorsal Attention efficiency had no significant effects on estradiol, nor were there significant autoregressive effects of the network on itself.

The Control and Temporal Parietal networks also yielded partial support for time-dependent modulation of efficiency by estradiol (Control R2 = 0.34, p = .039; Temporal Parietal R2 = 0.36, p = .026). The time-lagged effects of estradiol followed the trends observed above; however, the overall model fit (with respect to prediction error) was not significantly better than their empirical nulls (Control RMSE = 0.83, p = .133; Temporal Parietal RMSE = 0.79, p = .057). Estradiol did not explain a significant proportion of variance in efficiency for any other networks (see Supplementary Table 2 for a complete summary of VAR models for global efficiency).

Discussion

In this dense-sampling, deep-phenotyping project, a naturally-cycling female underwent resting state fMRI and venipuncture for 30 consecutive days, capturing the dynamic endocrine changes that unfold over the course of a complete menstrual cycle. Time-synchronous analyses illustrate estradiol’s widespread impact on cortical dynamics, spanning all but one of the networks in our parcellation. Time-lagged vector autoregressive models tested the temporal directionality of these effects, suggesting that intrinsic network dynamics are driven by recent states of estradiol, particularly with respect to within-network connectivity. Estradiol had the strongest predictive effects on the efficiency of Default Mode and Dorsal Attention Networks, with model fit being strongly driven by ovulation. In contrast to estradiol’s proliferative effects, progesterone was primarily associated with reduced coherence across the whole brain. These results reveal the rhythmic nature of brain network reorganization across the human menstrual cycle.

The network neuroscience community has begun to probe functional networks over the timescale of weeks, months, and years to understand the extent to which brain networks vary between individuals or within an individual over time5, 6, 26–29. These studies indicate that functional networks are dominated by common organizational principles and stable individual features, especially in frontoparietal control regions6, 7, 26, 28. An overlooked feature of these regions is that they are populated with estrogen and progesterone receptors and are exquisitely sensitivity to major changes in sex hormone concentrations11, 12, 15, 16, 30, 31. Our findings demonstrate significant effects of estradiol on functional network nodes belonging to the DMN, DAN, and FCN that overlap with ER-rich regions of the brain, including medial/dorsal PFC10, 32. This study merges the network neuroscience and endocrinology disciplines by demonstrating that higher-order processing systems are modulated by day-to-day changes in sex hormones over the timescale of one month.

Sex hormones regulate brain organization across species

Animal studies offer unambiguous evidence that sex steroid hormones shape the synaptic organization of the brain, particularly in regions that support higher order cognitive functions1–4, 8. In rodents, estradiol increases fast-spiking interneuron excitability in deep cortical layers33. In nonhuman primates, whose reproductive cycle length is similar to humans, estradiol increases the number of synapses in PFC3. Recently, this body of work has also begun to uncover the functional significance of sinusoidal changes in estradiol. For example, estradiol’s ability to promote PFC spinogenesis in ovariectomized animals occurs only if the hormone add-back regime mirrors the cyclic pattern of estradiol release typical of the macaque menstrual cycle9, 34. Pairing estradiol with cyclic administration of progesterone blunts this increase in spine density34. In the hippocampus, progesterone has a similar inhibitory effect on dendritic spines, blocking the proliferative effects of estradiol 6 hours after administration1. Together, the preclinical literature suggests that progesterone antagonizes the largely proliferative effects of estradiol (for review, see Brinton and colleagues35). We observed a similar relationship, albeit at a different spatiotemporal resolution, with estradiol enhancing coherence across cortical networks and progesterone diminishing it. In sum, animal studies have identified estradiol’s influence on regional brain organization at the microscopic scale. Here, we show that estradiol and progesterone’s influence is also evident at the mesoscopic scale of whole-brain activation, measured by spectral coherence, and macroscopic features of network topology.

Resting-state network characteristics differ by cycle stage

Group-based and sparser-sampling neuroimaging studies provide further support that cycle stage and sex hormones impact resting state networks13, 14. Arélin and colleagues36 sampled an individual every 2-3 days across four cycles and found that progesterone was associated with increased connectivity between the hippocampus, dorsolateral PFC, and the sensorimotor cortex, providing compelling evidence that inter-regional connectivity varies over the cycle. However, the sampling rate of this correlational study precluded the authors from capturing the neural effects of day-to-day changes in sex steroid hormones and from testing the temporal directionality of the effect with time-lagged models. Estradiol has both rapid, non-genomic effects and slower, genomic effects on the central nervous system. For example, over the rat estrous cycle, there is a dramatic 30% increase in hippocampal spine density within the 24-hour window in which estradiol concentrations peak. Here, we sought to capture both time-synchronous (rapid) and time-lagged (delayed) effects of sex steroid hormones, sampling every 24 hours for 30 consecutive days. In contrast to Arélin and colleagues, we observed robust, spatially-diffuse negative relationships between progesterone and coherence across the brain, while estradiol enhanced the global efficiency of discrete networks along with between-network integration. Our results illuminate how simultaneous, time-synchronous correlations and causal, time-lagged analysis reveal unique aspects of where and how hormones exert their effect on the brain’s intrinsic networks. Time synchronous analyses illustrate estrogen and progesterone’s widespread influence on cortical coupling. Time-lagged models, which allowed us to examine the temporal direction of those relationships, show that estradiol is driving increased connectivity, particularly in DMN and DAN.

Neurobiological interpretations of hormonal effects and future studies

The following considerations could enhance the interpretation of these data. First, this study represents extensive neural phenotyping of a healthy participant with canonical hormone fluctuations over a reproductive cycle. To enrich our understanding of the relationship between sex hormones and brain function, examining network organization in a hormonally-suppressed female (i.e. an oral contraceptive user) would serve as a valuable comparison. Oral hormonal contraceptives suppress the production of ovarian hormones. If dynamic changes in estradiol are indeed causing increases in resting connectivity, we expect hormonally-suppressed individuals to show blunted functional brain network dynamics over time. Given the widespread use of oral hormonal contraceptives (100 million users worldwide), it is critical to determine whether sweeping changes to an individual’s endocrine state impacts brain states and whether this, in turn, has any bearing on cognition.

Second, in normally-cycling individuals, sex hormones function as proportionally-coupled nonlinear oscillators37. Within-person cycle variability is almost as large as between-person cycle variability, which hints that there are highly-complex hormonal interactions within this regulatory system37, 38. The VAR models we have explored reveal linear dependencies between brain states and hormones, but other dynamical systems methods (e.g. coupled latent differential equations) may offer more biophysical validity37. Unfortunately, the current sample size precludes robust estimation of such a model. Our investigation deeply sampled a single individual across one complete cycle; future studies should enroll a larger sample of women to assess whether individual differences in hormone dynamics drive network changes.

Third, while coherence is theoretically robust to timing differences in the hemodynamic response function, hormones can affect the vascular system39. Therefore, changes in coherence may be due to vascular artifacts that affect the hemodynamic response in fMRI, rather than being neurally-relevant. Future investigations exploring the assumptions of hemodynamics in relation to sex steroid hormone concentrations will add clarity as to how the vascular system’s response to hormones might influence large-scale brain function.

Fourth, these findings contribute to an emerging body of work on estradiol’s ability to enhance the efficiency of PFC-based cortical circuits. In young women performing a working memory task, PFC activity is exaggerated under low estradiol conditions and reduced under high estradiol conditions12. The same pattern is observed decades later in life: as estradiol production decreases over the menopausal transition, working memory-related PFC activity becomes more exaggerated, despite no difference in working memory performance15. Here, we show that day-to-day changes in estradiol drive the global efficiency of functional networks, with the most pronounced effects in networks with major hubs in the PFC. Together, these findings suggest that estradiol generates a neurally efficient PFC response at rest and while engaging in a cognitive task. Estradiol’s action may occur by enhancing dopamine synthesis and release40. The PFC is innervated by midbrain dopaminergic neurons that form the mesocortical dopamine track41. Decades of evidence have established that dopamine signaling enhances the signal-to-noise ratio of PFC pyramidal neurons42 and drives cortical efficiency43–46. In turn, estradiol enhances dopamine synthesis and release and modifies the basal firing rate of dopaminergic neurons47–49, a plausible neurobiological mechanism by which alterations in estradiol could impact cortical efficiency. Future multimodal neuroimaging studies in humans can clarify the link between estradiol’s ability to stimulate dopamine release and the hormone’s ability to drive cortical efficiency within PFC circuits.

Dense-sampling approaches to probe brain-hormone interactions could reveal organizational principles of the functional connectome previously unknown, transforming our understanding of how hormones influence brain states. Human studies implicate sex steroids in the regulation of brain structure and function, particularly within ER-rich regions like the PFC and hippocampus11, 12, 15, 16, 30, 31, 50–52, and yet, the neuroendocrine basis of the brain’s network organization remains understudied. Here, we used a network neuroscience approach to investigate how hormonal dynamics modulate the integration of functional brain networks, showing that estradiol is associated with increased coherence across broad swaths of cortex. At the network level, estradiol enhances the efficiency of most functional networks (with robust effects in DAN and DMN) and, to a lesser extent, increases between-network participation. Moving forward, this network neuroscience approach can be applied to brain imaging studies of other major neuroendocrine transitions, such as pubertal development and reproductive aging (e.g. menopause).

Implications of hormonally regulated network dynamics for cognition

An overarching goal of network neuroscience is to understand how coordinated activity within and between functional brain networks supports cognition. Increased global efficiency is thought to optimize a cognitive workspace53, while between-network connectivity may be integral for integrating top-down signals from multiple higher-order control hubs54. The dynamic reconfiguration of functional brain networks is implicated in performance across cognitive domains, including motor learning55, 56, cognitive control57, and memory58. Our results demonstrate that within- and between-network connectivity of these large-scale networks at rest are hormonally regulated across the human menstrual cycle. Future studies should consider whether these network changes confer advantages to domain-general or domain-specific cognitive performance. Further, planned analyses from this dataset will incorporate task-based fMRI to determine whether the brain’s network architecture is hormonally regulated across the cycle when engaging in a cognitive task, or in the dynamic reconfiguration that occurs when transitioning from rest to task.

Implications of hormonally regulated network dynamics for clinical diagnoses

Clinical network neuroscience seeks to understand how large-scale brain networks differ between healthy and patient populations59, 60. Disruptions in functional brain networks are implicated in a number of neurodegenerative and neuropsychiatric disorders: intrinsic connectivity abnormalities in the DMN are evident in major depressive disorder61 and Alzheimer’s disease62. Notably, these conditions have a sex-skewed disease prevalence: women are at twice the risk for depression and make up two-thirds of the Alzheimer’s disease patient population63. Here, we show that global efficiency in the DMN and DAN are hormonally regulated, with estradiol driving increases in within-network integration. A long history of clinical evidence further implicates sex hormones in the development of mood disorders64–66. For example, the incidence of major depression increases with pubertal onset in females67, chronic use of hormonal contraceptives68, the postpartum period69, and perimenopause70. Moving forward, a network neuroscience approach might have greater success at identifying the large-scale network disturbances that underlie, or predict, the emergence of disease symptomology. Incorporating sex-dependent variables (such as endocrine status) into clinical models. This may be particularly true during periods of profound neuroendocrine change (e.g. puberty, pregnancy, menopause, and use of hormone-based medications, reviewed by Taylor and colleagues71) given that these hormonal transitions are associated with a heightened risk for mood disorders.

Conclusion

In sum, endogenous hormone fluctuations over the reproductive cycle have a robust impact on the intrinsic network properties of the human brain. Despite over 20 years of evidence from rodent, nonhuman primate, and human studies demonstrating the tightly-coupled relationship between our endocrine and nervous systems3, 72, 73, the field of network neuroscience has largely overlooked how endocrine factors shape the brain. The dynamic endocrine changes that unfold over the menstrual cycle are a natural feature of half of the world’s population. Understanding how these changes in sex hormones influence the large-scale functional architecture of the human brain is imperative for our basic understanding of the brain and for women’s health.

End Notes

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (EGJ), the California Nanosystems Institute (EGJ), the Institute for Collaborative Biotechnologies through grant W911NF-19-D-0001 from the U.S. Army Research Office (MBM), and the Rutherford B. Fett Fund (STG). Thanks to Mario Mendoza for phlebotomy and MRI assistance. We would also like to thank Courtney Kenyon, Maggie Hayes, and Morgan Fitzgerald for assistance with data collection.

Author contributions

The overall study was conceived by L.P., C.M.T., S.T.G., and E.G.J.; L.P., T.S., E.L., C.M.T., S.Y., and E.G.J. performed the experiments; data analysis strategy was conceived by T.S. and L.P. and implemented by T.S.; L.P., T.S., and E.G.J. wrote the manuscript; E.L., C.M.T., S.Y., M.B.M., and S.T.G. edited the manuscript.

Data/code availability

MRI data, code, and daily behavioral assessments will be publicly accessible upon publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Methods and Materials

Participants

The participant (author L.P.) was a right-handed Caucasian female, aged 23 years for duration of the study. The participant had no history of neuropsychiatric diagnosis, endocrine disorders, or prior head trauma. She had a history of regular menstrual cycles (no missed periods, cycle occurring every 26-28 days) and had not taken hormone-based medication in the prior 12 months. The participant gave written informed consent and the study was approved by the University of California, Santa Barbara Human Subjects Committee.

Study design

The participant underwent daily testing for 30 consecutive days, with the first test session determined independently of cycle stage for maximal blindness to hormone status. The participant began each test session with a daily questionnaire (see Behavioral assessments), followed by an immersive reality spatial navigation task (not reported here) (Figure 6). Time-locked collection of serum and whole blood started each day at 10:00am, when the participant gave a blood sample. Endocrine samples were collected, at minimum, after two hours of no food or drink consumption (excluding water). The participant refrained from consuming caffeinated beverages before each test session. The MRI session lasted one hour and consisted of structural and functional MRI sequences.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 6. Timeline of data collection for the 30 experiment sessions.

Endocrine and MRI assessments were collected at the same time each day to minimize time-of-day effects.

Behavioral assessments

To monitor state-dependent mood and lifestyle measures over the cycle, the following scales (adapted to reflect the past 24 hours) were administered each morning: Perceived Stress Scale (PSS)74, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI)75, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults (STAI)76, and Profile of Mood States (POMS)77. We observed very few significant relationships between hormone and state-dependent measures following an FDR-correction for multiple comparisons (q < .05)—and critically, none of these state-dependent factors were associated with estradiol (Figure 7A). The participant had moderate levels of anxiety as determined by STAI reference ranges; however, all other measures fell within the ‘normal’ standard range (Figure 7B).

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
Figure 7. Behavioral variation across the 30 day experiment.

(A) Correlation plot showing relationships between mood, lifestyle measures, and sex steroid hormone concentrations. Cooler cells indicate negative correlations, warm cells indicate positive correlations, and white cells indicate no relationship. Asterisks indicate significant correlations after FDR correction (q < .05). (B) Mood and lifestyle measures vary across the cycle. ‘Day 1’ indicates first day of menstruation, not first day of experiment. Abbreviations: LH, Lutenizing hormone; FSH, Follicle-stimulating hormone.

Endocrine procedures

A licensed phlebotomist inserted a saline-lock intravenous line into the dominant or non-dominant hand or forearm daily to evaluate hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis hormones, including serum levels of gonadal hormones (17β-estradiol, progesterone and testosterone) and the pituitary gonadotropins luteinizing hormone (LH) and follicle stimulating hormone (FSH). One 10cc mL blood sample was collected in a vacutainer SST (BD Diagnostic Systems) each session. The sample clotted at room temperature for 45 min until centrifugation (2,000 ×g for 10 minutes) and were then aliquoted into three 1 mL microtubes. Serum samples were stored at -20°C until assayed. Serum concentrations were determined via liquid chromatography-mass spectrometry (for all steroid hormones) and immunoassay (for all gonadotropins) at the Brigham and Women’s Hospital Research Assay Core. Assay sensitivities, dynamic range, and intra-assay coefficients of variation (respectively) were as follows: estradiol, 1 pg/mL, 1–500 pg/mL, < 5% relative standard deviation (RSD); progesterone, 0.05 ng/mL, 0.05–10 ng/mL, 9.33% RSD; testosterone, 1.0 ng/dL, 1–2000 ng/dL, < 4% RSD; FSH and LH levels were determined via chemiluminescent assay (Beckman Coulter). The assay sensitivity, dynamic range, and the intra-assay coefficient of variation were as follows: FSH, 0.2 mIU/mL, 0.2–200 mIU/mL, 3.1–4.3%; LH, 0.2 mIU/mL, 0.2–250 mIU/mL, 4.3–6.4%.

fMRI acquisition and preprocessing

The participant underwent a daily magnetic resonance imaging scan on a Siemens 3T Prisma scanner equipped with a 64-channel phased-array head coil. First, high-resolution anatomical scans were acquired using a T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence (TR = 2500 ms, TE = 2.31 ms, TI = 934 ms, flip angle = 7°; 0.8 mm thickness) followed by a gradient echo fieldmap (TR = 758 ms, TE1 = 4.92 ms, TE2 = 7.38 ms, flip angle = 60◦). Next, the participant completed a 10-minute resting-state fMRI scan using a T2∗-weighted multiband echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence sensitive to the blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) contrast (TR = 720 ms, TE = 37 ms, flip angle = 56°, multiband factor = 8; 72 oblique slices, voxel size = 2 mm3). In an effort to minimize motion, the head was secured with a custom, 3D-printed foam head case (https://caseforge.co/) (days 8-30). Overall motion (mean framewise displacement) was negligible (Supplementary Figure 3), with fewer than 130 microns of motion on average each day. Importantly, mean framewise displacement was also not correlated with estradiol concentrations (Spearman r = −0.06, p = .758).

Initial preprocessing was performed using the Statistical Parametric Mapping 12 software (SPM12, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London) in Matlab. Functional data were realigned and unwarped to correct for head motion and the mean motion-corrected image was coregistered to the high-resolution anatomical image. All scans were then registered to a subject-specific anatomical template created using Advanced Normalization Tools’ (ANTs) multivariate template construction (Supplementary Figure 4). A 4 mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) isotropic Gaussian kernel was subsequently applied to smooth the functional data. Further preparation for resting-state functional connectivity was implemented using in-house Matlab scripts. Global signal scaling (median = 1,000) was applied to account for transient fluctuations in signal intensity across space and time, and voxelwise timeseries were linearly detrended. Residual BOLD signal from each voxel was extracted after removing the effects of head motion and five physiological noise components (CSF + white matter signal). Motion was modeled based on the Friston-24 approach, using a Volterra expansion of translational/rotational motion parameters, accounting for autoregressive and nonlinear effects of head motion on the BOLD signal78. All nuisance regressors were detrended to match the BOLD timeseries.

Functional connectivity estimation

Functional network nodes were defined based on a 400-region cortical parcellation25 and 15 regions from the Harvard-Oxford subcortical atlas (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/). For each day, a summary timecourse was extracted per node by taking the first eigenvariate across functional volumes79. These regional timeseries were then decomposed into several frequency bands using a maximal overlap discrete wavelet transform. Low-frequency fluctuations in wavelets 3–6 (∼0.01–0.17 Hz) were selected for subsequent connectivity analyses80. Finally, we estimated the spectral association between regional timeseries using magnitude-squared coherence: this yielded a 415 × 415 functional association matrix each day, whose elements indicated the strength of functional connectivity between all pairs of nodes (FDR-thresholded at q < .05).

Statistical analysis

First, we assessed time-synchronous variation in functional connectivity associated with estradiol and progesterone through a standardized regression analysis. Data were Z-transformed and edgewise coherence was regressed against hormonal timeseries to capture day-by-day variation in connectivity relative to hormonal fluctuations. For each model, we computed robust empirical null distributions of test-statistics via 10,000 iterations of nonparametric permutation testing—while this process has been shown to adequately approximate false positive rates of 5%81, we elected to report only those edges surviving a conservative threshold of p < .001 to avoid over-interpretation of whole-brain effects.

Next, we sought to capture causal linear dependencies between hormonal fluctuations and network connectivity over time using vector autoregressive (VAR) models. A given VAR model takes a set of variables at time, t, and simultaneously regresses them against previous (time-lagged) states of themselves and each other. For consistency, we only considered second-order VAR models, given a fairly reliable first zero-crossing of brain/hormone autocovariance at lag two. Fit parameters for each VAR therefore reflect the following general form:

Embedded Image

With respect to brain states, we modeled both edgewise coherence and factors related to macroscale network topologies. Specifically, we computed measures of between-network integration (the participation coefficient; i.e. the average extent to which network nodes are communicating with other networks over time) and within-network integration (global efficiency, quantifying the ostensible ease of information transfer across nodes inside a given network). Regardless of brain measure, each VAR was estimated similarly to the time-synchronous analyses described above: data were Z-scored, models were fit, and all effects were empirically-thresholded against 10,000 iterations of nonparametric permutation testing.

Finally, for each set of edgewise models (time-synchronous and time-lagged), we attempted to disentangle both the general direction of hormone-related associations and whether certain networks were more or less susceptible to hormonal fluctuations. Toward that end, we estimated nodal association strengths per graph theory’s treatment of signed, weighted networks—that is, positive and negative association strengths were computed independently for each node by summing the positive and negative edges linked to them (after empirical thresholding), respectively. We then simply assessed mean association strengths across the various networks in our parcellation.

Here, networks were defined by grouping the subnetworks of the 17-network Schaefer parcellation, such that (for example), the A, B, and C components of the Default Mode Network were treated as one network. We chose this due to the presence of a unique Temporal Parietal Network in the 17-network partition, which is otherwise subsumed by several other networks (Default Mode, Salience/Ventral Attention, and SomatoMotor) in the 7-network partition. The subcortical nodes of the Harvard-Oxford atlas were also treated as their own network, yielding a total of nine networks. These definitions were subsequently used for computation of participation coefficients and global efficiencies in network-level VAR models.

Brain data visualization

Statistical maps of edgewise coherence v. hormones were visualized using the Surf Ice software (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/surfice/).

References

  1. [1].↵
    Woolley CS, McEwen BS (1993) Roles of estradiol and progesterone in regulation of hippocampal dendritic spine density during the estrous cycle in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 336(2):293–306.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  2. [2].↵
    Frick KM, Kim J, Tuscher JJ, Fortress AM (2015) Sex steroid hormones matter for learning and memory: Estrogenic regulation of hippocampal function in male and female rodents. Learn. Mem. 22(9):472–493.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. [3].↵
    Hara Y, Waters EM, McEwen BS, Morrison JH (2015) Estrogen effects on cognitive and synaptic health over the lifecourse. Physiol. Rev. 95(3):785–807.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. [4].↵
    Galea LAM, Frick KM, Hampson E, Sohrabji F, Choleris E (2017) Why estrogens matter for behavior and brain health. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 76(Pt B):363–379.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. [5].↵
    Poldrack RA, et al. (2015) Long-term neural and physiological phenotyping of a single human. Nat. Commun. 6:8885.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. [6].↵
    Gordon EM, et al. (2017) Precision functional fapping of individual human brains. Neuron 95(4):791–807.
    OpenUrl
  7. [7].↵
    Gratton C, et al. (2018) Functional brain networks are dominated by stable group and individual factors, not cognitive or daily variation. Neuron 98(2):439–452.
    OpenUrl
  8. [8].↵
    Frick KM, Kim J, Koss WA (2018) Estradiol and hippocampal memory in female and male rodents. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 23:65–74.
    OpenUrl
  9. [9].↵
    Hao J, et al. (2006) Estrogen alters spine number and morphology in prefrontal cortex of aged female rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 26(9):2571–2578.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. [10].↵
    Wang ACJ, Hara Y, Janssen WGM, Rapp PR, Morrison JH (2010) Synaptic estrogen receptor-alpha levels in prefrontal cortex in female rhesus monkeys and their correlation with cognitive performance. J. Neurosci. 30(38):12770–12776.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. [11].↵
    Berman KF, et al. (1997) Modulation of cognition-specific cortical activity by gonadal steroids: a positron-emission tomography study in women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94(16):8836–8841.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  12. [12].↵
    Jacobs E, D’Esposito M (2011) Estrogen shapes dopamine-dependent cognitive processes: Implications for women’s health. J. Neurosci. 31(14):5286–5293.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. [13].↵
    Petersen N, Kilpatrick LA, Goharzad A, Cahill L (2014) Oral contraceptive pill use and menstrual cycle phase are associated with altered resting state functional connectivity. Neuroimage 90:24–32.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  14. [14].↵
    Lisofsky N, et al. (2015) Hippocampal volume and functional connectivity changes during the female menstrual cycle. Neuroimage 118:154–162.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. [15].↵
    Jacobs EG, et al. (2016) Reorganization of functional networks in verbal working memory circuitry in early midlife: The impact of sex and menopausal status. Cereb. Cortex 27(5):2857–2870.
    OpenUrl
  16. [16].↵
    Jacobs EG, et al. (2016) Impact of sex and menopausal status on episodic memory circuitry in early midlife. J. Neurosci. 36(39):10163–10173.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. [17].↵
    Sheppard PAS, Choleris E, Galea LAM (2019) Structural plasticity of the hippocampus in response to estrogens in female rodents. Mol. Brain 12(1):22.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  18. [18].↵
    Warren SG, Juraska JM (1997) Spatial and nonspatial learning across the rat estrous cycle. Behav. Neurosci. 111(2):259–266.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  19. [19].
    Hampson E, Levy-Cooperman N, Korman JM (2014) Estradiol and mental rotation: Relation to dimensionality, difficulty, or angular disparity? Horm. Behav. 65(3):238–248.
    OpenUrl
  20. [20].↵
    Kim J, Frick KM (2017) Distinct effects of estrogen receptor antagonism on object recognition and spatial memory consolidation in ovariectomized mice. Psychoneuroendocrinology 85:110–114.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. [21].
    Hjelmervik H, Hausmann M, Osnes B, Westerhausen R, Specht K (2014) Resting states are resting traits—an fmri study of sex differences and menstrual cycle effects in resting state cognitive control networks. PLoS One 9(7):e103492.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. [22].
    De Bondt T, et al. (2015) Stability of resting state networks in the female brain during hormonal changes and their relation to premenstrual symptoms. Brain Res. 1624:275–285.
    OpenUrl
  23. [23].
    Syan SK, et al. (2017) Influence of endogenous estradiol, progesterone, allopregnanolone, and dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate on brain resting state functional connectivity across the menstrual cycle. Fertil. Steril. 107(5):1246–1255.
    OpenUrl
  24. [24].↵
    Leiva R, Bouchard T, Boehringer H, Abulla S, Ecochard R (2015) Random serum progesterone threshold to confirm ovulation. Steroids 101:125–129.
    OpenUrl
  25. [25].↵
    Schaefer A, et al. (2018) Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity mri. Cereb. Cortex 28(9):3095–3114.
    OpenUrl
  26. [26].↵
    Finn ES, et al. (2015) Functional connectome fingerprinting: Identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nat. Neurosci. 18(11):1664–1671.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. [27].
    Betzel RF, et al. (2019) The community structure of functional brain networks exhibits scale-specific patterns of inter- and intra-subject variability. Neuroimage.
  28. [28].↵
    Horien C, Shen X, Scheinost D, Constable RT (2019) The individual functional connectome is unique and stable over months to years. Neuroimage 189:676–687.
    OpenUrl
  29. [29].↵
    Seitzman BA, et al. (2019) Trait-like variants in human functional brain networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116(45):22851–22861.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. [30].↵
    Hampson E, Morley EE (2013) Estradiol concentrations and working memory performance in women of reproductive age. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38(12):2897–2904.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  31. [31].↵
    Shanmugan S, Epperson CN (2014) Estrogen and the prefrontal cortex: towards a new understanding of estrogen’s effects on executive functions in the menopause transition. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 35(3):847–865.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. [32].↵
    Yeo BTT, et al. (2011) The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106(3):1125–1165.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  33. [33].↵
    Clemens AM, et al. (2019) Estrus-cycle regulation of cortical inhibition. Curr. Biol. 29(4):605–615.
    OpenUrl
  34. [34].↵
    Ohm DT, et al. (2012) Clinically relevant hormone treatments fail to induce spinogenesis in prefrontal cortex of aged female rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 32(34):11700–11705.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. [35].↵
    Brinton RD, et al. (2008) Progesterone receptors: form and function in brain. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 29(2):313–339.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  36. [36].↵
    Arelin K, et al. (2015) Progesterone mediates brain functional connectivity changes during the menstrual cycle-a pilot resting state mri study. Front. Neurosci. 9:44.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. [37].↵
    1. Molenaar PC,
    2. Lerner R,
    3. Newll K
    Boker SM, Neale MC, Klump KL (2014) A differential equations model for the ovarian hormone cycle., eds. Molenaar PC, Lerner R, Newll K. (Guilford Press, New York), pp. 369–391.
  38. [38].↵
    Fehring RJ, Schneider M, Raviele K (2006) Variability in the phases of the menstrual cycle. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Neonatal. Nurs. 35(3):376–384.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  39. [39].↵
    Krause DN, Duckles SP, Pelligrino DA (2006) Influence of sex steroid hormones on cerebrovascular function. J. Appl. Physiol. 101(4):1252–1261.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  40. [40].↵
    Creutz LM, Kritzer MF (2002) Estrogen receptor-beta immunoreactivity in the midbrain of adult rats: regional, subregional, and cellular localization in the a10, a9, and a8 dopamine cell groups. J. Comp. Neurol. 446(3):288–300.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  41. [41].↵
    Kritzer MF, Creutz LM (2008) Region and sex differences in constituent dopamine neurons and immunoreactivity for intracellular estrogen and androgen receptors in mesocortical projections in rats. J. Neurosci. 28(38):9525–9535.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  42. [42].↵
    Williams GV, Goldman-Rakic PS (1995) Modulation of memory fields by dopamine d1 receptors in prefrontal cortex. Nature 376(6541):572–575.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  43. [43].↵
    Cai JX, Arnsten AF (1997) Dose-dependent effects of the dopamine d1 receptor agonists a77636 or skf81297 on spatial working memory in aged monkeys. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 283(1):183–189.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. [44].
    Granon S, et al. (2000) Enhanced and impaired attentional performance after infusion of d1 dopaminergic receptor agents into rat prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 20(3):1208–1215.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. [45].
    Gibbs SEB, D’Esposito M (2005) Individual capacity differences predict working memory performance and prefrontal activity following dopamine receptor stimulation. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5(2):212–221.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  46. [46].↵
    Vijayraghavan S, Wang M, Birnbaum SG, Williams GV, Arnsten AFT (2007) Inverted-u dopamine d1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 10(3):376–384.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  47. [47].↵
    Thompson TL, Moss RL (1994) Estrogen regulation of dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens: genomic- and nongenomic-mediated effects. J. Neurochem. 62(5):1750–1756.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  48. [48].
    Pasqualini C, Olivier V, Guibert B, Frain O, Leviel V (1995) Acute stimulatory effect of estradiol on striatal dopamine synthesis. J. Neurochem. 65(4):1651–1657.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  49. [49].↵
    Becker JB (1990) Estrogen rapidly potentiates amphetamine-induced striatal dopamine release and rotational behavior during microdialysis. Neurosci. Lett. 118(2):169–171.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  50. [50].↵
    Jacobs EG, et al. (2015) 17beta-estradiol differentially regulates stress circuitry activity in healthy and depressed women. Neuropsychopharmacology 40(3):566–576.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. [51].
    Girard R, et al. (2017) Hormone therapy at early post-menopause increases cognitive control-related prefrontal activity. Sci. Rep. 7:44917.
    OpenUrl
  52. [52].↵
    Zeydan B, et al. (2019) Association of bilateral salpingo-oophorectomy before menopause onset with medial temporal lobe neurodegeneration. JAMA Neurol. 76(1):95–100.
    OpenUrl
  53. [53].↵
    Bullmore ET, Bassett DS (2011) Brain graphs: graphical models of the human brain connectome. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 7:113–140.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. [54].↵
    Gratton C, Sun H, Petersen SE (2018) Control networks and hubs. Psychophysiology 55(3).
    OpenUrl
  55. [55].↵
    Bassett DS, et al. (2011) Dynamic reconfiguration of human brain networks during learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108(18):7641–7646.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. [56].↵
    Mattar MG, et al. (2018) Predicting future learning from baseline network architecture. Neuroimage 172:107–117.
    OpenUrl
  57. [57].↵
    Seeley WW, et al. (2007) Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27(9):2349–2356.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. [58].↵
    Fornito A, Harrison BJ, Zalesky A, Simons JS (2012) Competitive and cooperative dynamics of large-scale brain functional networks supporting recollection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109(31):12788–12793.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  59. [59].↵
    Fox MD, Greicius M (2010) Clinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:19.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. [60].↵
    Hallquist MN, Hillary FG (2019) Graph theory approaches to functional network organization in brain disorders: A critique for a brave new small-world. Netw. Neurosci. 3(1):1–26.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. [61].↵
    Greicius MD, et al. (2007) Resting-state functional connectivity in major depression: Abnormally increased contributions from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus. Biol. Psychiatry 62(5):429–437.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  62. [62].↵
    Buckner RL, et al. (2009) Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 29(6):1860–1873.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. [63].↵
    Nebel RA, et al. (2018) Understanding the impact of sex and gender in alzheimer’s disease: A call to action. Alzheimers Dement. 14(9):1171–1183.
    OpenUrl
  64. [64].↵
    Plotsky PM, Owens MJ, Nemeroff CB (1998) Psychoneuroendocrinology of depression: hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Psychiatr. Clin. North. Am. 21(2):293–307.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  65. [65].
    Young EA, Korszun A (2002) The hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis in mood disorders. Endocrinol. Metab. Clin. North. Am. 31(1):63–78.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  66. [66].↵
    Rubinow DR, Schmidt PJ (2006) Gonadal steroid regulation of mood: the lessons of premenstrual syndrome. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 27(2):210–216.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. [67].↵
    Angold A, Costello EJ (2006) Puberty and depression. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 15(4):919–937.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  68. [68].↵
    Young EA, et al. (2007) Influences of hormone-based contraception on depressive symptoms in premenopausal women with major depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32(7):843–853.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  69. [69].↵
    Bloch M, et al. (2000) Effects of gonadal steroids in women with a history of postpartum depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 157(6):924–930.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  70. [70].↵
    Schmidt PJ, Rubinow DR (2009) Sex hormones and mood in the perimenopause. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1179:70–85.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  71. [71].↵
    Taylor CM, Pritschet L, Yu S, Jacobs EG (2019) Applying a women’s health lens to the study of the aging brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:224.
    OpenUrl
  72. [72].↵
    McEwen BS (2018) Redefining neuroendocrinology: Epigenetics of brain-body communication over the life course. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 49:8–30.
    OpenUrl
  73. [73].↵
    Beltz AM, Moser JS (2019) Ovarian hormones: A long overlooked but critical contributor to cognitive brain structures and function. Ann N Y Acad Sci.
  74. [74].↵
    Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R (1983) A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health. Soc. Behav. 24(4):385–396.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  75. [75].↵
    Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ (1989) The pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28(2):193–213.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  76. [76].↵
    Spielberger CD, Vagg PR (1984) Psychometric properties of the STAI: A reply to ramanaiah, franzen, and schill. J. Pers. Assess. 48(1):95–97.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. [77].↵
    Pollock V, Cho DW, Reker D, Volavka J (1979) Profile of mood states: the factors and their physiological correlates. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 167(10):612–614.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. [78].↵
    Friston KJ, Williams S, Howard R, Frackowiak RS, Turner R (1996) Movement-related effects in fmri time-series. Magn. Reson. Med. 35(3):346–355.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  79. [79].↵
    Friston KJ, Rotshtein P, Geng JJ, Sterzer P, Henson RN (2006) A critique of functional localisers. Neuroimage 30(4):1077–1087.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  80. [80].↵
    Patel AX, Bullmore ET (2016) A wavelet-based estimator of the degrees of freedom in denoised fmri time series for probabilistic testing of functional connectivity and brain graphs. Neuroimage 142:14–26.
    OpenUrl
  81. [81].↵
    Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H (2016) Cluster failure: Why fmri inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113(28):7900–7905.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top
PreviousNext
Posted December 06, 2019.
Download PDF

Supplementary Material

Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word about bioRxiv.

NOTE: Your email address is requested solely to identify you as the sender of this article.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Functional reorganization of brain networks across the human menstrual cycle
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from bioRxiv
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the bioRxiv website.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Share
Functional reorganization of brain networks across the human menstrual cycle
Laura Pritschet, Tyler Santander, Evan Layher, Caitlin M. Taylor, Shuying Yu, Michael B. Miller, Scott T. Grafton, Emily G. Jacobs
bioRxiv 866913; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/866913
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo LinkedIn logo Mendeley logo
Citation Tools
Functional reorganization of brain networks across the human menstrual cycle
Laura Pritschet, Tyler Santander, Evan Layher, Caitlin M. Taylor, Shuying Yu, Michael B. Miller, Scott T. Grafton, Emily G. Jacobs
bioRxiv 866913; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/866913

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Subject Area

  • Neuroscience
Subject Areas
All Articles
  • Animal Behavior and Cognition (4105)
  • Biochemistry (8808)
  • Bioengineering (6509)
  • Bioinformatics (23446)
  • Biophysics (11784)
  • Cancer Biology (9200)
  • Cell Biology (13314)
  • Clinical Trials (138)
  • Developmental Biology (7430)
  • Ecology (11403)
  • Epidemiology (2066)
  • Evolutionary Biology (15143)
  • Genetics (10430)
  • Genomics (14036)
  • Immunology (9167)
  • Microbiology (22142)
  • Molecular Biology (8802)
  • Neuroscience (47539)
  • Paleontology (350)
  • Pathology (1427)
  • Pharmacology and Toxicology (2489)
  • Physiology (3729)
  • Plant Biology (8076)
  • Scientific Communication and Education (1437)
  • Synthetic Biology (2220)
  • Systems Biology (6036)
  • Zoology (1252)