Skip to main content
Log in

Joint angle variability in 3D bimanual pointing: uncontrolled manifold analysis

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The structure of joint angle variability and its changes with practice were investigated using the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) computational approach. Subjects performed fast and accurate bimanual pointing movements in 3D space, trying to match the tip of a pointer, held in the right hand, with the tip of one of three different targets, held in the left hand during a pre-test, several practice sessions and a post-test. The prediction of the UCM approach about the structuring of joint angle variance for selective stabilization of important task variables was tested with respect to selective stabilization of time series of the vectorial distance between the pointer and aimed target tips (bimanual control hypothesis) and with respect to selective stabilization of the endpoint trajectory of each arm (unimanual control hypothesis). The components of the total joint angle variance not affecting (VCOMP) and affecting (VUN) the value of a selected task variable were computed for each 10% of the normalized movement time. The ratio of these two components RV=VCOMP/VUN served as a quantitative index of selective stabilization. Both the bimanual and unimanual control hypotheses were supported, however the RV values for the bimanual hypothesis were significantly higher than those for the unimanual hypothesis applied to the left and right arm both prior to and after practice. This suggests that the CNS stabilizes the relative trajectory of one endpoint with respect to the other more than it stabilizes the trajectories of each of the endpoints in the external space. Practice-associated improvement in both movement speed and accuracy was accompanied by counter-intuitive lack of changes in RV. Both VCOMP and VUN variance components decreased such that their ratio remained constant prior to and after practice. We conclude that the UCM approach offers a unique and under-explored opportunity to track changes in the organization of multi-effector systems with practice and allows quantitative assessment of the degree of stabilization of selected performance variables.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anton H, Rorres C (2000) Elementary linear algebra: applications version. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Baud-Bovy G, Soechting JF (2001) Two virtual fingers in the control of the tripod grasp. J Neurophysiol 86:604–615

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baud-Bovy G, Soechting JF (2002) Factors influencing variability in load forces in a tripod grasp. Exp Brain Res 143:57–66

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein N (1935) The problem of interrelation between coordination and localization. Arch Biol Sci 38:1–35 (in Russian)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein N (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Pergamon Press, New York

  • Danion F, Schoner G, Latash ML, Li S, Scholz JP, Zatsiorsky VM (2003) A mode hypothesis for finger interaction during multi-finger force-production tasks. Biol Cybern 88:91–98

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Djupsjöbacka M, Lönn J, Olsson A (1999) A method for calibration of upper-limb kinematic data from an electromagnetic tracker system and estimation of receiver movement error. In: Herzog H, Jinha A (eds) 17th Congress of the International Society of Biomechanics, Calgary, Canada, p 218

  • Domkin D, Laczko J, Jaric S, Johansson H, Latash ML (2002) Structure of joint variability in bimanual pointing tasks. Exp Brain Res 143:11–23

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gelfand IM, Tsetlin ML (1966) On mathematical modeling of the mechanisms of the central nervous system. In: Gelfand IM, Gurfinkel VS, Fomin SV, Tsetlin ML (eds) Models of the structural-functional organization of certain biological systems. Nauka, Moscow, pp 9–26 (in Russian; a translation is available in 1971 edition by MIT Press, Cambridge MA)

  • Jaric S, Latash ML (1999) Learning a pointing task with a kinematically redundant limb: Emerging synergies and patterns of final position variability. Hum Mov Sci 18:819–838

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaric S, Milanovic S, Blesic S, Latash ML (1999) Changes in movement kinematics during single-joint movements against expectedly and unexpectedly changed inertial loads. Hum Mov Sci 18:49–66

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kang N, Shinohara M, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2004) Learning multi-finger synergies: an uncontrolled manifold analysis. Exp Brain Res 157:336–350

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kugler PN, Kelso JAS, Turvey MT (1980) On the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures I. Theoretical lines of convergence. In: Stelmach GE, Requin J (eds) Tutorials in motor behavior. North Holland, Amsterdam, pp 3–45

  • Latash ML (1993) Control of human movement. Human Kinetics, Urbana, IL

  • Latash ML (1996) How does our brain make its choices? In: Latash ML, Turvey MT (eds) Dexterity and its development. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp 277–304

  • Latash ML, Scholz JF, Danion F, Schoner G (2001) Structure of motor variability in marginally redundant multifinger force production tasks. Exp Brain Res 141:153–165

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML, Scholz JP, Schoner G (2002) Motor control strategies revealed in the structure of motor variability. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 30:26–31

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Latash ML, Yarrow K, Rothwell JC (2003) Changes in finger coordination and responses to single pulse TMS of motor cortex during practice of a multifinger force production task. Exp Brain Res 151:60–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKenzie CL, Iberall T (1994) The grasping hand. North Holland, Amsterdam

  • Newell KM (1991) Motor skill acquisition. Annu Rev Psychol 42:213–237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piek JP (1995) The contribution of spontaneous movements in the acquisition of motor coordination in infants. In: Glencross DJ, Piek JP (eds) Motor control and sensory motor integration: issues and directions. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 199–230

  • Reisman DS, Scholz JP (2003) Aspects of joint coordination are preserved during pointing in persons with post-stroke hemiparesis. Brain 126:2510–2527

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reisman DS, Scholz JP, Schoner G (2002) Coordination underlying the control of whole body momentum during sit-to-stand. Gait Posture 15:45–55

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum DA, Loukopoulos LD, Meulenbroek RGJ, Vaughan J, Engelbrecht SE (1995) Planning reaches by evaluating stored postures. Psychol Rev 102:28–67

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz JP, Danion F, Latash ML, Schoner G (2002) Understanding finger coordination through analysis of the structure of force variability. Biol Cybern 86:29–39

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz JP, Kang N, Patterson D, Latash ML (2003) Uncontrolled manifold analysis of single trials during multi-finger force production by persons with and without Down syndrome. Exp Brain Res 153:45–58

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz JP, Reisman D, Schoner G (2001) Effects of varying task constraints on solutions to joint coordination in a sit-to-stand task. Exp Brain Res 141:485–500

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz JP, Schoner G (1999) The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task. Exp Brain Res 126:289–306

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz JP, Schoner G, Latash ML (2000) Identifying the control structure of multijoint coordination during pistol shooting. Exp Brain Res 135:382–404

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seif-Naraghi AH, Winters JM (1990) Optimized strategies for scaling goal-directed dynamic limb movements. In: Winters JM, Woo SL-Y (eds) Multiple muscle systems: biomechanics and movement organization. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 312–334

  • Shim JK, Latash ML, Zatsiorsky VM (2003) Prehension synergies: trial-to-trial variability and hierarchical organization of stable performance. Exp Brain Res 152:173–184

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shinohara M, Latash ML, Zatsiorsky VM (2003) Age effects on force produced by intrinsic and extrinsic hand muscles and finger interaction during MVC tasks. J Appl Physiol 95:1361–1369

    Google Scholar 

  • Tseng YW, Scholz JP, Schoner G, Hotchkiss L (2003) Effect of accuracy constraint on joint coordination during pointing movements. Exp Brain Res 149:276–288

    Google Scholar 

  • Turvey MT (1990) Coordination. Am Psychol 45:938–953

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weeks DL, Aubert MP, Feldman AG, Levin MF (1996) One-trial adaptation of movement to changes in load. J Neurophysiol 75:60–74

    Google Scholar 

  • Vereijken B, Vanemmerik REA, Whiting HTA, Newell KM (1992) Free(z)ing degrees of freedom in skill acquisition. J Mot Behav 24:133–142

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The study was supported by grant NS-35032 from the National Institute of Health, USA, grant Nr. 208 from the University of Delaware Research Foundation (UDRF), USA and grants from Stiftelsen för internationalisering av högre utbildning och forskning (STINT), Sweden. The authors also would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments and suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dmitry Domkin.

Appendix

Appendix

Computation of variance within the uncontrolled manifold and within the orthogonal manifold

A selected task variable defines an uncontrolled manifold (UCM) in the appropriate joint space. Here we describe how the studied task variables define the corresponding UCMs at each percentage of movement time. Let M denote the mean joint configuration across trials. Let r 0 be the value of the task variable for the mean joint configuration and r k the value of the task variable for the kth trial. If the joint configuration vector (A k ) of a particular trial remains in the vicinity of M, then the deviation of the task variable Δr k =r k −r 0 relates to the deviation of joint-configuration Δk=M−A k approximately as:

$$ {\mathbf{\Delta r}}_{\mathbf{k}} = {\mathbf{J}}{\text{*}}{\mathbf{\Delta k}} $$
(1)

where J is a Jacobian matrix. Its elements are the partial derivatives of the coordinates of the task variable with respect to the joint angles in the mean joint configuration. The nullspace of J represents those changes of joint configurations that do not cause any change in the task variable. The UCM is defined by this nullspace and spanned by m independent vectors (e1,...,em) at each instant of time. If DV is the dimension of the task variable and DF is the number of degrees of freedom of the multi-joint system, then m=DF−DV (Scholz et al. 2000).The component of Δk, which lies in the UCM, is obtained by its projection on to the nullspace. Let us denote this projection as ΔkUCM:

$$ {\mathbf{\Delta k}}^{{\text{UCM}}} = \sum\limits_{i = 1}^m {\left\langle {{\mathbf{\Delta k}},\;{\mathbf{ei}}} \right\rangle } * {\mathbf{ei}} $$
(2)

where < > denotes scalar product.

The component that is orthogonal to the null space is defined as:

$$ {\mathbf{\Delta k}}^{{\text{ORT}}} = {\mathbf{\Delta k}} - {\mathbf{\Delta k}}^{{\text{UCM}}} $$
(3)

We computed the variances per DF of both components of the joint configuration vectors across N trials. The variance of the component, which lies within the UCM, is defined as compensated variance VCOMP:

$$ {\text{V}}_{{\text{COMP}}} = \sum\limits_{k = 1}^N {\left( {{\mathbf{\Delta k}}^{{\text{UCM}}} } \right)} ^{\text{2}} {\text{/}}\left( {\left( {{\text{DF}} - {\text{DV}}} \right) * {\text{N}}} \right) $$
(4)

The variance per DF of the orthogonal component is defined as uncompensated variance VUN:

$$ {\text{V}}_{{\text{UN}}} = \sum\limits_{k = 1}^N {\left( {{\mathbf{\Delta k}}^{{\text{ORT}}} } \right)} ^{\text{2}} {\text{/}}\left( {{\text{DV}} * {\text{N}}} \right) $$
(5)

We computed the Jacobians for the described bimanual task variable and for the unimanual task variables for each arm separately. Numerical derivation was applied for this computation.

Bimanual control hypothesis

The hypothesized task variable for the bimanual control hypothesis is the vectorial distance between the pointer tip (Px, Py, Pz) and aimed target tip (Tx, Ty, Tz). The vectorial distance is denoted by R=(Rx, Ry, Rz)=(Tx, Ty, Tz)−(Px, Py, Pz).

Forward kinematics was used to compute R from given joint angles, thus the coordinates of R were given as functions of 14 joint angles. The angles 1 to 7 relate to the left arm, the angles 8 to 14 relate to the right arm. The angles for the shoulder, elbow and wrist joints are described in the Methods. The joint angles (α1(t), α2(t), ..., α14(t)) and the associated vectorial distance as a function of the angles R(α1(t), ..., αj(t), ..., α14(t)) were given at each percentage of movement time (t=1, ..., 100).

Let Jxj(t), Jyj(t) and Jzj(t) be the partial derivatives of Rx, Ry and Rz with respect to αj for t=1, ..., 100.

For numerical derivation of the partial derivatives of R with respect to the joint angle αj we need to compute the value of R at the virtual joint configuration (α1(t), ..., αj(t-1), ..., α14(t)).

Note that (α1(t), ..., αj(t-1), ..., α14(t)) is a virtual joint configuration in which the jth joint angle is taken from the time bin that precedes the time bin when the other angles are considered.

Then the elements of the Jacobian are computed as the following:

$$\begin{array}{*{20}c} {{{\text{Jxj}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)}={\left( {{\text{Rx(a1}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)},...,{\text{aj}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)},...,{\text{a14}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)}} \right)} - {\text{Rx(a1}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)},...,{\text{aj}}{\left( {{\text{t - 1}}} \right)},...,{\text{a14}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)}{\text{)) / (aj}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)} - {\text{aj}}{\left( {{\text{t - 1}}} \right)}{\text{)}}{\text{,}}}} \\ {{{\text{Jyj}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)}={\left( {{\text{Ry(a1}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)},...,{\text{aj}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)},...,{\text{a14}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)}} \right)} - {\text{Ry(a1}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)},...,{\text{aj}}{\left( {{\text{t - 1}}} \right)},...,{\text{a14}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)}{\text{)) / (aj}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)} - {\text{aj}}{\left( {{\text{t - 1}}} \right)}{\text{)}}{\text{,}}}} \\ {{{\text{Jzj}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)}={\left( {{\text{Rz(a1}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)},...,{\text{aj}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)},...,{\text{a14}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)}} \right)} - {\text{Rz(a1}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)},...,{\text{aj}}{\left( {{\text{t - 1}}} \right)},...,{\text{a14}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)}{\text{)) / (aj}}{\left( {\text{t}} \right)} - {\text{aj}}{\left( {{\text{t - 1}}} \right)}{\text{)}}}} \\ \end{array} $$

The Jacobian is a matrix with 3 rows and 14 columns at each instant of time:

$$ {\mathbf{J}}\left( {\text{t}} \right) = \begin{array}{*{20}c} {{\text{Jx1}}\left( {\text{t}} \right){\text{,}}} & {{\text{Jx2}}\left( {\text{t}} \right),} & {...{\text{,}}} & {{\text{Jx14}}\left( {\text{t}} \right)} \\ {{\text{Jy1}}\left( {\text{t}} \right){\text{,}}} & {{\text{Jy2}}\left( {\text{t}} \right),} & {...{\text{,}}} & {{\text{Jy14}}\left( {\text{t}} \right)} \\ {{\text{Jz1}}\left( {\text{t}} \right){\text{,}}} & {{\text{Jz2}}\left( {\text{t}} \right),} & {...{\text{,}}} & {{\text{Jz14}}\left( {\text{t}} \right)} \\ \end{array} $$

The nullspace of J is 11-dimensional. The mean and the related deviation vectors were computed for the lower, middle and upper target separately. The variance within the UCM for the bimanual control hypothesis was also computed for each target separately using a particular form of Eq. (4) with DF=14 and DV=3:

$$ {\text{V}}_{{\text{COMP}}} = \sum\limits_{k = 1}^N {\left( {{\mathbf{\Delta k}}^{{\text{UCM}}} } \right)} ^{\text{2}} {\text{/}}\left( {11 * {\text{N}}} \right) $$
(6)

The variance within the orthogonal manifold was computed from Eq. (5):

$$ {\text{V}}_{{\text{UN}}} = \sum\limits_{k = 1}^N {\left( {{\mathbf{\Delta k}}^{{\text{ORT}}} } \right)} ^{\text{2}} {\text{/}}\left( {{\text{3}} * {\text{N}}} \right) $$
(7)

where N is the number of pointing movements (i.e. trials) to the aimed target.

Unimanual control hypothesis applied to the left and right arm

If two arms are considered separately, then the joint space for each arm is seven-dimensional. For the unimanual control hypothesis applied to the left arm the task variable is the position of the aimed target tip (Tx, Ty, Tz). For the unimanual control hypothesis applied to the right arm the task variable is the position of the pointer tip (Px, Py, Pz). Therefore, the corresponding UCMs for the left and right arm are approximated by the nullspaces of 3×7 matrices at each instant.

The elements of the Jacobian for the left and right arm are computed similarly to the computation of the Jacobian elements for the bimanual hypothesis, only using the task variables T and P instead of the vectorial distance R and seven angles of the corresponding arm instead of all 14 angles.

The nullspaces of the Jacobians for the left and right arm and thus, their UCMs are four-dimensional, while their ORTs are three-dimensional. The deviation vectors Δk left and Δk right are differences of the joint configuration of the left (α1k, α2k, ..., α7k) or right arm (α8k, α9k, ..., α14k) in the kth trial and the mean joint configuration of the left or right arm, respectively. The mean and the related deviation vectors were computed for the lower, middle and upper target separately. The variance per DF within the UCMs of the left and right arm can be obtained from Eq. (4) with DF=7 and DV=3, using Δk left or Δk right , respectively. The variance within the ORTs for the left and right arm can be computed from Eq. (5) with DV=3, using Δk left or Δk right , respectively.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Domkin, D., Laczko, J., Djupsjöbacka, M. et al. Joint angle variability in 3D bimanual pointing: uncontrolled manifold analysis. Exp Brain Res 163, 44–57 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-2137-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-2137-1

Keywords

Navigation