Skip to main content
Log in

Influence of biomechanical factors on substructure of pointing movements

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Irregularities in the velocity profile near the end of pointing movements have been interpreted as corrective submovements whose purpose is to provide accuracy of pointing to the target. The purpose of the present study was to investigate whether two additional factors related to biomechanical properties of the arm also cause submovements. First, motion termination and stabilization of the limb in the final position required by a discrete pointing task may contribute to submovements. Second, inaccurate regulation of interactive torque at the joints may also cause submovements. To investigate the contributions of these two biomechanical factors and the traditionally considered factor of pointing accuracy, the incidence of submovements was analyzed during three types of experimental manipulations. In addition to target size manipulations (small and large targets), conditions for motion termination were manipulated by examining discrete movements (which terminated at the target) and reciprocal movements (which reversed direction without dwelling on the target). Interaction torques were varied by using targets that require different shoulder–elbow coordination patterns. Submovements were detected in 41% of all analyzed movements. Data supported influences from the accuracy and motion termination factors but not from the interactive torque regulation factor on submovement incidence. Gross submovements were associated with motion termination; fine submovements primarily with accuracy demands. These findings and the analysis of temporal movement characteristics suggest that motion termination is an extra movement component that makes control of discrete movements different to control of reciprocal movements. Implications of the findings to a noise-related interpretation of Fitts’ law are discussed. The study emphasizes the influence of arm biomechanics on endpoint kinematics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adam JJ, van der Bruggen DPW, Bekkering H (1993) The control of discrete and reciprocal target-aiming responces: evidence for the exploitation of mechanics. Hum Mov Sci 12:353–364

    Google Scholar 

  • Adam JJ, Nieuwenstein JH, Huys R, Paas FG, Kingma H, Willems P, Werry M (2000) Control of rapid aimed hand movements: the one-target advantage. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26:295–312

    Google Scholar 

  • Adamovich SV, Levin MF, Feldman AG (1994) Merging different motor patterns: coordination between rhythmical and discrete single-joint movements. Exp Brain Res 99:325–337

    Google Scholar 

  • Amrhein PC (1996) Age-related slowing in movement parameterization studies: not what you might think. In: Fernandez AM, Teasdale N (eds) Changes in sensory motor behavior in aging, vol 114. Elsevier, Amsterdam

  • Berardelli A, Hallett M, Rothwell JC, Agostino R, Manfredi M, Thompson PD, Marsden CD (1996) Single-joint rapid arm movements in normal subjects and in patients with motor disorders. Brain 119:661–674

    Google Scholar 

  • Birren JE (1974) Translations in gerontology—from lab to life. Psychophysiology and speed of response. Am Psychol 29:808–815

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan JJ, Park JH, Ryu YU, Shea CH (2003) Discrete and cyclical units of action in a mixed target pair aiming task. Exp Brain Res 150:473–489

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan JJ, Park JH, Shea CH (2004) Systematic scaling of target width: dynamics, planning, and feedback. Neurosci Lett 367:317–322

    Google Scholar 

  • Chua R, Elliott D (1993) Visial regulation of manual aiming. Hum Mov Sci 12:365–401

    Google Scholar 

  • Crossman ERFW, Goodeve PJ (1983) Feedback control of hand-movement and Fitts’ law. Q J Exp Psychol 35A:251–278

    Google Scholar 

  • Darling WG, Cooke JD, Brown SH (1989) Control of simple arm movements in elderly humans. Neurobiol Aging 10:149–157

    Google Scholar 

  • Dounskaia NV, Ketcham CJ, Stelmach GE (2002a) Commonalities and differences in control of a large set of drawing movements. Exp Brain Res 146:11–25

    Google Scholar 

  • Dounskaia N, Ketcham CJ, Stelmach GE (2002b) Influence of biomechanical constraints on horizontal arm movements. Motor Control 6:366–87

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott D, Helsen WF, Chua R (2001) A century later: two-component model of goal-directed aiming. Psychol Bull 127:342–357

    Google Scholar 

  • Enoka RM, Christou EA, Hunter SK, Kornatz KW, Semmler JG, Taylor AM, Tracy BL (2003) Mechanisms that contribute to differences in motor performance between young and old adults. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 13:1–12

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol 47:381–391

    Google Scholar 

  • Galganski ME, Fuglevand AJ, Enoka RM (1993) Reduced control of motor output in a human hand muscle of elderly subjects during submaximal contractions. J Neurophysiol 69:2108–2115

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghez C, Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Sainburg RL (1994) Contributions of vision and proprioception to accuracy in limb movements. In: Gazzaniga MS (ed) The cognitive neurosciences. MIT, Cambridge, MA, pp 549–564

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Cooper SE, Ghez C (1994) Accuracy of planar reaching movements. II. Systematic extent errors resulting from inertial anisotropy. Exp Brain Res 99:112–130

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb GL (1998) Muscle activation patterns during two types of voluntary single-joint movements. J Neurophysiol 80:1860–1867

    Google Scholar 

  • Gribble PL, Ostry DJ (1999) Compensation for interaction torques during single- and multijoint limb movement. J Neurophysiol 82:2310–2326

    Google Scholar 

  • Guiard Y (1993) On Fitts’s and Hooke’s laws: simple harmonic movement in upper limb cyclical aiming. Acta Psychol 82:139–159

    Google Scholar 

  • Guiard Y (1997) Fitts’ law in the discrete vs. cyclical paradigm. Hum Mov Sci 16:97–131

    Google Scholar 

  • Hallet M, Shanani BT, Young RR (1975) EMG analysis of stereotyped voluntary movements in man. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 38:1154–1162

    Google Scholar 

  • Hallett M, Khoshbin S (1980) A physiological mechanism of bradykinesia. Brain 103:301–304

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannaford B, Stark L (1985) Roles of the elements of the triphasic control signal. Exp Neurol 90:619–635

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollerbach JM, Flash T (1982) Dynamic interactions between limb segments during planar arm movement. Biol Cybern 44:67–77

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso JAS (1992) Theoretical concepts and strategies for understanding perceptual-motor skill: from information capacity in closed systems to self-organization in open, nonequilibrium systems. J Exp Psychol Gen 121:260–262

    Google Scholar 

  • Ketcham CJ, Seidler RD, Van Gemmert AWA, Stelmach GE (2002) Age-related kinematic differences as influenced by task difficulty, target-size, and movement amplitude. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 57:54–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Khan MA, Franks IM (2003) Online versus offline processing of visual feedback in the production of component submovements. J Mot Behav 35:285–295

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer DE, Abrams RA, Kornblum S, Wright CE, Smith JEK (1988) Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol Rev 95:340–370

    Google Scholar 

  • Novak KE, Miller LE, Houk JC (2000) Kinematic properties of rapid hand movements in a knob turning task. Exp Brain Res 132:419–433

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratt J, Chasteen AL, Abrams RA (1994) Rapid aimed limb movements: age differences and practice effects in component submovements. Psychol Aging 9:325–334

    Google Scholar 

  • Rand MK, Stelmach GE, Bloedel JR (2000) Movement accuracy constraints in Parkinson’s disease patients. Neuropsychologia 38:203–212

    Google Scholar 

  • Sainburg RL, Poizner H, Ghez C (1993) Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination. J Neurophysiol 70:2136–2147

    Google Scholar 

  • Sainburg RL, Ghilardi MF, Poizner H, Ghez C (1995) Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. J Neurophysiol 73:820–835

    Google Scholar 

  • Sainburg RL, Ghez C, Kalakanis D (1999) Intersegmental dynamics are controlled by sequential anticipatory, error correction, and postural mechanisms. J Neurophysiol 81:1045–1056

    Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse TA (1985) A theory of cognitive aging. In: Stelmach GE, Vroon PA (eds) Advances in psychology, vol 28. Elsevier, Amsterdam

  • Schmidt RA, Sherwood DE, Waler CB (1988) Rapid movements with reversals in direction I. The control of movement time. Exp Brain Res 69:344–354

    Google Scholar 

  • Schöner G (1990) A dynamic theory of coordination of discrete movement. Biol Cybern 63:257–270

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidler RD, Alberts JL, Stelmach GE (2001) Multijoint movement control in Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res 140:335–344

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidler-Dobrin RD, Stelmach GE (1998) Persistence in visual feedback control by the elderly. Exp Brain Res 119:467–474

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidler-Dobrin RD, He J, Stelmach GE (1998) Coactivation to reduce variability in elderly. Motor Control 2:314–330

    Google Scholar 

  • Smits-Engelsman BCM, Van Galen GP, Duysens J (2002) The breakdown of Fitts’ law in rapid, reciprocal aiming movements. Exp Brain Res 145:222–230

    Google Scholar 

  • Smyrnis N, Evdokimidis I, Constantinidis TS (2000) Speed-accuracy trade-off in the performance of pointing movements in different directions in two-dimensional space. Exp Brain Res 134:21–31

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternad D, Dean WJ, Schaal S (2000) Interaction of rhythmic and discrete pattern generators in single-joint movements. Hum Mov Sci 19:627–664

    Google Scholar 

  • Teeken JC, Adam JJ, Paas FG, van Boxtel MP, Houx PJ, Jolles J (1996) Effects of age and gender on discrete and reciprocal aiming movements. Psychol Aging 11:195–198

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Galen GP, De Jong WP (1995) Fitts’ law as the outcome of a dynamic noise filtering mode of motor control. Hum Mov Sci 14:539–572

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Galen GP, Schomaker LRB (1992) Fitts’ law as a low-pass filter effect of muscle stiffness. Hum Mov Sci 11:11–22

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker N, Philbin DA, Fisk AD (1997) Age-related differences in movement control: adjusting submovement structure to optimize performance. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 52B:40–52

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth RS (1899) The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychol Rev Suppl 3:1–119

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelaznik HN, Hawkins B, Kisselburgh L (1983) Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. J Mot Behav 17:217–236

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The study was supported by NSF grant BCS 0213653 awarded to Dr. Natalia Dounskaia.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Natalia Dounskaia.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dounskaia, N., Wisleder, D. & Johnson, T. Influence of biomechanical factors on substructure of pointing movements. Exp Brain Res 164, 505–516 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-2271-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-2271-4

Keywords

Navigation