Skip to main content
Log in

Are Extended Twin Family Designs Worth the Trouble? A Comparison of the Bias, Precision, and Accuracy of Parameters Estimated in Four Twin Family Models

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Behavior Genetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The classical twin design (CTD) uses observed covariances from monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs to infer the relative magnitudes of genetic and environmental causes of phenotypic variation. Despite its wide use, it is well known that the CTD can produce biased estimates if its stringent assumptions are not met. By modeling observed covariances of twins’ relatives in addition to twins themselves, extended twin family designs (ETFDs) require less stringent assumptions, can estimate many more parameters of interest, and should produce less biased estimates than the CTD. However, ETFDs are more complicated to use and interpret, and by attempting to estimate a large number of parameters, the precision of parameter estimates may suffer. This paper is a formal investigation into a simple question: Is it worthwhile to use more complex models such as ETFDs in behavioral genetics? In particular, we compare the bias, precision, and accuracy of estimates from the CTD and three increasingly complex ETFDs. We find the CTD does a decent job of estimating broad sense heritability, but CTD estimates of shared environmental effects and the relative importance of additive versus non-additive genetic variance can be biased, sometimes wildly so. Increasingly complex ETFDs, on the other hand, are more accurate and less sensitive to assumptions than simpler models. We conclude that researchers interested in characterizing the environment or the makeup of genetic variation should use ETFDs when possible.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12
Fig. 13
Fig. 14

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We follow the convention that \( \hat{V}_{ \cdot } \) is the estimate of the population parameter \( V_{ \cdot } \)

  2. Strictly speaking, \( C\hat{V}(A,F) \) is a nonlinear constraint and is not freely estimated in ETFDs. It is determined by, and helps to determine, estimated parameters by constraining their inter-relationships in a way that keeps the entire model internally consistent.

References

  • Carey G (2005) Cholesky problems. Behav Genet 35:653–665

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casela G, Berger RL (1990) Statistical inference. Wadsworth, Belmont

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger CR, Rice J, Reich T (1979) Multifactorial inheritance with cultural transmission and assortative mating II: a general model of combined polygenic and cultural inheritance. Am J Hum Genet 31:176–198

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coventry WL, Keller MC (2005) Estimating the extent of parameter bias in the classical twin design: a comparison of parameter estimates from extended twin-family and classical twin designs. Twin Res Hum Genet 8:214–223

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crnokrak P, Roff DA (1995) Dominance variation: associations with selection and fitness. Heredity 75:530–540

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eaves LJ (1979) The use of twins in the analysis of assortative mating. Heredity 43:399–409

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eaves LJ (2009) Putting the ‘human’ back in genetics: modeling the extended kinship of twins. Twin Res Hum Genet 12:1–7

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eaves LJ, Last KA, Young PA, Martin NG (1978) Model-fitting approaches to the analysis of human behavior. Heredity 41:249–320

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eaves LJ, Eysenck HJ, Martin JM (eds) (1989) Genes, culture, and personality: an empirical approach. Academic Press, Londong

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher RA (1918) The correlation between relatives on the supposition of Mendelian inheritance. Trans Roy Soc Edinb 52:399–433

    Google Scholar 

  • Fulker DW (1982) Extension of the classical twin method. In Bonné-Tamir B, Cohen T, Goodman RM (eds) Human genetics, part A: the unfolding genome (Progress in clinical and biological research 103A). Alan R Liss, New York, pp. 395–406

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson DA (1989) Twins reared together: minimizing shared environmental effects. Behav Genet 19:593–604

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haldane JBS (1932) The causes of evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton, N.J.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heath AC, Kendler KS, Eaves LJ, Markell D (1985) The resolution of cultural and biological inheritance: informativeness of different relationships. Behav Genet 15:439–465

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hill WG, Goddard ME, Visscher PM (2008) Data and theory point to mainly additive genetic variance for complex traits. PLos Genet 4:1–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, M. C. (2007). PedEvolve: a simulator of genetically informative data implemented in R. Annual meeting of the behavior genetics association, Amsterdam

  • Keller MC, Coventry WL (2005) Quantifying and addressing parameter indeterminacy in the classical twin design. Twin Res Hum Genet 8:201–213

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keller MC, Medland SE, Duncan LE, Hatemi PK, Neale MC, Maes HMM et al (2009) Modeling extended twin family data I: description of the cascade model. Twin Res Hum Genet 12:8–18

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maes HMM, Neale MC, Medland SE, Keller MC, Martin NG, Heath AC et al (2009) Flexible Mx specifications of various extended twin kinship designs. Twin Res Hum Genet 12:26–34

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin NG, Boomsma DI, Machin G (1997) A twin-pronged attach on complex traits. Nat Genet 17:387–392

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Medland SE, Keller MC (2009) Modeling extended twin family data II: power associated with different family structures. Twin Res Hum Genet 12:19–25

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller G, Todd PM (1998) Mate choice turns cognitive. Trends Cogn Sci 2:190–198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nance WE, Corey LA (1976) Genetic models for the analysis of data from the families of identical twins. Genetics 83:811–826

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neale MC (1999) MX: statistical modelling, 5th edn. Department of Psychiatry, Richmond, VA

    Google Scholar 

  • Neale MC, Fulker DW (1984) A bivariate path analysis of fear data on twins and their parents. Acta Genetica Medica Gemellol (Roma) 33:273–286

    Google Scholar 

  • Operario D, Tschann J, Flores E, Bridges M (2006) Brief report: associations of parental warmth, peer support, and gender with adolescent emotional distress. J Adolesc 29(2):299–305

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, McGuffin P (2001) Behavioral genetics, 4th edn. Worth Publishers, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Posthuma D, Boomsma DI (2000) A note on the statistical power in extended twin designs. Behav Genet 30:147–158

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Development Team (2009) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds CA, Baker LA, Pedersen NL (2000) Multivariate models of mixed assortment: phenotypic assortment and social homogamy for education and fluid ability. Behav Genet 30(6):455–476

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thiessen DD, Gregg B (1980) Human assortative mating and genetic equilibrium: an evolutionary perspective. Ethol Sociobiol 1:111–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Truett KR, Eaves LJ, Walters EE, Heath AC, Hewitt JK, Meyer JM et al (1994) A model system for analysis of family resemblance in extended kinships of twins. Behav Genet 24:35–49

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wahlberg P (2009) Chicken genomics-linkage and QTL mapping. Digital comprehensive summaries of Uppsala. Dissertations from the Faculty of Medicine

  • Wright S (1929) Fisher’s theory of dominance. Am Nat 63:274–279

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthew C. Keller.

Additional information

Edited by Gitta Lubke.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Keller, M.C., Medland, S.E. & Duncan, L.E. Are Extended Twin Family Designs Worth the Trouble? A Comparison of the Bias, Precision, and Accuracy of Parameters Estimated in Four Twin Family Models. Behav Genet 40, 377–393 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-009-9320-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-009-9320-x

Keywords

Navigation