Elsevier

Biological Psychiatry

Volume 44, Issue 8, 15 October 1998, Pages 726-732
Biological Psychiatry

Original Article
Risperidone vs. haloperidol on reaction time, manual dexterity, and motor learning in treatment-resistant schizophrenia patients

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00088-2Get rights and content

Abstract

Background: The present study compared the effects of risperidone vs. haloperidol on reaction time, manual dexterity, and two types of motor learning in a sample of treatment-resistant schizophrenia patients.

Methods: Fifty-six DSM-III-R diagnosed schizophrenia inpatients participated in a randomized, double-blind comparison of risperidone vs. haloperidol. Measures of reaction time, manual dexterity, motor sequence learning, and gross motor learning were administered at baseline, after 4 weeks of fixed-dose medication, and after 4 weeks of flexible-dose medication.

Results: The results indicated that patients receiving risperidone showed greater improvement in reaction time and manual dexterity than patients receiving haloperidol. After covarying symptom changes and movement disorder ratings, the results remained significant. The two treatment groups did not differ on either measure of motor learning.

Conclusions: The differences in performance in reaction time and manual dexterity may be due to a specific beneficial effect of risperidone, as opposed to a general reduction in extrapyramidal symptom liability, compared to haloperidol.

Introduction

Schizophrenia patients are generally slower than normal adults on tasks of fine motor speed, a consistent finding from the mid 1930s Huston et al 1937, Shakow and Huston 1936. Impairment is noted on measures that involve quick initiation of response (e.g., reaction time measures), maintenance of maximal speed over brief periods of time (e.g., finger tapping), and rapid fingertip manipulations (e.g., Pin Test, Purdue Pegboard) (King 1991). On the other hand, motor learning appears to be relatively preserved, a finding supported by most, but not all, of the recent studies Clare et al 1993, Goldberg et al 1993b, Granholm et al 1993, Green et al 1997a, Kern et al 1997.

Among measures of psychomotor speed and dexterity, reaction time has been examined in relation to clinical and functional outcome Cancro et al 1971, Wykes and Dunn 1992, Wykes et al 1990, Zahn and Carpenter 1978. Zahn and Carpenter (1978) found that acute schizophrenic patients who had faster reaction times at admission tended to show greater clinical improvement over the course of a brief hospital stay (3–4 months) than patients with slower reaction times. Similarly, Cancro et al (1971), using a longer period of follow-up, found reaction time to be predictive of clinical outcome 3 years later. It was speculated that the overall level of reaction time reflects an individual’s “environmental responsivity,” which may be a critical factor in the recovery process that potentiates the effects of treatment.

Two lines of evidence suggest that the types of psychomotor deficits noted in schizophrenia are largely independent of the effects of conventional neuroleptics. First, the observation of psychomotor slowing was documented well before the introduction of antipsychotic medication into clinical treatment in the 1950s. Second, a number of studies have examined the effects of chlorpromazine and similar acting antipsychotic agents on reaction time and other measures of psychomotor functioning. The preponderance of evidence indicates that conventional antipsychotic medications neither facilitate nor impede reaction time at either acute or chronic dose administrations (for a review see Cassens et al 1990, King 1990, Spohn and Strauss 1989). Patient performances on other types of psychomotor tasks show more varied effects (Gold et al 1991; for a review see Medalia et al 1988). Performance on measures that involve the maintenance of fine motor movement at maximal speed (e.g., finger tapping test) or ones that involve more gross musculature in the execution of perceptuomotor tracking (e.g., pursuit rotor) are unaffected by acute administration of low doses of chlorpromazine (<150 mg), but are impaired by acute administration of higher doses (200–300 mg) (Kornetsky et al 1959); however, chronic administration of chlorpromazine appears to yield no adverse effect on performance on tasks of fine motor speed, fine manipulative movements, or perceptuomotor tracking Pearl 1962, Pugh 1968, Small et al 1972, Whitehead and Thune 1958.

Relatively little is known about the pharmacologic effects of the new generation of antipsychotic medications on psychomotor movement. Although the newer antipsychotic medications are associated with reduced risk for the development of extrapyramidal symptoms, this finding may not necessarily translate directly to improvements in speed and coordination. Studies thus far have been limited to clozapine, and the results show no clear-cut effect. A nonblind comparison between haloperidol and clozapine failed to reveal any advantage for clozapine on measures of reaction time, finger tapping, or a task requiring fine motor precision (e.g., pegboard; Classen and Laux 1988). Also, Goldberg et al (1993a) reported no beneficial effect of clozapine on a number of neurocognitive functions that included two measures of psychomotor speed. More recently though, Hoff et al (1996) tested patients on conventional neuroleptics (unspecified) and then after 12 weeks on clozapine on tasks of motor speed and other neurocognitive functions using a within-subject design. Clozapine was associated with improvement in motor speed (finger tapping); other neurocognitive functions were differentially affected (e.g., verbal fluency improved, visual memory and executive functioning decreased). In one other study, a single-blind investigation that compared clozapine with placebo medication and fluphenazine, clozapine failed to improve performance on a complex reaction time measure of attention despite yielding clinical improvement (Zahn et al 1994). It is not known whether other atypical antipsychotic drugs will yield any beneficial effects for psychomotor functioning.

Risperidone is a relatively new atypical antipsychotic agent. Although risperidone shares some pharmacologic characteristics with clozapine (i.e., they both act on dopaminergic and serotonergic neurotransmitter systems), risperidone’s receptor binding profile is considerably different than that of clozapine’s. Hence, the largely negative findings for clozapine on motor speed and dexterity may or may not be relevant to patients treated with risperidone. The present study compared the effects of haloperidol vs. risperidone on measures of reaction time, manual dexterity, and two types of motor learning in a group of treatment-resistant schizophrenia patients using a double-blind design.

Section snippets

Human subjects

Patients for the study were recruited from the Camarillo–UCLA Clinical Research Unit and the West Los Angeles Veteran’s Affairs Medical Center. The sample included 56 patients who met criteria for schizophrenia based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (Spitzer et al 1990). All interviewers were trained by the Diagnosis and Psychopathology Unit of the UCLA Clinical Research Center (CRC) for the Study of Schizophrenia (R.P. Liberman, PI), and attained a minimum kappa of .75 for

Results

There were no differences between the two groups on any demographic or chronicity measures, and the two groups were also comparable in terms of their level of symptom severity and EPS ratings at baseline.

The primary analyses for between-group effects on the outcome measures revealed a significant effect of treatment on reaction time (F = 12.94, df = 1,41, p = .0009; ANCOVA) and manual dexterity (F = 5.93, df = 1,53, p = .018; ANCOVA). There were no significant effects of phase or treatment ×

Discussion

Patients participating in a double-blind study who were randomly assigned to risperidone treatment showed greater improvement in reaction time and manual dexterity than patients assigned to haloperidol. The differences in performance showed little change over the two study phases (fixed and flexible dose). Although significant group differences were found on these two measures, which emphasize speed of fine motor movement, no differences were found between risperidone- and haloperidol-treated

Acknowledgements

The project received support from the NIMH UCLA Clinical Research Center for the Study of Schizophrenia, an investigator-initiated grant from the Janssen Research Foundation, and a Merit Award from the Department of Veterans Affairs.

We thank Mary Jane Robertson, MS, Jeffery L. Hayden, Kimmy S. Kee, PhD, Mark McGee, Dustin Salveson, Robert Mangano, Renee Galbavy, Katherine Narr, Maryam Etemadjam, Kristin Mangis, Crystal Ray, Vanessa Walker-Oakes, and Henry Koehn for their collective efforts in

References (37)

  • J.M Gold et al.

    The impact of symptomatic state and pharmacological treatment on cognitive functioning of patients with schizophrenia and mood disorders

  • T.E Goldberg et al.

    The effect of clozapine on cognition and psychiatric symptoms in patients with schizophrenia

    Br J Psychiatry

    (1993)
  • T.E Goldberg et al.

    Learning and memory in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia

    Psychol Med

    (1993)
  • M.F Green et al.

    Procedural learning in schizophreniaEvidence from serial reaction time

    Cogn Neuropsychiatry

    (1997)
  • M.F Green et al.

    Does risperidone improve verbal working memory in treatment-resistant schizophrenia?

    Am J Psychiatry

    (1997)
  • W.C Heindel et al.

    Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systemsA comparison of Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s disease patients

    J Neurosci

    (1989)
  • P Huston et al.

    Studies of motor function in schizophrenia. II. Reaction time

    J Gen Psychol

    (1937)
  • J Kane et al.

    Clozapine for the treatment-resistant schizophrenicA double-blind comparison with chlorpromazine

    Arch Gen Psychiatry

    (1988)
  • Cited by (81)

    • Novel Dopamine Therapeutics for Cognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia

      2017, Biological Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      Conversely, effects on negative symptoms are limited (109–112). Effects on neurocognitive deficits (e.g., working memory, verbal memory, attention, executive functioning) have also been reported (1,2,113–118) but have been small (99). The relative lack of effects of antipsychotic medications on cognition are particularly important given that approximately 75% of patients with schizophrenia have clinically meaningful deficits in at least two cognitive domains and 90% have deficits in at least one domain (119).

    • Mouse pharmacological models of cognitive disruption relevant to schizophrenia

      2012, Neuropharmacology
      Citation Excerpt :

      FGA-induced cognitive disruption may have contributed to studies suggesting the superiority of second generation antipsychotics (SGA), which have diverse actions on dopamine D2, 5-HT2A, and other receptors (Richelson and Souder, 2000). While some studies report no effects of FGA or SGA on cognition (Kunitachi et al., 2009; Nagai et al., 2009; Thomsen et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2007), other clinical trials (Harvey et al., 2003, 2004; Kern et al., 1998; Kern et al., 1999; Purdon et al., 2001; Velligan et al., 2002, 2003) and meta-analyses (Harvey and Keefe, 2001; Keefe et al., 1999) reported superiority of SGA over FGA for treating cognitive symptoms in schizophrenia. Lack of cognitive improvement from FGAs could be related to co-administration of anti-cholinergic drugs (Keefe et al., 1999; Paulsen et al., 1995; Spohn and Strauss, 1989), used to counter the extra-pyramidal side-effects of FGA, because cholinergic antagonists can impair cognitive performance (see below).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text