Elsevier

Animal Behaviour

Volume 81, Issue 5, May 2011, Pages 949-954
Animal Behaviour

Honeybee foragers increase the use of waggle dance information when private information becomes unrewarding

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.014Get rights and content

Social insect foragers often have access to both social and private information about the locations of food sources. In honeybees, Apis mellifera, foragers can follow waggle dances (social information) to obtain vector information about the location of profitable food sources or they can use route memories (private information) acquired during previous foraging trips. The relative use of social information versus private information is poorly understood and currently debated. It is hypothesized that social information should be prioritized when the use of private information has a low benefit. We tested this hypothesis by training foragers to a high-quality 2 M sucrose feeder, which subsequently became unrewarding. As foragers continued to experience zero reward from their private route information they increased the time spent following waggle dances advertising an alternative food source with the same odour. A significant proportion of foragers successfully switched to the food source indicated by dances. Overall, trained foragers showed a strong attachment to the known but currently unrewarding feeder, even after repeatedly following dances advertising a profitable alternative. Successful recruits to the novel food source advertised by the waggle dances had more social information about this source in that they had followed dances for longer. Our results suggest that honeybee foragers follow a strategy that is conservative in terms of switching from one food patch to another.

Section snippets

Methods

Experiments were performed in September and October 2009. Ivy, Hedera helix, was the only important natural pollen and nectar source available at that time. We studied three colonies of Apis mellifera at the Laboratory of Apiculture and Social Insects, University of Sussex. Each colony was housed in an observation hive containing three deep Langstroth frames or the equivalent comb area in medium frames (H1–H3). Each colony was thriving with a queen, about 3000 workers, brood, pollen and honey

Dance Following of F1 Foragers

Across the three trials, F2 foragers performed 472 waggle dances comprising 5272 waggle runs (mean ± SD = 11.17 ± 8.05 per dance,) for the F2 feeder during test days 1 (245 dances) and 2 (227 dances). F1 foragers showed a strong interest in these dances and most (104 of 118, 88.1%) followed dances (4203 waggle runs in total). On average, F1 foragers followed 11.59 ± 5.60 (range 1–35) dances and 40.03 ± 28.92 (range 1–127) waggle runs for the F2 feeder, with six (5.8%) following more than 100 waggle runs.

Discussion

Foragers that repeatedly found that retrieving private information about feeding sites was unrewarded subsequently increased the number of waggle runs followed per dance for an alternative location of the same food type. As a consequence, foragers started switching from visiting the familiar but now unrewarding feeder, F1, to the unfamiliar but rewarding feeder, F2, advertised by F2 dancers (Fig. 1). F1 foragers did not increase their interest in dances advertising natural food sources. They

Acknowledgments

We thank Francisca Segers, Fiona Riddell, Tomer Czaczkes and Johanneke Oosten for help with data collection and Margaret Couvillon, Walter Farina and two anonymous referees for comments on the manuscript. C.G. was supported by a postdoctoral fellowship from the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF grant PBBEP3-123648).

References (51)

  • Y. van Bergen et al.

    Nine-spined sticklebacks exploit the most reliable source when public and private information conflict

    Proceedings of the Royal Society B

    (2004)
  • J.C. Biesmeijer et al.

    The use of waggle dance information by honeybees throughout their foraging careers

    Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

    (2005)
  • J.C. Biesmeijer et al.

    Information flow and organization of stingless bee foraging

    Apidologie

    (2004)
  • J.C. Biesmeijer et al.

    The role of internal and external information in foraging decisions of Melipona workers (Hymenoptera: Meliponinae)

    Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

    (1998)
  • J. Bozic et al.

    Attendants and followers of honey bee waggle dances

    Journal of Apicultural Research

    (1991)
  • L. Chittka et al.

    The significance of landmarks for path integration in homing honeybee foragers

    Naturwissenschaften

    (1995)
  • A. Dornhaus et al.

    Why do honey bees dance?

    Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

    (2004)
  • H. Esch et al.

    How do newly recruited honey bees approach a food site?

    Zeitschrift für Vergleichende Physiologie

    (1970)
  • W.M. Farina et al.

    Social learning of floral odours inside the honeybee hive

    Proceedings of the Royal Society B

    (2005)
  • N.R. Franks et al.

    Teaching in tandem-running ants

    Nature

    (2006)
  • K. von Frisch

    The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees

    (1967)
  • M. Gil et al.

    Foraging reactivation in the honeybee Apis mellifera L.: factors affecting the return to known nectar sources

    Naturwissenschaften

    (2002)
  • C. Grüter et al.

    Propagation of olfactory information within the honeybee hive

    Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

    (2006)
  • C. Grüter et al.

    Informational conflicts created by the waggle dance

    Proceedings of the Royal Society B

    (2008)
  • C. Grüter et al.

    Decision making in ant foragers (Lasius niger) facing conflicting private and social information

    Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

    (2011)
  • Cited by (65)

    • Do honey bees modulate dance following according to foraging distance?

      2022, Animal Behaviour
      Citation Excerpt :

      As s serves as an integrative measure of the effect of each waggle run on the likelihood of successful recruitment, these opposing effects may have also contributed to the lack of difference observed across our treatments. In agreement with earlier studies (Grüter & Ratnieks, 2011; Grüter et al., 2013; Hasenjager et al., 2020), most foragers visited the empty recruit feeder several times before searching for the target feeder. Yet, rather than foragers being more reluctant to abandon this site when alternatives were more distant (as predicted), the opposite pattern was observed (Fig. 2).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text