Elsevier

Neurobiology of Aging

Volume 36, Issue 11, November 2015, Pages 3020-3028
Neurobiology of Aging

Regular article
Breadth and age-dependency of relations between cortical thickness and cognition

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.08.011Get rights and content

Abstract

Recent advances in neuroimaging have identified a large number of neural measures that could be involved in age-related declines in cognitive functioning. A popular method of investigating neural-cognition relations has been to determine the brain regions in which a particular neural measure is associated with the level of specific cognitive measures. Although this procedure has been informative, it ignores the strong interrelations that typically exist among the measures in each modality. An alternative approach involves investigating the number and identity of distinct dimensions within the set of neural measures and within the set of cognitive measures before examining relations between the 2 types of measures. The procedure is illustrated with data from 297 adults between 20 and 79 years of age with cortical thickness in different brain regions as the neural measures and performance on 12 cognitive tests as the cognitive measures. The results revealed that most of the relations between cortical thickness and cognition occurred at a general level corresponding to variance shared among different brain regions and among different cognitive measures. In addition, the strength of the thickness-cognition relation was substantially reduced after controlling the variation in age, which suggests that at least some of the thickness-cognition relations in age-heterogeneous samples may be attributable to the influence of age on each type of measure.

Introduction

A large number of measures of brain structure and brain function have been found to be negatively related to age, and many of these measures have also been found to be related to measures of cognitive functioning. Consider measures of cortical thickness, as assessed by the distance between the gray matter–cerebral spinal fluid (CSF) boundary and the gray matter–white matter boundary. Because it is postulated to reflect the density of neurons, dendrites, spines, synapses, and glial cells, cortical thickness is a potentially important neural substrate of cognition.

Negative relations between adult age and measures of cortical thickness have been reported in numerous studies (e.g., Ecker et al., 2009, Fjell et al., 2006, Fjell et al., 2009, Fjell et al., 2014, Hogstrom et al., 2013, Hutton et al., 2009, McGinnis et al., 2011, Salat et al., 2004, Salat et al., 2009, Tustison et al., 2014, Westlye et al., 2011), and many studies have also reported positive relations between measures of cortical thickness and cognitive functioning (e.g., Choi et al., 2008, Desrivieres et al., 2015, Ehrlich et al., 2012, Engvig et al., 2010, Fjell et al., 2006, Haier et al., 2009, Karama et al., 2009, Karama et al., 2011, Narr et al., 2007, Schilling et al., 2013, Walhovd et al., 2006, Westlye et al., 2009, Westlye et al., 2011; but see Colom et al., 2013). Based on these 2 sets of findings, it is tempting to postulate that age-related reductions in cortical thickness in specific neuroanatomical regions are involved in age-related reductions in particular types of cognitive functioning. However, we suggest that it is important to consider 2 issues when making these types of inferences; level of analysis, and the degree to which the relation between the 2 types of measures might be dependent on the relation of each measure with age.

Although sometimes considered individually, most neuroanatomical measures derived from different brain regions are highly related with one another, and most cognitive measures are highly related with one another. This lack of independence implies that some of the relations observed with an individual measure could be shared with influences that affect many measures and are not unique to the target measure. However, shared and unique influences cannot be distinguished unless multiple measures are examined in some type of organizational structure.

Consider Fig. 1 which portrays 3 possible organizations with sets of neural measures and cognitive measures. Panel A illustrates a situation with no structure in either the neural or cognitive measures. Neural-cognition relations could be investigated within a framework such as this by examining all possible combinations of neural measures and cognitive measures. However, this is almost never done because of the extremely large number of possible neural measures that could be obtained across different regions of the brain. Instead, analyses are often conducted to determine which clusters of neural measures are significantly related to particular cognitive measures. Any structure that emerges with this approach is therefore based on relations the neural measures have with that set of cognitive measures and does not necessarily reflect the intrinsic dimensionality of the neural measures, independent of their relations with other types of measures.

An alternative approach to investigate neural-cognition relations is portrayed in panel B in which the 2 types of measures are first grouped into factors representing shared individual difference variance, and then neural-cognition relations are examined at the level of factors instead of individual measures. Unlike the situation portrayed in panel A, interrelations among each type of measure are examined to determine a set of dimensions that parsimoniously represents the structure of individual differences within each type of measure before examining relations between the 2 types of measures.

A third possible structure is illustrated in panel C in which the measures are organized into both specific factors and a common factor. In this latter structure, the common factor represents influences shared among all measures, and the specific factors represent influences shared among subsets of measures that are independent of what is common across all measures. In the psychometric literature, this type of model is known as a bifactor (or nested-factors or orthogonal common factor) model, and the common factor is often designated g.

Bifactor and related hierarchical models of cognitive functioning have been investigated in a large number of studies, including several examining influences associated with adult age (e.g., Hildebrandt et al., 2011, Salthouse and Davis, 2006, Salthouse and Ferrer-Caja, 2003, Salthouse, 2009, Schmiedek and Li, 2004). A consistent finding in these studies has been a moderately large influence of age on the common factor, with additional influences on one or more cognitive ability factors. Several studies have also examined relations of neural measures with structural models of cognitive ability. For example, Karama et al. (2011) and Menary et al. (2013) found that most of the relations between cortical thickness and cognition in samples of adolescents were evident at the level of the common cognitive factor, and Booth et al. (2013) and Penke et al. (2010) found that most of the relations between measures of white matter integrity and cognitive measures in older adults were at the level of the common cognitive factor.

Although cortical thickness is often measured across very small cortical regions, broad areas of significant thickness-cognition relations have been reported in many studies (e.g., Choi et al., 2008, Desrivieres et al., 2015, Karama et al., 2011, Menary et al., 2013, Narr et al., 2007, Walhovd et al., 2006), and some researchers have used a measure of the average thickness across multiple brain regions when examining relations with cognition (e.g., Hedden et al., 2014, Hutton et al., 2009, Schnack et al., 2015). We are aware of only 1 study in which interrelations of cortical thickness measures were examined. In that study, Ecker et al. (2009) found that a hierarchical model with first-order factors corresponding to different lobes provided a good fit to cortical thickness data based on 28 gyrus-defined brain regions. Significant interrelations of measures of regional volume (e.g., Kennedy et al., 2009, Raz et al., 2005) and white matter integrity (e.g., Li et al., 2012, Lovden et al., 2013, Penke et al., 2010, Penke et al., 2012, Wahl et al., 2010) have also been found in several recent studies, which implies that the measures could be organized into a relatively small number of factors.

Because very few measures within a given modality are unrelated to other measures in that modality, it is important to consider relations among the measures within a given modality when investigating relations between neural measures and cognitive measures. The method advocated here is to first determine the organizational structure with each type of measure, and then examine relations at the broadest or most general levels in the structures before considering any relations that might exist at more specific levels.

As noted earlier, reductions in cortical thickness with increased age could be postulated to contribute to age-related differences in cognitive performance. However, because many neural and cognitive measures are related to age, at least some of the relations between neural and cognitive measures could be attributable to the relations each type of measure has with age. This possibility can be investigated by comparing the cortical thickness–cognition relations before and after statistical control of the variability in age. The reasoning is that, if the neural-cognition relations are substantially reduced when there is little variation in age, at least some of the relations could be inferred to be attributable to the associations of both the neural and cognitive measures with age.

To summarize, the primary goal of the present study was to demonstrate the usefulness of a proposed analytical procedure for investigating neural substrates of age-cognition relations involving examination of the structure of cortical thickness measures and of cognitive measures, and investigating the levels in the respective structures at which the 2 types of measures are related to another. In addition, the thickness-cognition relations were examined before and after control of the variation in age to investigate the role of age on those relations.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were recruited using market-mailing procedures, flyers, and by word of mouth. Potential participants were initially screened by telephone to ensure that they met basic inclusion criteria (i.e., right handed, English speaking, no psychiatric or neurological disorders, and normal or corrected-to-normal vision). All participants found eligible via the initial telephone screen were further screened in person with structured medical, neurological, psychiatric, and neuropsychological

Results

The initial factor analysis on the 12 cognitive measures yielded 3 factors with eigenvalues >1. However, prior research with similar measures (e.g., Salthouse and Ferrer-Caja, 2003, Salthouse, 2009) suggested the existence of 4 factors, and therefore, the analysis was repeated after specifying 4 factors. This analysis resulted in the expected pattern of loadings of the measures on factors corresponding to vocabulary, memory, speed, and reasoning, as indicated in Table 2. Correlations among the

Discussion

A popular approach to investigating neural-cognition relations involves examining the association between a set of neural measures and a set of cognitive measures, with each neural-cognitive pair treated independently. For example, a bivariate analysis in the data from this study might focus on the relation between the score on the block design test and cortical thickness in the superior temporal region. The correlation between these measures in the present study was 0.34, which could be

Disclosure statement

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by grants from the National Institute on Aging (AG038465, PI Dr. Stern; R37AG024270, PI Dr. Salthouse). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The sponsors had no role in the study design, data collection, analysis or interpretation, writing of the report, or decision to submit the article for publication.

References (54)

  • M. Lövdén et al.

    Changes in perceptual speed and white matter microstructure in the corticospinal tract are associated in very old age

    Neuroimage

    (2014)
  • K. Menary et al.

    Associations between cortical thickness and general intelligence in children, adolescents and young adults

    Intelligence

    (2013)
  • D.H. Salat et al.

    Age-associated alterations in cortical gray and white matter signal intensity and gray to white matter contrast

    Neuroimage

    (2009)
  • T.A. Salthouse et al.

    Organization of cognitive abilities and neuropsychological variables across the lifespan

    Dev. Rev.

    (2006)
  • N.J. Tustison et al.

    Large-scale evaluation of ANTs and FreeSurfer cortical thickness measurements

    Neuroimage

    (2014)
  • M. Wahl et al.

    Microstructural correlations of white matter tracts in the human brain

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • K.B. Walhovd et al.

    Regional cortical thickness matters in recall after months more than minutes

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • L.T. Westlye et al.

    Increased sensitivity to effects of normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease on cortical thickness by adjustment for local variability in gray/white contrast: a multi-sample MRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2009)
  • T. Booth et al.

    Brain white matter tract integrity and cognitive abilities in community-dwelling older people: the Lothian Birth Cohort, 1936

    Neuropsychology

    (2013)
  • H. Buschke et al.

    Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning

    Neurology

    (1974)
  • Y.Y. Choi et al.

    Multiple bases of human intelligence revealed by cortical thickness and neural activation

    J. Neurosci.

    (2008)
  • S. Desrivieres et al.

    Single nucleotide polymorphism in the neuroplastin locus associates with cortical thickness and intellectual ability in adolescents

    Mol. Psychiatry

    (2015)
  • C. Ecker et al.

    Is there a common underlying mechanism for age-related decline in cortical thickness?

    Neuroreport

    (2009)
  • S. Ehrlich et al.

    Associations of cortical thickness and cognition in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls

    Schizophrenia Bulletin

    (2012)
  • B. Fischl et al.

    Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2004)
  • A.M. Fjell et al.

    High consistency of regional cortical thinning across multiple samples

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2009)
  • A.M. Fjell et al.

    Accelerating cortical thinning: unique to dementia or universal in aging?

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2014)
  • Cited by (39)

    • The role of neural flexibility in cognitive aging

      2022, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      The studies were approved by the Internal Review Board of the College of Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University, and participants provided written informed consent prior to participation. Additional details about procedures can be found in previous reports (Habeck et al., 2016; Salthouse et al., 2015b; Stern, 2009; Stern et al., 2014). In the study, 561 participants were enrolled to the study at baseline with at least one reference ability composite score.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text