Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 80, 8 January 2016, Pages 142-156
Neuropsychologia

Review article
From mind wandering to involuntary retrieval: Age-related differences in spontaneous cognitive processes

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.11.017Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Frequency of mind-wandering and involuntary retrieval decreases with increasing age.

  • Evidence for relatively preserved spontaneous prospective memory retrieval.

  • Cognitive control, motivation and current concerns may contribute to age difference.

Abstract

The majority of studies that have investigated the effects of healthy aging on cognition have focused on age-related differences in voluntary and deliberately engaged cognitive processes. Yet many forms of cognition occur spontaneously, without any deliberate attempt at engaging them. In this article we review studies that have assessed age-related differences in four such types of spontaneous thought processes: mind-wandering, involuntary autobiographical memory, intrusive thoughts, and spontaneous prospective memory retrieval. These studies suggest that older adults exhibit a reduction in frequency of both mind-wandering and involuntary autobiographical memory, whereas findings regarding intrusive thoughts have been more mixed. Additionally, there is some preliminary evidence that spontaneous prospective memory retrieval may be relatively preserved in aging. We consider the roles of age-related differences in cognitive resources, motivation, current concerns and emotional regulation in accounting for these findings. We also consider age-related differences in the neural correlates of spontaneous cognitive processes.

Introduction

The effects of healthy aging on cognition have been studied over many years, and it is now well established that increasing age is associated with changes in several cognitive domains including episodic memory, autobiographical memory, working memory and attention (Craik and Salthouse, 2000). However, the majority of studies of aging cognition to date have focused on age-related differences in voluntary and deliberately engaged cognitive processes. For example, in a typical episodic memory experiment, participants are asked to voluntarily recall a list of items seen earlier and in studies of autobiographical memory, participants are typically instructed to recall experiences from their everyday lives. Yet many forms of cognition occur spontaneously, without any deliberate attempt at engaging them. For instance, numerous studies during the past decade have shown that when people are asked to perform a cognitive task in the laboratory, they often spontaneously focus on task-irrelevant thoughts, an activity known as mind-wandering (MW; Smallwood and Schooler, 2015). Indeed, recent evidence suggests close to 50% of our daily lives may be spent engaging in MW (Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010). Despite its prevalence, however, relatively little is known about age-related changes in MW. Similarly, researchers have also delineated other spontaneous thought types such as involuntary autobiographical memory (IAM; Berntsen, 2009), intrusive thoughts (Rachman, 1981), intrusive memories (Holmes et al., 2004), involuntary semantic memories (Kvavilashvili and Mandler, 2004), spontaneously recurring tunes or “earworms” (Beaman and Williams, 2010), déjà vu (Brown, 2003) and some forms of prospective memory (PM) (McDaniel and Einstein, 2000). But we still know relatively little about the effects of aging on these forms of spontaneous cognition.

Despite the relatively modest amount of evidence available concerning the effects of aging on spontaneous cognition, we believe that the time is right to review what is known on this topic, for at least three reasons. First, interest in the general topic of spontaneous cognition has increased rapidly during recent years in both psychology and neuroscience (e.g., Andrews-Hanna et al., 2010; Christoff et al., 2011; O’Callaghan et al., 2015; Smallwood and Schooler, 2015), and the trend seems likely to continue. Second, the emergence of studies on MW in older adults during the past few years (e.g., Jackson and Balota, 2012; Krawietz et al., 2012; Maillet and Rajah, 2013; Nagamatsu et al., 2013; Staub et al., 2014) suggests an increasing focus on spontaneous cognition among cognitive aging researchers. Third, although studies of different spontaneous thought types are typically pursued independently of one another, we think that it is useful to consider them together in order to encourage questions and future investigations about similarities and differences among these processes as a function of aging. In the current review, we focus on four types of spontaneous cognitive processes for which some systematic evidence exists regarding cognitive aging: MW, IAM, intrusive thoughts and spontaneous PM retrieval. We will focus primarily on age-related differences in MW because this type of spontaneous cognition has been studied most extensively. By contrast, there has been virtually no research on age-related differences in earworms or involuntary semantic memory, so we do not consider these phenomena further. Similarly, there is little research on intrusive memories in older adults other than in the context of post-traumatic stress disorder (Clapp and Beck, 2012). Because the present review is concerned with healthy aging, consideration of psychological disorders is beyond our scope. Finally, we do not consider déjà vu, but refer interested readers to a recent overview of age-related differences in this phenomenon (Moulin et al., 2014).

When examining the literatures on MW, IAM, spontaneous PM and intrusive thoughts, one may notice differences in which aspects of spontaneous cognition have been most extensively investigated in each. For example, meta-awareness has been a topic of much interest in the MW literature ever since early studies demonstrated that MW often occurs without it (Schooler et al., 2004). In the IAM literature, there has been considerable interest on elucidating what cues trigger these spontaneous thoughts. Both laboratory and naturalistic studies have found that most IAM have identifiable triggers that tend to predominantly be external (i.e. triggered by an environmental stimulus or event) rather than internal (i.e. triggered by one's train of thought) (Berntsen, 1996, Schlagman and Kvavilashvili, 2008). In the PM literature, it was emphasized early on that PM can occur both spontaneously or deliberately, and researchers expended much effort in developing methodologies to assess spontaneity of thoughts (Einstein et al., 2005, McDaniel and Einstein, 2000). These differences in which aspects of thoughts have been most extensively studied in each literature likely reflect differences in research trends, as well as differences in methodologies used, rather than actual differences between the phenomena of interest. For example, a recent study suggests that similar to MW, intrusive thoughts can also occur without meta-awareness (Takarangi et al., 2014). In the MW literature, there has recently been a surge of interest in identifying triggers for MW (e.g., Smallwood, 2013), with studies showing that, similar to findings from the IAM literature (e.g., Schlagman and Kvavilashvili, 2008), the content of off-task thoughts can be modulated by task stimuli (McVay and Kane, 2013, Plimpton et al., 2015). There has also been a resurgence of interest in distinguishing between spontaneous and deliberate MW (e.g., Seli et al., 2015a, b), a topic that has been of interest for some time in the PM literature. These findings highlight that MW, IAM, spontaneous PM, and intrusive thoughts may share much in common, and that greater insight regarding age-related differences in spontaneous thoughts may emerge from considering these phenomena together rather than separately.

Two broad theories of cognitive changes with aging are particularly relevant for making predictions regarding age-related differences in spontaneous cognitive processes: the inhibition deficit theory (Hasher and Zacks, 1988) and the reduced cognitive resources theory (Craik, 1983, Craik, 1986, Craik and Byrd, 1982). The inhibition deficit theory of aging suggests that reduced inhibitory abilities in older adults facilitate the initial entrance into working memory of information that is off the goal path, and the prolonged maintenance of such information in working memory (Hasher and Zacks, 1988). Off-goal path thoughts include environmental distractions, evaluations of ongoing events (e.g., “this is not the best talk he’s given) and thoughts/plans unrelated to ongoing events (e.g., “I need to remember to make dinner reservations after the talk”). Thus, the inhibitory deficit theory leads to the prediction that older adults may be more likely than young adults to exhibit off-task spontaneous thoughts. There is some evidence that supports this idea, at least indirectly. For example, studies in which young and older adults produce narrative discourse indicate that older adults are more likely than young to spontaneously produce “off-topic” details that are not relevant to the main topic at hand (e.g., Arbuckle and Gold, 1993; Trunk and Abrams, 2009), and related findings have been reported in studies that require young and old adults to produce extended narratives that involve remembering past experiences, imagining future experiences, or describing a scene (Gaesser et al., 2011; Madore et al., 2014; for review, see Schacter et al., 2013). By contrast, some of the earliest direct evidence concerning MW suggested that older adults exhibit a reduction in MW compared to young adults (e.g., Giambra, 1989; Singer, 1961), a result seemingly inconsistent with predictions made based on the basis of the inhibitory deficit theory.

These findings may instead be more consistent with a second theory of aging that emphasizes a reduction in cognitive resources in older adults (Craik, 1983, Craik, 1986, Craik and Byrd, 1982, Lindenberger and Mayr, 2014). Craik (1986) argued that cognitive resources are particularly needed in tasks that provide minimal environmental support and instead rely on self-initiated processing. Thus Craik proposed that, because older adults have fewer cognitive resources compared to young adults, they should be particularly impaired in tasks requiring a high level of self-initiated processing, but minimally impaired in tasks with high environmental support. In the MW literature, it has been suggested that if older adults spend a greater proportion of their cognitive resources on an ongoing task or external activity compared to young adults, then they may have fewer resources left over to exhibit MW compared to young (e.g., Giambra, 1989; Krawietz et al., 2012; Maillet and Rajah, 2013). In the PM literature, the same theory has been used to suggest that older adults should exhibit preserved PM in tasks that rely on spontaneous processing of a PM cue (e.g. when the PM intention of pressing a special key is spontaneously triggered by a target word), but that older adults may exhibit reduced behavioral performance in tasks relying on deliberate PM (McDaniel and Einstein, 2000). In considering why the reduced cognitive resources theory of aging has been used to predict both an age-related reduction in one spontaneous thought type (MW) and the preservation of another (PM), it is important to note that while MW is task-irrelevant, cue-driven PM is thought to occur spontaneously as part of the ongoing task. Thus, from a reduced cognitive resource perspective, it may be argued that older adults should exhibit intact spontaneous processing of PM cues since this does not require the use of cognitive resources beyond those already used to perform the ongoing task, whereas older adults should exhibit reduced MW since MW does require additional processing resources. A related and more general prediction would be that spontaneous thinking that is cued by environmental stimuli in the focus of attention is preserved in aging, while spontaneous thinking that is environment-independent is reduced.

As we discuss in this article, there is now increasing evidence that factors other than cognitive resources, such as age-related differences in task interest, current concerns, and emotional regulation, may also play an important role in explaining age-related differences in frequency of spontaneous thoughts. In the following sections, we review, in turn, studies that have examined psychological and behavioral characteristics of MW, IAM, intrusive thoughts and spontaneous PM retrieval in young and old adults. Next, we review studies that have used neuroimaging to assess effects of aging on the neural correlates of these thought types. We conclude by integrating findings across these different literatures and linking them to observations from related studies of cognitive aging.

Section snippets

Definition and methodological aspects

MW refers to a shift of attention away from an ongoing task and/or from events in the external environment, towards the processing of self-generated thoughts about a different time and place, which often occurs without meta-awareness (Smallwood and Schooler, 2006, Smallwood and Schooler, 2015). A common example is reading to the end of a paragraph, only to realize that one’s attention was not focused on the text, and thus having to start over. Although MW is usually thought of as being a

Definition and methodological aspects

We now shift our focus to studies that have examined age-related differences in involuntary autobiographical memory (IAM). IAMs have been defined as personal memories that come to mind spontaneously, without any deliberate attempt to retrieve them; they simply “pop up” in response to an environmental trigger or to an ongoing train of thought (Berntsen, 2009). An interesting question is whether IAM should be regarded as equivalent to past-oriented spontaneous MW (see Plimpton et al., 2015 for a

Intrusive thoughts

Intrusive thoughts refer to repeatedly occurring, unwanted, and difficult to control thoughts that are generally accompanied by subjective discomfort (Rachman, 1981). Intrusive thoughts share with MW and IAM their spontaneous and autobiographical nature. However, intrusive thoughts are associated with negative appraisal and stress (e.g., Brose et al., 2011), whereas these features are not generally associated with MW and IAM. Three main methods have been used to study age-related differences in

Definition and methodological aspects

Prospective memory refers to memory for actions to be performed in the future, for example, remembering to make a phone call once one gets home. In a typical PM laboratory task (e.g., Einstein and McDaniel, 1990; Einstein et al., 1995), subjects perform an ongoing task (e.g. pleasantness judgements on a word list), while remembering to press a special key when a target word (PM cue) appears. A critical component of PM tasks is that there is no explicit prompt to initiate memory retrieval:

Age-related differences in the neural correlates of MW

We have focused so far on cognitive and behavioral studies, but one promising avenue for future research concerns the investigation of age-related differences in the neural correlates of MW, IAM, intrusive thoughts, and spontaneous PM retrieval. In young adults, studies of individual differences in MW frequency, and of comparisons of periods of MW relative to on-task reports have revealed that MW is associated with activation in regions of the default network, a set of regions involved in

Conclusions and additional considerations

In summary, the studies reviewed here suggest that healthy older adults generally exhibit a decrease in frequency of MW, whether measured with thought probes in the laboratory or with retrospective questionnaires. IAM studies using diary methodologies have also reported age-related decreases, whereas studies measuring retrospective estimates of IAM in everyday life have predominantly reported no age-related differences. Studies using experimenter-induced intrusive thoughts have found either no

Acknowledgments

We thank Paul Seli for comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. This work was supported by a Fonds de la Recherche du Québec postdoctoral training award to D. Maillet and by National Institute on Aging Grant AG08441 to D. Schacter.

References (131)

  • L. Hasher et al.

    Workng memory, comprehension, and aging: a review and a new view

    Psychol. Learn.

    (1988)
  • K. Kopp et al.

    Influencing the occurrence of mind wandering while reading

    Conscious Cogn.

    (2015)
  • L. Kvavilashvili et al.

    Out of one's mind: a study of involuntary semantic memories

    Cogn. Psychol.

    (2004)
  • E.D. Leshikar et al.

    The impact of increased relational encoding demands on frontal and hippocampal function in older adults

    Cortex

    (2010)
  • U. Lindenberger et al.

    Cognitive aging: is there a dark side to environmental support?

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2014)
  • C. Lustig et al.

    Questions of age differences in interference control: when and how, not if?

    Brain Res.

    (2015)
  • D. Maillet et al.

    Dissociable roles of default-mode regions during episodic encoding

    Neuroimage

    (2014)
  • M. Mather et al.

    Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2005)
  • M.A. McDaniel et al.

    The neuropsychology of prospective memory in normal aging: a componential approach

    Neuropsychologia

    (2011)
  • J.C. McVay et al.

    Aging ebbs the flow of thought: adult age differences in mind wandering, executive control, and self-evaluation

    Acta Psychol.

    (2013)
  • K. Mevel et al.

    Age effect on the default mode network, inner thoughts, and cognitive abilities

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2013)
  • C. O’Callaghan et al.

    Shaped by our thoughts--a new task to assess spontaneous cognition and its associated neural correlates in the default network

    Brain Cogn.

    (2015)
  • B. Plimpton et al.

    Role of triggers and dysphoria in mind-wandering about past, present and future: A laboratory study

    Conscious Cogn.

    (2015)
  • S. Rachman

    Part I. Unwanted intrusive cognitions

    Adv. Behav. Res. Ther.

    (1981)
  • M.N. Rajah et al.

    Age-related changes in prefrontal cortex activity are associated with behavioural deficits in both temporal and spatial context memory retrieval in older adults

    Cortex

    (2010)
  • A.S. Rasmussen et al.

    The unpredictable past: spontaneous autobiographical memories outnumber autobiographical memories retrieved strategically

    Conscious Cogn.

    (2011)
  • D.R. Addis et al.

    Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events

    Psychol. Sci.

    (2008)
  • J.R. Andrews-Hanna et al.

    Evidence for the default network's role in spontaneous cognition

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2010)
  • J.S. Antrobus et al.

    Studies in the stream of consciousness: experimental enhancement and suppression of spontaneous cognitive processes

    Percept. Mot. Ski.

    (1966)
  • T.Y. Arbuckle et al.

    Aging, inhibition, and verbosity

    J. Gerontol.

    (1993)
  • V. Axelrod et al.

    Increasing propensity to mind-wander with transcranial direct current stimulation

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (2015)
  • R.A. Baer et al.

    Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness

    Assessment

    (2006)
  • C.P. Beaman et al.

    Earworms (stuck song syndrome): towards a natural history of intrusive thoughts

    Br. J. Psychol.

    (2010)
  • S.L. Bengtsson et al.

    Motivation to do well enhances responses to errors and self-monitoring

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2009)
  • D. Berntsen

    Involuntary autobiographical memories

    Appl. Cogn. Psychol.

    (1996)
  • D. Berntsen

    Involuntary Autobiographical Memories

    (2009)
  • D. Berntsen et al.

    Emotionally charged autobiographical memories across the life span: the recall of happy, sad, traumatic, and involuntary memories

    Psychol. Aging

    (2002)
  • E. Borella et al.

    Working memory, control of interference and everyday experience of thought interference: when age makes the difference

    Aging Clin. Exp. Res.

    (2007)
  • A. Brose et al.

    Normal aging dampens the link between intrusive thoughts and negative affect in reaction to daily stressors

    Psychol. Aging

    (2011)
  • A.S. Brown

    A review of the deja vu experience

    Psychol. Bull.

    (2003)
  • R.L. Buckner et al.

    The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2008)
  • Carlson, E,B., Putnam, F,W., (1993). An update on the dissociative experiences scale. Dissociation,...
  • K. Christoff et al.

    Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (2009)
  • K. Christoff et al.

    The role of spontaneous thought in human cognition

  • A.-L. Cohen et al.

    The intention interference effect and aging: Similar magnitude of effects for young and old adults

    Appl. Cogn. Psychol.

    (2005)
  • P. Costa et al.

    Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual

    (1992)
  • F.I.M. Craik

    On the transfer of information from temporary to permanent memory

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond.

    (1983)
  • F.I.M. Craik

    A functional account of age differences in memory

  • F.I.M. Craik et al.

    Aging and cognitive deficits: the role of attentional resources

  • F.I.M. Craik et al.

    The Handbook of Aging and Cognition

    (2000)
  • Cited by (93)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text