Orbitofrontal cortex inactivation impairs between- but not within-session Pavlovian extinction: An associative analysis

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.08.002Get rights and content

Abstract

The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is argued to be the neural locus of Pavlovian outcome expectancies. Reinforcement learning theories argue that extinction learning in Pavlovian procedures is caused by the discrepancy between the expected value of the outcome (US) that is elicited by a predictive stimulus (CS), and the lack of experienced US. If the OFC represents Pavlovian outcome expectancies that are necessary for extinction learning, then disrupting OFC function prior to extinction training should impair extinction learning. This was tested. In experiment 1, Long Evans rats received infusions of saline or muscimol targeting the lateral OFC prior to three appetitive Pavlovian extinction sessions. Muscimol infused into the OFC disrupted between-session but not within-session extinction behaviour. This finding was not due to muscimol infusions disrupting the memory consolidation process per se as there was no effect of muscimol infusion when administered immediately post session (experiment 2). These findings support a role for the OFC in representing outcome expectancies that are necessary for learning. A number of ways in which disrupting outcome expectancy information might block learning will be discussed in the context of traditional associative learning theories and the associative structures they depend on.

Introduction

Extinction learning procedures form the basis of a number of animal models of psychopathology and behavioural therapy (Huston et al., 2013, Kaplan et al., 2011, Laborda et al., 2012). Similarly, learning and performance in these animal models and in extinction depend on limbic, hippocampal and prefrontal cortical structures (Delamater, 2004, Herry et al., 2010, Sah and Westbrook, 2008). In simple Pavlovian extinction procedures a cue (CS) that reliably predicts an outcome (US; e.g. the delivery of food) is presented in the absence of the US. As extinction proceeds, conditioned responses (CRs) to the CS decline and new extinction learning occurs (Bouton, 1993, Delamater, 2004, Rescorla, 2004). It is therefore important to specify the exact role that these neural regions might play in supporting the associative structures underlying Pavlovian extinction.

Reinforcement learning theories argue that extinction learning in Pavlovian procedures is caused by the discrepancy between the expected value of the US that is elicited by the CS, and the lack of experienced US (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972, Sutton and Barto, 1998). This discrepancy between expected and actual outcomes generates a prediction error signal that drives learning. The rodent orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) has been proposed as a neural site for representing US expectancy information (Delamater, 2007, Ostlund and Balleine, 2007, Schoenbaum et al., 2011). For example, in an odour discrimination task single unit neural firing increases in the period after CS presentation in anticipation of the US (Roesch, Calu, Burke, & Schoenbaum, 2007). It is therefore likely that OFC activity is required for prediction error learning in extinction. This is in line with other evidence that OFC lesions and inactivation impair reversal learning, a procedure that involves extinction learning (Boulougouris et al., 2007, Butter, 1969, Rudebeck and Murray, 2008, Schoenbaum et al., 2003). The non-human primate OFC is also implicated in extinction (Izquierdo and Murray, 2005, Reekie et al., 2008).

Recently, Burke, Takahashi, Correll, Brown, and Schoenbaum (2009) found that inactivation of rodent OFC did not impair extinction learning in a Pavlovian learning task. This finding is at odds with the hypothesised role of the OFC in representing Pavlovian US expectancy. However, the rats used in this experiment had previously been exposed to extinction of the cues that were used before inactivation (Takahashi et al., 2009). Furthermore, while deficits in reversal learning is the hallmark impairment associated with OFC (Boulougouris et al., 2007), this impairment is abolished when animals are exposed to reversal learning prior to OFC damage (Boulougouris & Robbins, 2009). Therefore, Burke et al.’s (2009) failure to find an effect of OFC inactivation in extinction may well have been due to the animals having pre-exposure to extinction. The re-expression of extinction may not require prediction error learning (Hart, Harris, & Westbrook, 2010) and hence may be independent of outcome expectancy and the OFC.

The first experiment sought to establish the role of the OFC in Pavlovian extinction. Pavlovian extinction learning has been shown to be dependent on prediction error signal (Leung and Westbrook, 2008, Steinberg et al., 2013). Prediction error signalling in dopamine neurons has been found to be dependent on outcome expectancy information from the OFC (Takahashi et al., 2011). Together, these findings suggest that the OFC should be necessary for prediction error learning in Pavlovian extinction.

We conducted two experiments to directly assess the role of the rodent OFC in simple Pavlovian extinction learning. Both experiments used an appetitive Pavlovian procedure in which animals initially acquired an auditory click CS – food pellet US association. Cannulae targeting OFC were then implanted, and OFC function was inactivated (via micro-infusions of the GABAA agonist muscimol) prior to three sessions of extinction training (experiment 1). Experiment 2 replicated the procedures in experiment 1 except that post-session OFC inactivation was used to determine if the results of experiment 1 were due to disruption of memory consolidation processes.

Section snippets

Subjects

Subjects were sixteen (total N = 16) male Long Evans rats (Monash Animal Services, Gippsland, Victoria, Australia) approximately 4 months old, weighing between 303 and 384 g. Rats were housed eight per cage under a 12:12 h light: dark cycle (lights on at 7am) with a constant temperature of 22 °C. Rats were placed on a food restriction schedule, with water freely available, ensuring they never reduced to below 85% of their free feeding weight. All training occurred during the light cycle of the colony

Histology

Cannulae placements are illustrated in Fig. 1A. One animal from the muscimol group was excluded from analysis as a result of a cannula tip placed within white matter. Cannulae were located within lateral or dorsolateral OFC for the remaining rats, and showed no white matter damage. Final group numbers were saline (n = 8) muscimol (n = 7).

Final day of acquisition

On the final acquisition day (Fig. 2A), discriminative responding to the stimulus did not differ between groups. A Group (muscimol, saline) × Period (Pre, CS) mixed

Discussion

The present studies tested the hypothesis that if the rat OFC is necessary for representing CS generated outcome expectancy values that are required for learning in extinction then OFC inactivation should impair Pavlovian extinction. In experiment 1, we found that functional inactivation of the rat OFC immediately prior to extinction sessions impaired extinction learning between days. Furthermore, the findings from experiment 2 suggest that the OFC is not the site of the memory consolidation

Conclusions

The role of the OFC in CS generated outcome expectancies is supported by a number of behavioural experiments incorporating neural recording, excitotoxic lesions and functional inactivation (Delamater, 2007, Gallagher et al., 1999, Ostlund and Balleine, 2007, Pickens et al., 2003, Pickens et al., 2005, Ramirez and Savage, 2007, Takahashi et al., 2009, van Duuren et al., 2008, van Wingerden et al., 2010). Associative theories provide concrete predictions about how manipulating OFC function should

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge Fred Westbrook, Andrew Delamater and Nathan Holmes for their help discussing these data. Research supported by grants to Simon Killcross from the Australian Research Council (ARC Discovery Grant DP0989027 and DP120103564).

References (61)

  • K.A. Burke et al.

    Orbitofrontal inactivation impairs reversal of Pavlovian learning by interfering with ‘disinhibition’ of responding for previously unrewarded cues

    European Journal of Neuroscience

    (2009)
  • Butter, C. M. (1969). Perseveration in Extinction and in Discrimination Reversal Tasks Following Selective Frontal...
  • Y. Chudasama et al.

    Dissociable contributions of the orbitofrontal and infralimbic cortex to pavlovian autoshaping and discrimination reversal learning: further evidence for the functional heterogeneity of the rodent frontal cortex

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2003)
  • J.W. Dalley et al.

    Time-limited modulation of appetitive Pavlovian memory by D1 and NMDA receptors in the nucleus accumbens

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA

    (2005)
  • A.R. Delamater

    Experimental extinction in Pavlovian conditioning: Behavioural and neuroscience perspectives

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B – Comparative and Physiological Psychology

    (2004)
  • A.R. Delamater

    The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in sensory-specific encoding of associations in Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning

  • A.R. Delamater

    On the nature of CS and US representations in Pavlovian learning

    Learning & Behavior

    (2012)
  • A.R. Delamater et al.

    Renewal and spontaneous recovery, but not latent inhibition, are mediated by gamma-aminobutyric acid in appetitive conditioning

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

    (2009)
  • Dickinson, A., & Dearing, M. F. (1979). Appetitive-aversive interactions and inhibitory processes. In A. Dickinson, R....
  • M. Gallagher et al.

    Orbitofrontal cortex and representation of incentive value in associative learning

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • Hall, G. (2002). Associative structures in Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning. In: C. R. Gallistel (Ed.), Stevens’...
  • J.A. Harris et al.

    Response rate and reinforcement rate in Pavlovian conditioning

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

    (2011)
  • G. Hart et al.

    Systemic or intra-amygdala infusion of the benzodiazepine, midazolam, impairs learning, but facilitates re-learning to inhibit fear responses in extinction

    Learning & Memory

    (2010)
  • C. Herry et al.

    Neuronal circuits of fear extinction

    European Journal of Neuroscience

    (2010)
  • J.P. Huston et al.

    Animal models of extinction-induced depression: Loss of reward and its consequences

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2013)
  • A. Izquierdo et al.

    Opposing effects of amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex lesions on the extinction of instrumental responding in macaque monkeys

    European Journal of Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • M.A. Laborda et al.

    Animal models of psychopathology: Historical models and the pavlovian contribution

    Terapia Psicologica

    (2012)
  • E.A. Lantz

    Effect of number of trials, interstimulus interval, and dishabituation during CS habituation on subsequent conditioning in a CER paradigm

    Animal Learning & Behavior

    (1973)
  • M.E. LePelley

    The role of associative history in models of associative learning: A selective review and a hybrid model

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (2004)
  • H.T. Leung et al.

    Spontaneous recovery of extinguished fear responses deepens their extinction: a role for error-correction mechanisms

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

    (2008)
  • Cited by (22)

    • D-Cycloserine facilitates fear extinction in adolescent rats and differentially affects medial and lateral prefrontal cortex activation

      2018, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, the nature of this role remains unclear. OFC may encode the prediction error critical for instructing fear extinction when the CS no longer predicts the US (Panayi and Killcross, 2014), so DCS may act to augment this error and deepen extinction learning. Based on this hypothesis, one would not anticipate DCS upregulating pMAPK levels in the OFC in animals merely exposed to a novel context (i.e., the No Extinction condition; Experiment 2).

    • Functional inactivation of the orbitofrontal cortex disrupts context-induced reinstatement of alcohol seeking in rats

      2018, Drug and Alcohol Dependence
      Citation Excerpt :

      Further, OFC neurons fired in anticipation of the presentation of either reward or punishment during an odor-discrimination task (Schoenbaum et al., 1998; Schoenbaum et al., 2003). Moreover, evidence has demonstrated that the OFC plays an important role in the acquisition and expression of Pavlovian stimulus-outcome associations (Ostlund and Balleine, 2007; Panayi and Killcross, 2014). Overall, these findings indicated that neural activity of the OFC and associated limbic areas plays a role in the memory or motivational significance of associated stimuli to guide goal-directed behavior.

    • Reward loss and addiction: Opportunities for cross-pollination

      2017, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      These results were interpreted as a lesion-induced deficit in a nonassociative factor (e.g., drive) tied to reward loss. A role of the OFC in reward omission has also been suggested by recent work showing impaired appetitive extinction after functional inactivation of the OFC in rats (Panayi and Killcross, 2014), and on the observation of complex patterns of brain activation and deactivation during instrumental extinction in humans (Finger et al., 2008). Several possible disruptions on psychobiological processes have been associated with mPFC function under addiction states.

    • Back to basics: Making predictions in the orbitofrontal-amygdala circuit

      2016, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
      Citation Excerpt :

      Further, several studies have found reductions in neural activity in anticipation of a devalued reward in OFC or BLA (Critchley & Rolls, 1996; Gottfried, 2003). Both OFC and BLA have also been found to be critical for extinction, the process whereby humans and animals learn that a cue that was previously predictive of some outcome no longer leads to that outcome (Falls, Miserendino, & Davis, 1992; Gottfried & Dolan, 2004; Laurent & Westbrook, 2008; Panayi & Killcross, 2014). Similarly, they have both been implicated in reversal of previously-acquired contingencies, though only the OFC is now thought to be functionally necessary for reversal learning (Butter, 1969; Rudebeck & Murray, 2008; Schoenbaum, Nugent, Saddoris, & Setlow, 2002; Schoenbaum, Setlow, Saddoris, & Gallagher, 2003), and then only under some conditions (Jang et al., 2015; Machado & Bachevalier, 2007; Rudebeck & Murray, 2011; Rudebeck, Saunders, Prescott, Chau, & Murray, 2013; Walton, Behrens, Buckley, Rudebeck, & Rushworth, 2010).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text