Language and Conceptual Development series
Core systems of number

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002Get rights and content

Abstract

What representations underlie the ability to think and reason about number? Whereas certain numerical concepts, such as the real numbers, are only ever represented by a subset of human adults, other numerical abilities are widespread and can be observed in adults, infants and other animal species. We review recent behavioral and neuropsychological evidence that these ontogenetically and phylogenetically shared abilities rest on two core systems for representing number. Performance signatures common across development and across species implicate one system for representing large, approximate numerical magnitudes, and a second system for the precise representation of small numbers of individual objects. These systems account for our basic numerical intuitions, and serve as the foundation for the more sophisticated numerical concepts that are uniquely human.

Section snippets

Core system 1 in infants

Even in infancy, children exhibit numerical knowledge. Xu and Spelke tested 6-month-old infants' discrimination of the numerosities 8 vs. 16 using a habituation paradigm [2]. Infants first saw repeated presentations of either 8 or 16 dots (Figure 1a). Careful controls for non-numerical dimensions ensured that infants responded to numerosity only (see Box 1). When tested with alternating arrays of 8 and 16 dots, infants looked longer at the numerically novel test arrays regardless of whether

Core system 2 in infants

The approximate system is not our only source of numerical information. Infants and adults have a second system for precisely keeping track of small numbers of individual objects and for representing information about their continuous quantitative properties.

In one experiment, 10- and 12-month-old infants chose between two quantities of hidden crackers (Figure 1c) [20]. Infants watched an experimenter sequentially hide, for example, one cracker in a bucket on the left, and 1+1=2 crackers in a

The core systems' shared heritage

Core representations of number are common across many species. When given tasks comparable with those presented to human infants and adults, animals show the same signature limits, suggesting that core knowledge of number depends on mechanisms with a long phylogenetic history.

Cerebral bases of the core systems

Recently, neuroimaging and neurophysiological techniques have begun to provide access to the neuronal underpinnings of the core number systems. The system for representing approximate numerical magnitudes has become well characterized and is associated by a convergent series of results with the bilateral horizontal segment of the intraparietal sulcus (HIPS; for a review see [40]). This brain region is implicated by both event-related potentials 41, 42 and fMRI 43, 44 as the source of the

Conclusion

Why is number so easy and yet so hard? Although studies of human infants have not definitively answered this question (see Box 2), they offer several suggestions. First, number is easy because it is supported by core systems of representation with long ontogenetic histories. One system serves to represent approximate numerical magnitudes independently of non-numerical quantities. Because this system is active early in infancy, humans are attuned to the cardinal values of arrays from the

References (68)

  • S. Carey et al.

    Infants' knowledge of objects: beyond object files and object tracking

    Cognition

    (2001)
  • J.R. Platt et al.

    Localization of position within a homogeneous behavior chain: effects of error contingencies

    Learn. Motiv.

    (1971)
  • M.D. Hauser et al.

    Spontaneous representations of small numbers of objects by rhesus macaques: examinations of content and format

    Cogn. Psychol.

    (2003)
  • S. Dehaene et al.

    Cerebral pathways for calculation: double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic

    Cortex

    (1997)
  • M. Delazer et al.

    Arithmetic facts without meaning

    Cortex

    (1997)
  • C. Lemer

    Approximate quantities and exact number words: dissociable systems

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • N. Molko

    Functional and structural alterations of the intraparietal sulcus in a developmental dyscalculia of genetic origin

    Neuron

    (2003)
  • A. Nieder et al.

    Coding of cognitive magnitude. Compressed scaling of numerical information in the primate prefrontal cortex

    Neuron

    (2003)
  • M. Bruandet

    A cognitive characterization of dyscalculia in Turner syndrome

    Neuropsychologia

    (2004)
  • K. Wynn

    Children's understanding of counting

    Cognition

    (1990)
  • K. Wynn

    Children's acquisition of the number words and the counting system

    Cogn. Psychol.

    (1992)
  • S.C. Johnson

    The recognition of mentalistic agents in infancy

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2000)
  • S. Dehaene

    The Number Sense

    (1997)
  • M.P. van Oeffelen et al.

    A probabilistic model for the discrimination of visual number

    Percept. Psychophys.

    (1982)
  • J.S. Lipton et al.

    Origins of number sense: large number discrimination in human infants

    Psychol. Sci.

    (2003)
  • Lipton, J.S. and Spelke, E.S. Discrimination of large and small numerosities by human infants. Infancy (in...
  • McKrink and Wynn Large number addition and subtraction by 9-month-old infants. Psychol. Sci. (in...
  • J. Whalen

    Nonverbal counting in humans: the psychophysics of number representation

    Psychol. Sci.

    (1999)
  • S. Dehaene et al.

    Development of elementary numerical abilities: a neuronal model

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (1993)
  • W.H. Meck et al.

    A mode control model of counting and timing processes

    J. Exp. Anal. Behav.

    (1983)
  • S. Cordes

    Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both small and large numbers

    Psychonomic Bull. Rev

    (2001)
  • G. Huntley-Fenner et al.

    Preschoolers' magnitude comparisons are mediated by a preverbal analog mechanism

    Psychol. Sci.

    (2000)
  • E. Temple et al.

    Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-olds and adults

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (1998)
  • R.S. Moyer et al.

    Time required for judgments of numerical inequality

    Nature

    (1967)
  • Cited by (1868)

    • Neurobiology of numerical learning

      2024, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text