Review
Music acquisition: effects of enculturation and formal training on development

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.008Get rights and content

Musical structure is complex, consisting of a small set of elements that combine to form hierarchical levels of pitch and temporal structure according to grammatical rules. As with language, different systems use different elements and rules for combination. Drawing on recent findings, we propose that music acquisition begins with basic features, such as peripheral frequency-coding mechanisms and multisensory timing connections, and proceeds through enculturation, whereby everyday exposure to a particular music system creates, in a systematic order of acquisition, culture-specific brain structures and representations. Finally, we propose that formal musical training invokes domain-specific processes that affect salience of musical input and the amount of cortical tissue devoted to its processing, as well as domain-general processes of attention and executive functioning.

Introduction

Interest in musical origins has increased dramatically in the past decade 1, 2, 3, 4. Fundamental to this topic is the question of how musical experiences affect development. It is clear that some aspects of musical competence, such as the ability to read music, require formal music lessons. However, just as children come to understand their spoken language, most individuals acquire basic musical competence through everyday exposure to music during development 5, 6, 7. Such implicit musical knowledge enables listeners, regardless of formal music training, to tap and dance to music, detect wrong notes, remember and reproduce familiar tunes and rhythms, and feel the emotions expressed in music. Recent work also suggests that explicit musical instruction, in addition to enhancing music-specific knowledge, substantially affects development of basic behaviors and neural processes in a range of domains and modalities. A key goal of current research in the field is to understand the effects of experience on domain-specific and domain-general developmental outcomes (Figure 1).

Here we outline two types of experience that fundamentally shape development: (1) ‘enculturation processes’, in which basic auditory capacities are modified by everyday experience listening to the music of a particular culture, and (2) ‘formal musical experience’, through which perception and production skills are trained to a high level, and musical knowledge becomes explicit.

Section snippets

Enculturation: from universal to system-specific processing

Musical enculturation is the process by which individuals acquire culture-specific knowledge about the structure of the music they are exposed to through everyday experiences, such as listening to the radio, singing and dancing. Just as there are different languages, there are many different musical systems, each with unique scales, categories and grammatical rules governing pitch and rhythmic structures [8]. Additionally, there are universal, or near universal, aspects of musical structure

Formal musical experience

Virtually all members of society acquire implicit culture-specific knowledge of the spectral and temporal structure of music, but there is a wide range of musical experience and expertise, with some individuals practicing and performing music for many hours a day over the course of many years. An increasing number of studies suggest that music lessons profoundly influence the developing brain, making music training a promising model for examining learning, brain plasticity and critical periods

Conclusions

Musically trained and untrained adults alike process music using perceptual and cognitive networks set up through experience. Throughout development, these networks become increasingly specialized for encoding the musical structure of a particular culture. Certain universal aspects of musical structure, such as the preference for consonance over dissonance, are found early in development and probably arise from properties of the basilar membrane and auditory nerve, in conjunction with general

References (71)

  • I. Peretz

    The nature of music from a biological perspective

    Cognition

    (2006)
  • A. Compston

    Music and the brain

    Brain

    (2006)
  • J. Spiro

    Music and the brain

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • L.J. Trainor

    Are there critical periods for musical development?

    Dev. Psychobiol.

    (2005)
  • S.E. Trehub et al.

    Infant music perception: domain-general or domain-specific mechanisms?

    Cognition

    (2006)
  • E. Bigand et al.

    Are we “experienced listeners”? A review of the musical capacities that do not depend on formal music training

    Cognition

    (2006)
  • I. Cross

    Music, cognition, culture and evolution

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2001)
  • S. Koelsch

    Investigating emotion with music: an fMRI study

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (2006)
  • Y.I. Fishman

    Consonance and dissonance of musical chords: neural correlates in auditory cortex of monkeys and humans

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2001)
  • S. Watanabe

    Discrimination of consonance and dissonance in Java sparrows

    Behav. Processes

    (2005)
  • L.J. Trainor

    Preference for sensory consonance in 2- and 4-month-old infants

    Music Percept.

    (2002)
  • N. Masataka

    Preference for consonance over dissonance by hearing newborns of deaf parents and hearing parents

    Dev. Sci.

    (2006)
  • J. McDermott et al.

    Are consonant intervals music to their ears? Spontaneous acoustic preferences in a nonhuman primate

    Cognition

    (2004)
  • J. McDermott et al.

    Nonhuman primates prefer slow tempos but dislike music overall

    Cognition

    (2006)
  • L.J. Trainor

    Automatic and controlled processing of melodic contour and interval information measured by electrical brain activity

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • T. Fujioka

    Musical training enhances automatic encoding of melodic contour and interval structure

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2004)
  • S. Koelsch et al.

    Towards a neural basis of music perception

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2005)
  • E.G. Schellenberg

    Children's implicit knowledge of harmony in Western music

    Dev. Sci.

    (2005)
  • S. Koelsch

    Children processing music: electric brain responses reveal musical competence and gender differences

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • E. Costa-Giomi

    Young children's harmonic perception

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2003)
  • E.E. Hannon et al.

    Infants use meter to categorize rhythms and melodies: implications for musical structure learning

    Cognit. Psychol.

    (2005)
  • B.H. Repp

    Production and synchronization of uneven rhythms at fast tempi

    Music Percept.

    (2005)
  • J.S. Snyder

    Synchronization and continuation tapping to complex meters

    Music Percept.

    (2006)
  • M.R. Jones

    Temporal aspects of stimulus-driven attending in dynamic arrays

    Psychol. Sci.

    (2002)
  • E.E. Hannon et al.

    Metrical categories in infancy and adulthood

    Psychol. Sci.

    (2005)
  • T.R. Bergeson et al.

    Infants’ perception of rhythmic patterns

    Music Percept.

    (2006)
  • J. Phillips-Silver et al.

    Feeling the beat: movement influences infant rhythm perception

    Science

    (2005)
  • J. Phillips-Silver et al.

    Hearing what the body feels: auditory encoding of rhythmic movement.

    Cognition

    (2007)
  • Trainor, L.J. et al. The primal role of the vestibular system in determining musical rhythm. Cortex (in...
  • E.E. Hannon et al.

    Tuning in to rhythms: infants learn more readily than adults

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2005)
  • L.S. Scott

    A domain general theory of the development of perceptual discrimination

    Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci.

    (2007)
  • J.D. McAuley

    The time of our lives: life span development of timing and event tracking

    J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.

    (2006)
  • J. Provasi et al.

    Spontaneous motor tempo and rhythmical synchronization in 2 ½- and 4-year-old children

    Int. J. Behav. Dev.

    (2003)
  • C. Drake

    Synchronizing with music: intercultural differences

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2003)
  • Cited by (314)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text