Tactile perception: Finger friction, surface roughness and perceived coarseness

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.triboint.2010.04.010Get rights and content

Abstract

Finger friction measurements performed on a series of printing papers are evaluated to determine representativeness of a single individual. Results show occasionally large variations in friction coefficients. Noteworthy though is that the trends in friction coefficients are the same, where coated (smoother) papers display higher friction coefficients than uncoated (rougher) papers. The present study also examined the relationship between the measured friction coefficients and surface roughness to the perceived coarseness of the papers. It was found that both roughness and finger friction can be related to perceived coarseness, where group data show that perceived coarseness increases with increasing roughness.

Introduction

This study forms part of a larger project with the overall scope to interconnect human tactile perception of various materials with their physical properties. While it is not yet established which surface properties determine tactile feel, it seems likely that in addition to the (visco)elastic properties of the material, properties such as topography and friction are important [1], [2]. In order to investigate the importance of various surface properties in tactile perception, there is a need for relevant techniques to measure assorted physical properties of the objects under investigation. Since skin is one of the contacting surfaces in tactile perception, one important and relevant physical measure is friction between a real finger and the surfaces of interest. Ideally the counterface for such studies should permit systematic variation of a limited number of parameters, such as for example roughness, while as many other variables as possible are held constant. Paper is a useful model surface in this regard since large areas of identical sample can be manufactured, and different manufacturing approaches lead to subtle differences in surface properties. Furthermore, there is a great interest from the paper community in controlling the tactile response to paper.

In the literature, a number of studies have emerged during the last couple of years, where friction has been measured while sliding a finger over a stationary surface [2], [3], [4], [5], [6], [7], [8], [9], [10]. Gee et al. [4] developed a friction device with the aim of measuring friction while sliding a finger over the sample surface in order to more accurately mimic sensory perception. Instead of the traditional technique of a probe sliding over the skin, they let a finger slide over the sample surface. This apparatus discriminated friction coefficients between a range of different materials, for example rubber, steel and glass. Whereas it is the dynamic friction coefficient that is most often considered, Lewis et al. studied grip on packaging materials by measuring the static coefficient of friction. Much focus lies in studying interactions between skin and fabrics, both by sliding a finger [5], [9] and a forearm [11], [12] over a textile. For example, Darden and Schwartz [9] measured friction between a fingertip and fabrics, and also investigated possible tactile attributes of fabrics using human evaluators. However, measured friction coefficients were not correlated well with the tactile descriptors found. In addition, a couple of other studies have combined finger friction measurements and sensory evaluation [2], [3], [6]. Childs and Henson [3] measured friction between a finger and screen-printed surfaces with different patterns. The participants stroked their index finger over the surface, while reporting their tactile perceptions of given word pairs such as “smooth–coarse” and “happy–sad”. Both the sliding friction coefficient and the roughness (peak separation) could partially be related to the perceived feelings. In another study, friction measurements between an index finger and different car interior materials and aluminium samples with different roughness have been performed by Liu et al. [2] in order to investigate touch-feel perception. Both measured surface roughness and friction could be correlated with the “rough–smooth” and “grippy–slippery” perceptions.

In order to obtain a better understanding of what is affecting tactile perception, perceptual features, measured with subjects, need to be studied. Most studies on tactile perception have a one-dimensional approach, where roughness is the most extensively investigated perceptual feature [13], [14], [15], [16].

The aim of this paper was to investigate if and how perceived coarseness (“strävhet” in Swedish) is linked to physical roughness and friction. Free magnitude estimation was used to scale the perceived coarseness on 8 coated and uncoated printing papers. Magnitude estimation is the most common psychophysical ratio scaling method [17]. The method is based on the hypothesis that people can make direct numerical estimations of their intensity impression of a stimulus. To avoid biased numerical judgements, free magnitude estimation was used.

The authors have already managed to measure friction between a moving finger and a stationary paper surface, using a piezoelectric force sensor [18]. These results employing a single participant show that measurements with the device can discriminate a set of similar samples such as printing papers in terms of finger friction. A negative correlation was also found between the measured friction and the surface roughness. A goal of this study is to determine whether finger friction measurements performed by an experienced, trained experimenter can be considered representative of a larger population.

In this article, finger friction measurements are reported together with a tactile experiment on perceived coarseness performed by a group of undergraduate students.

Section snippets

Paper samples

Finger friction measurements, together with a free magnitude estimation experiment of the perceived coarseness, were performed on 8 printing papers, provided by an industrial partner (KCL, Finland). These paper samples are listed in Table 1 in alphabetical order, together with their respective paper grade, coating and grammage. These papers were selected to cover a broad range of paper grades out of the 21 papers used in a previous study [18]. The coated papers are classified as woodfree coated

Friction measurements

Fig. 4 displays the average friction coefficients for the paper samples, sorted in decreasing order. The average friction coefficients range from 0.35 to 0.60. The WFC coated papers display the highest friction coefficient, followed by the coated papers with mechanical pulp. The uncoated papers show the lowest friction coefficient. To the Authors’ knowledge, there are only two studies where friction has been measured between a finger and a paper, and furthermore in those cases paper was only

Conclusions

A challenge in the field of tactile perception is isolating a single parameter for systematic variation while maintaining constant values of other variables. The choice of paper as a substrate appears to be a fortunate one in this regard; it is clearly possible for a group of participants to discriminate between different printing papers on the basis of the roughness and frictional interaction with a human finger. In terms of the representativeness of finger friction measurements performed by

Acknowledgements

Support for this work was provided by the Institute Excellence Centre CODIRECT (Controlled Delivery and Release Centre), which is sponsored by Vinnova, The Swedish Foundation for Strategic Research, The Knowledge Foundation and Industry. M.R. is a fellow of the Swedish Science Council. Thanks are given to Docent Mats Nilsson at the Department of Psychology at Stockholm University for fruitful discussions. We also extend our thanks to Kari Niemi at KCL for performing the surface roughness

References (25)

  • X. Liu et al.

    Quantifying touch-feel perception: tribological aspects

    Meas Sci Technol

    (2008)
  • T.H.C. Childs et al.

    Human tactile perception of screen-printed surfaces: self-report and contact mechanics experiments

    Proc Inst Mech Eng Part J-J Eng Tribol

    (2007)
  • Cited by (108)

    • New insights into ‘technologies of touch’: Information processing in product evaluation and purchase intention

      2021, Technological Forecasting and Social Change
      Citation Excerpt :

      Rendering tactile sensations through touchscreens varies from friction, force feedback (i.e., mechanical vibrations) and surface modifications. Whereas these techniques are continuously being improved (Allerkamp et al., 2007; Fuchs and Moreau, 2003; Gueorguiev et al., 2017; Philpott and Summers, 2012; Skedung et al., 2011; Unger et al., 2011; Zeng et al., 2010), simulating heat and cold is not yet fully possible while stiffness, hardness and texture rendering have become more accessible (Basdogan et al., 2020; Laycock and Day, 2007). Accordingly, in this research, we focus on texture rendering.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Now at VTT Technical Research Centre of Finland, P.O. Box 1000, FI-02044 VTT, Finland.

    View full text