Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Physiol., 01 February 2018
Sec. Invertebrate Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic IC-E3 - Insect Chemical Ecology, Ethology and Evolution View all 15 articles

Combinatorial Codes and Labeled Lines: How Insects Use Olfactory Cues to Find and Judge Food, Mates, and Oviposition Sites in Complex Environments

  • Department of Evolutionary Neuroethology, Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology, Jena, Germany

Insects, including those which provide vital ecosystems services as well as those which are devastating pests or disease vectors, locate their resources mainly based on olfaction. Understanding insect olfaction not only from a neurobiological but also from an ecological perspective is therefore crucial to balance insect control and conservation. However, among all sensory stimuli olfaction is particularly hard to grasp. Our chemical environment is made up of thousands of different compounds, which might again be detected by our nose in multiple ways. Due to this complexity, researchers have only recently begun to explore the chemosensory ecology of model organisms such as Drosophila, linking the tools of chemical ecology to those of neurogenetics. This cross-disciplinary approach has enabled several studies that range from single odors and their ecological relevance, via olfactory receptor genes and neuronal processing, up to the insects' behavior. We learned that the insect olfactory system employs strategies of combinatorial coding to process general odors as well as labeled lines for specific compounds that call for an immediate response. These studies opened new doors to the olfactory world in which insects feed, oviposit, and mate.

Introduction

While flying or walking through its natural environment any insect, be it a mosquito, a fly or a giant sphinx moth, encounters a nearly infinite number of chemical signals. These chemical messages differ widely in the specificity at which they target potential receivers as well as in their signal complexity (Junker et al., 2017). Some chemical signals, like insect sex pheromones for example target only conspecifics of the opposite sex, while a flower might aim to attract as many pollinators as possible with its odor bouquet. In parallel, a single compound might be enough for an insect to identify a toxic food source while in other cases an entire blend of compounds might be required to identify a suitable oviposition site. These different types of chemical messages, however, do not only arise in the way the message is produced by the sender, but also by the way these messages are coded on the sensory periphery and in the brain of the receiver. Over the last years, great progress has been made to decipher the code by which insects read the chemical messages of their environment using neurobiological and ethological methods (Hansson and Stensmyr, 2011). These methods form the cornerstone of this review in which we approach the neuronal computation, namely labeled lines, and combinatorial coding, used by insects while they are foraging and/or searching for oviposition sites or mates. We discuss the different systems by which insects might detect specific odors of significant importance or multiple odors in a combinatorial system within their environment.

The Neuronal Basis of Insect Olfaction

Due to the existence of comprehensive reviews on the insect olfactory system (de Bruyne and Baker, 2008; Hansson and Stensmyr, 2011), let us just briefly summarize: of all sensory stimuli, scent, and taste are the hardest to capture. Contrary to the visual and auditory systems that basically deal with wavelengths and amplitudes of stimuli along a linear scale, chemical senses detect stimuli, which might vary on dozens of properties, such as chain length, polarity, chirality, and many more. This multitude of properties presumably explains why the olfactory system requires many more different receptor types than for example the visual system. Insects detect volatile molecules with olfactory sensory neurons (OSNs) expressing either olfactory receptors (Ors), ionotropic receptors (Irs), or (rarely) gustatory receptors (Grs) (for a review on the function of chemosensory receptors see Touhara and Vosshall, 2009). OSNs expressing the same receptor project to the same spherical structure (called glomerulus) within the first olfactory processing center, the antennal lobe. Within the antennal lobe, OSNs connect to local interneurons and projection neurons that convey the olfactory information to higher brain centers like the mushroom bodies and the lateral horn. In the winged insects the number of described olfactory receptors ranges from 10 in lice (Kirkness et al., 2011) to ca 375 in some leafcutter ants (Zhou et al., 2015). The identity of the expressed Or (individual OSNs usually express only one Or gene) dictates to which volatiles an OSN will respond. From studies in the model Drosophila melanogaster, where most Ors have been deorphanized (Hallem and Carlson, 2006), we know that many receptors are broadly tuned and contribute to the so-called olfactory code: an OSN expressing a broadly tuned receptor detects several dozens of different compounds, and a specific compound can activate OSNs expressing different receptors. Hence, odor identity is coded by the activation of a subpopulation of those OSNs whose receptors interact with this odor. To complicate matters further, a certain odor source such as a flower or a ripening fruit, might emit a great variety of odors, detected by these broadly tuned receptors, resulting in the so-called “olfactory Gestalt” of the odor source (Dethier, 1982; Figures 1A,B). However, more recent investigations have shown that odors of outstanding ecological relevance are often detected by OSNs expressing highly selective Ors. Drosophila for example expresses Ors that are narrowly tuned to sex pheromones, or food and oviposition cues. For example, DmOr67d (each Drosophila Or is numbered based on the position of its gene within the Drosophila genome) detects 11-cis-vaccenyl acetate, a male produced pheromone that becomes transferred to females during copulation and inhibits further mating of the female (Kurtovic et al., 2007). DmOr47b is tuned to methyl laurate (and—following Lin et al.—also to palmitoleic acid), a pheromone carried by both males and females that increases courtship motivation and mating success (Dweck et al., 2015b; Lin et al., 2016; Zhuang et al., 2016). Furthermore, DmOr71a detects ethylphenols and governs the fly's preference for food enriched with healthy dietary antioxidants (Dweck et al., 2015a). In contrast to the labeled lines for pheromones, we call these “ecologically labeled lines,” meaning olfactory circuits tuned to specific ecologically relevant cues, whose activation leads to a predictable behavior, in this case feeding (Figure 1C; for an overview of examples of labeled lines and combinatorial coding see Table 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Processing of olfactory information in the insect antennal lobe. (Ai) Insects can easily navigate toward an odor source (orange), in front of a chemically distinct odor background. (Aii) This navigation however becomes corrupted, when the same volatiles are present both in the background as well as in the odor source (gray) (Riffell et al., 2014). (Bi) When the odor blends of two different host plants were presented spatially separated, both were attractive to the female moth (green), but (Bii) when these two blends were mixed both spatially and temporally into a novel mixture, this new blend became meaningless or even repellent to the moth (gray) (Spaethe et al., 2013). (C) Ecologically labeled lines (red) are processed independently of background odors or other odor plumes present in the environment of the insect (Badeke et al., 2016).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Examples of behaviorally investigated coding systems in vinegar flies, hawkmoths, and mosquitoes.

The Ecological Function of Labeled Lines

When it comes to the detailed analysis of an ecologically labeled line, Drosophila might not be an ideal model species (Hansson et al., 2010), yet the ability to specifically silence OSNs of interest (or even activate them artificially in the absence of the corresponding ligand) still gives the fly a head start before potential other study systems. This targeted activation or deactivation of course helps to decipher, whether a specific neuronal circuit is necessary and sufficient to elicit a certain behavior and whether this circuit fulfills the criteria for an ecologically labeled line. However, such ecologically labeled lines are not restricted to food-related cues but are also involved in finding and/or judging suitable oviposition sites. OSNs expressing DmOr19a are tuned to limonene (an odor associated with citrus fruits) and their activation increases the female's probability to oviposit (Dweck et al., 2013). Contrary to that, OSNs expressing either DmOr56a, or coexpressing DmOr49a and DmOr85f detect geosmin or iridomyrmecin, respectively. While the former compound is a key odor of harmful bacteria and mold, the latter is the sex pheromone of a genus of parasitoid wasps (Leptopilina), which oviposit in—and thereby finally kill—Drosophila larvae. Hence, both odors are associated with danger for the offspring and, accordingly, the activation of the OSNs detecting these odorants results in a decreased oviposition motivation (Stensmyr et al., 2012; Ebrahim et al., 2015).

However, it is also easy to understand, why the fly does not only express highly specific receptors: if all its 60 Ors would be narrowly tuned, a fly—in the most extreme case—would detect only 60 compounds, i.e., only a negligible part of the olfactory world with its aliphatic acids, alcohols, aldehydes, esters, ketones, as well as chemicals with aromatic, alicyclic, polycyclic, and heterocyclic ring structures, and the substituted chemicals of each of these types. From our current knowledge, we can estimate that in Drosophila about a tenth of the Or repertoire is narrowly tuned, focusing on specific cues, while the rest of the Ors detects general features of the habitat (for a detailed description of the tuning properties of Drosophila Ors see Grabe et al., 2016). Why then does the fly invest such a significant part of its olfactory system into labeled lines? One hypothesis could be, that the processes needed for the combinatorial code are time consuming, and that signals of sex or danger need instant responses. However, flies respond to food odors in <200 ms (Bhandawat et al., 2010; Steck et al., 2012) although these odors are detected by broadly tuned receptors (Hallem and Carlson, 2006). It is, hence, rather unlikely that the detection of e.g., harmful mold and the resulting oviposition blockade, or the detection of cVA on mated females requires faster responses than that. Another hypothesis could be that combinatorial coding with broad receptors is less efficient than labeled lines in discriminating structurally similar odors. In this case the fly rather should trust in narrowly tuned receptors when signals definitely should not become misinterpreted like sex pheromones or parasitoid odors, but rather should depend on broadly tuned receptors to identify e.g., all kinds of rotten fruits. Moreover, it is conceivable that the additional computation by local interneurons, which might be necessary to establish a combinatorial odor code, introduces metabolic costs that an insect aims to avoid whenever possible to resort to a computationally simpler labeled line (Sterling and Laughlin, 2015).

The concept of labeled lines has originally been borrowed from the literature on taste processing in vertebrates and has been intensively studied in many different insect species over the last decades (Dethier, 1982). The direct link between such a line and a specific behavior makes the insect vulnerable to pest management for example through pheromone traps. Hence, the identification of labeled lines might help us to develop novel counterstrategies against insect disease vectors.

For hematophagous insects, carbon dioxide plays a crucial role in controlling host searching behavior, by gating the response to thermal targets and important human host odors (McMeniman et al., 2014). In the malaria mosquito, Anopheles gambiae, CO2 is detected by the gustatory receptors AgGr 22-24 (Lu et al., 2007). Interestingly, genetically modified mosquitoes that lack functional olfactory receptors, lose their attraction to general host odors but are still strongly attracted to CO2 (DeGennaro et al., 2013), showing that CO2 alone is sufficient to provoke host-finding behavior. This has led to the development of many models of CO2 baited mosquito traps already in the last century (Reeves, 1951). In addition, it will be interesting to test, whether mosquitoes also possess labeled lines for signals of danger like the mold-sensing and parasitoid-sensing ecologically labeled lines in Drosophila (Stensmyr et al., 2012; Dweck et al., 2013; Ebrahim et al., 2015), as this could facilitate the development of effective long-range repellents.

Moreover, labeled lines may not only be established through narrowly tuned receptors, but also by positioning olfactory receptors at the part of the insect body, which is closely linked to the task at hand. Recent studies in hawkmoths identified OSNs at the tip of the proboscis as well as on the ovipositor (Haverkamp et al., 2016b; Klinner et al., 2016). Due to their position these olfactory neurons are likely to convey exclusive information about a suitable feeding or oviposition place, even though these neurons have been shown to detect a variety of compounds relevant for their behavioral function. Similar olfactory neurons have also been found on the proboscis of the mosquitoes A. gambiae and Aedes aegypti (Jung et al., 2015; Riabinina et al., 2016), where neurons convey specific information about the quality of a host, making them promising targets for novel close-range repellents. Based on these studies, we suggest to broaden the definition of an ecologically labeled line by including neuronal circuits dedicated to a certain olfactory stimulus and very specific behavioral task, independent of the tuning breadth of their receptors.

Odor Detection in A Chemically Complex Environment

In the present review, we do not enter into a discussion of the details on processing of positive or negative stimuli in higher brain centers as they were recently reviewed elsewhere (Sachse and Beshel, 2016), but rather discuss on how insects finally make use of the olfactory information. As already said, most of our knowledge regarding the neuronal and molecular basis of insect olfaction comes from Drosophila. Apart from this, scientists have today started to regard Drosophila not just as a model in a test tube but as an insect that evolved complex olfactory and behavioral strategies to cope with all kinds of environmentally-derived selection pressures. From our approaches we learned, that flies actively decide, whether or not to approach a food site or mating partner, or whether or not to drop eggs, and that they often do so based on olfactory cues. However, in spite of all the mentioned advantages of Drosophila, this fly feeds, mates, and oviposits within the rather narrowly defined world of a rotting fruit, which probably also limits the number of olfactory stimuli it has to process (Hansson et al., 2010). In contrast to this many other insects such as bees, mosquitoes, or moths may encounter a much wider and dynamic environment, which might then also require a higher degree of combinatorial coding. When it comes to olfactory-guided behavior, male Lepidoptera are probably the most famous example, as they locate ready-to-mate females over large distances just based on pheromone plumes. Apart from pheromone detection, olfaction is of course involved in many other tasks like host location (Proffit et al., 2015) and flower choice (Haverkamp et al., 2016a). Among the Lepidoptera, the hawkmoth Manduca sexta has developed into a major model species for insect neurophysiology, behavior, and ecology (Matsumoto and Hildebrand, 1981; Goyret et al., 2008; Riffell et al., 2013, 2014; Kessler et al., 2015; Sponberg et al., 2015; Levin et al., 2017).

Within its native range across North and South America M. sexta forages on a wide range of flowers from different plant species, which show the usual characteristics of the so the called “hawkmoth-pollination syndrome”: white corollas, slender corolla tubes and strong night-time volatile emissions (Riffell et al., 2013). Although very few, if any, compounds can be found that are present in all these flower bouquets, hawkmoth flowers still emit a characteristic blend of aromatic and terpenoid odors (Riffell et al., 2013). Hence, rather than focusing on a specific compound, the moths use a certain olfactory “Gestalt” to select a nectar resource or oviposition site (a phenomenon that was also observed in the malaria mosquito A. gambiae which targets odor combinations of hosts such as humans and cows, but not those of non-host species like chicken Majeed et al., 2016). In a series of experiments Riffell and his colleges reduced the blend complexity of a Datura wrightii flower, which normally contains 60–80 compounds to three essential volatiles: benzyl alcohol, benzaldehyde, and linalool (Riffell et al., 2009a,b, 2014). Notably though, the degree to which the moths were able to use this abstract representation of the D. wrightii flower bouquet was dependent on how chemically similar the odor background was to the target blend (Riffell et al., 2014; Figure 1A). These findings could indicate that the olfactory code, which an insect uses, might strongly depend on its olfactory environment i.e., in an environment that is olfactory very different from the odor of interest, few or even a single compound might be sufficient to provide reliable information, whereas a more complex representation might be required in an olfactory environment that is of lower contrast to the target odor. Interestingly, a slightly different form of background-dependent odor coding has been found in the malaria mosquito Anopheles coluzzii (A. gambiae sensu stricto molecular “M form”; van Loon et al., 2015). Here, it was shown that although under ambient CO2 conditions a three compound mix of ammonia, lactic acid, and tetradecanoic acid was sufficient to mimic a human host for blood-feeding (Smallegange et al., 2012), different other compounds could alter the attractiveness of this blend, but only under elevated CO2 levels (van Loon et al., 2015). Hence in case of the mosquito, CO2 probably helps the olfactory system to decide whether certain compounds belong to the general environment or to a potential host. Importantly, the ability of the mosquito to perceive a certain combination of odorants as a host blend might also be disrupted when artificial compounds are released from the same emission side as the natural blend. Verhulst and colleges for example found that isopropyl tetradecanoate, a compound commonly found in deodorants, significantly reduces the attraction of mosquitoes to the part of the human body to which the deodorant was applied (Verhulst et al., 2016), likely due to the formation of a novel odor blend, which was not recognized any longer by the mosquito as a host profile.

Two further experiments with hawkmoths taught us how complex such information gained from a combinatorial code can be: M. sexta exhibits an innate preference to the flower odors of D. wrightii, and (to a lower extend) to flowers of Nicotiana attenuata. This preference significantly increases, when one does not only present the floral odors alone, but presents them together with the leaf odors of the same plant. Hence, one could argue that both floral odors and leaf odors activate individual OSN populations and that the additive activation of whatever “positive” signals results in an increased attraction. However, the synergistic effect that was observed when odors from flowers and leaves of the same species were mixed, disappeared, when instead the flower and leaf odors from different species were mixed (Kárpáti et al., 2013). Similarly, ovipositing moths become highly attracted by leaf odors of N. attenuata and D. wrightii, but ignore the source as soon as both odors are mixed (Figure 1B; Spaethe et al., 2013). Obviously, it is not just a specific set of OSNs that, whenever it becomes activated, dictates the moth's behavior, but rather the combination of activated OSN populations that tell the moth, whether a scent is meaningful or not.

Combinatorial coding hence seems to be complex but highly important in the decision making of both moths and mosquitoes. Contrary to Drosophila, where several ecologically labeled lines have been identified (for a review see Mansourian and Stensmyr, 2015), the only so far fully unraveled labeled lines in these insects are involved in pheromone detection in case of the hawkmoth (Christensen and Hildebrand, 1987) or in CO2 detection in case of the mosquitoes. As with CRISPR/Cas9 (Doudna and Charpentier, 2014) finally molecular tools have emerged that e.g. can knockout genes of interest, future studies hopefully will reveal, whether the olfactory systems of M. sexta, mosquitoes and other insects follow the same logic like the Drosophila one, i.e., a functional combination of combinatorial code and ecologically labeled lines. However, to fully tackle these questions, it will also be necessary to follow the animal into its natural environment and to analyze the nervous system while it is dealing with real life challenges. Although, such undertakings might still appear far off, the advancements made in recording and stimulating neurons of freely flying dragonflies and hawkmoth (Hinterwirth et al., 2012; Thomas et al., 2012), have also shown us that analyzing the insect brain in its natural habitat might in fact already be in reach (Dickinson, 2015).

But how could such knowledge be applied? The understanding of labeled lines, their ligands, and their behavioral impact has already helped us to better control agricultural pest insects. Sex pheromones for example are widely used for trapping and mating disruption both in crop production and food storage (Reisenman et al., 2016), while at the same time protecting the important ecosystem function of many insects, such as pollination (Klein et al., 2007). Apart from crop-related issues, the world is facing huge problems due to insect vectors, such as mosquitoes, spreading diseases like Malaria or Zika. So far no long-distance sex pheromones in mosquitoes have been identified that could be used for trapping and/or mating disruption. However, any identified ecologically labeled line and its corresponding ligand could open up new possibilities for controlling insect pests and disease vectors as well as for supporting insect ecosystem services such as pollination or pest control.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the outline of this review. AH and MK: wrote the first draft of the manuscript and all authors contributed to the revision.

Funding

This work was supported by the Max-Planck Society.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Danny Kessler and Anna Späthe for providing some of the original images.

References

Badeke, E., Haverkamp, A., Hansson, B. S., and Sachse, S. (2016). A challenge for a male noctuid moth? Discerning the female sex pheromone against the background of plant volatiles. Front. Physiol. 7:143. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2016.00143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becher, P. G., Bengtsson, M., Hansson, B. S., and Witzgall, P. (2010). Flying the fly: long-range flight behavior of Drosophila melanogaster to attractive odors. J. Chem. Ecol. 36, 599–607. doi: 10.1007/s10886-010-9794-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhandawat, V., Maimon, G., Dickinson, M. H., and Wilson, R. I. (2010). Olfactory modulation of flight in Drosophila is sensitive, selective and rapid. J. Exp. Biol. 213, 3625–3635. doi: 10.1242/jeb.040402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christensen, T. A., and Hildebrand, J. G. (1987). Male-specific, sex pheromone-selective projection neurons in the antennal lobes of the moth Manduca sexta. J. Comp. Physiol. A 160, 553–569. doi: 10.1007/BF00611929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Bruyne, M., and Baker, T. C. (2008). Odor detection in insects: volatile codes. J. Chem. Ecol. 34, 882–897. doi: 10.1007/s10886-008-9485-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeGennaro, M., McBride, C. S., Seeholzer, L., Nakagawa, T., Dennis, E. J., Goldman, C., et al. (2013). Orco mutant mosquitoes lose strong preference for humans and are not repelled by volatile DEET. Nature 498, 487–491. doi: 10.1038/nature12206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dethier, V. G. (1982). Mechanism of host-plant recognition. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 31, 49–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1570-7458.1982.tb03118.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickinson, M. H. (2015). Motor control: how dragonflies catch their prey. Curr. Biol. 25, R232–R234. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doudna, J. A., and Charpentier, E. (2014). The new frontier of genome engineering with CRISPR-Cas9. Science 346, 1077–1076. doi: 10.1126/science.1258096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dweck, H. K. M., Ebrahim, S. A. M., Farhan, A., Hansson, B. S., and Stensmyr, M. C. (2015a). Olfactory proxy detection of dietary antioxidants in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 25, 455–466. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dweck, H. K. M., Ebrahim, S. A. M., Kromann, S., Bown, D., Hillbur, Y., Sachse, S., et al. (2013). Olfactory preference for egg laying on citrus substrates in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 23, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dweck, H. K. M., Ebrahim, S. A. M., Thoma, M., Mohamed, A. A. M., Keesey, I. W., Tronaa, F., et al. (2015b). Pheromones mediating copulation and attraction in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, E2829–E2835. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504527112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebrahim, S. A. M., Dweck, H. K. M., Stökl, J., Hofferberth, J. E., Trona, F., Weniger, K., et al. (2015). Drosophila avoids parasitoids by sensing their semiochemicals via a dedicated olfactory circuit. PLoS Biol. 13:e1002318. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goyret, J., Markwell, P. M., and Raguso, R. A. (2008). Context- and scale-dependent effects of floral CO2 on nectar foraging by Manduca sexta. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 105, 4565–4570. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0708629105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grabe, V., Baschwitz, A., Dweck, H. K. M., Lavista-Llanos, S., Hansson, B. S., and Sachse, S. (2016). Elucidating the neuronal architecture of olfactory glomeruli in the Drosophila antennal lobe. Cell Rep. 16, 3401–3413. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.08.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerenstein, P. G., Christensen, T. A., and Hildebrand, J. G. (2004a). Sensory processing of ambient CO2 information in the brain of the moth Manduca sexta. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 190, 707–725. doi: 10.1007/s00359-004-0529-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerenstein, P. G., Yepez, E. A., Haren, J., Van Williams, D. G., and Hildebrand, J. G. (2004b). Floral CO2 emission may indicate food abundance to nectar-feeding moths. Naturwissenschaften 91, 329–333. doi: 10.1007/s00114-004-0532-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallem, E. A., and Carlson, J. R. (2006). Coding of odors by a receptor repertoire. Cell 125, 143–160. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.01.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansson, B. S., Knaden, M., Sachse, S., Stensmyr, M. C., and Wicher, D. (2010). Towards plant-odor-related olfactory neuroethology in Drosophila. Chemoecology 20, 51–61. doi: 10.1007/s00049-009-0033-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hansson, B. S., and Stensmyr, M. C. (2011). Evolution of insect olfaction. Neuron 72, 698–711. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haverkamp, A., Bing, J., Badeke, E., Hansson, B. S., and Knaden, M. (2016a). Innate olfactory preferences for flowers matching proboscis length ensure optimal energy gain in a hawkmoth. Nat. Commun. 7, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11644

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haverkamp, A., Yon, F., Keesey, I. W., Mißbach, C., Koenig, C., Hansson, B. S., et al. (2016b). Hawkmoths evaluate scenting flowers with the tip of their proboscis. Elife 5:e15039. doi: 10.7554/eLife.15039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinterwirth, A. J., Medina, B., Lockey, J., Otten, D., Voldman, J., Lang, J. H., et al. (2012). Wireless stimulation of antennal muscles in freely flying hawkmoths leads to flight path changes. PLoS ONE 7:e52725. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joseph, R. M., and Carlson, J. R. (2015). Drosophila chemoreceptors : a molecular interface between the chemical world and the brain. Trends Genet. 31, 683–695. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2015.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, J. W., Baeck, S., Perumalsamy, H., Hansson, B. S., Young-Joon, A., and Hyung Wook, K. (2015). A novel olfactory pathway is essential for fast and efficient blood-feeding in mosquitoes. Sci. Rep. 5:13444. doi: 10.1038/srep13444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Junker, R. R., Kuppler, J., Amo, L., Blande, J. D., Borges, R. M., van Dam, N. M., et al. (2017). Covariation and phenotypic integration in chemical communication displays: biosynthetic constraints and eco-evolutionary implications. New Phytol. doi: 10.1111/nph.14505. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kárpáti, Z., Knaden, M., Reinecke, A., and Hansson, B. S. (2013). Intraspecific combinations of flower and leaf volatiles act together in attracting hawkmoth pollinators. PLoS ONE 8:e72805. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0072805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kent, L. B., Walden, K. K. O., and Robertson, H. M. (2008). The Gr family of candidate gustatory and olfactory receptors in the yellow-fever mosquito Aedes aegypti. Chem. Senses 33, 79–93. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjm067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, D., Kallenbach, M., Diezel, C., Rothe, E., Murdock, M., and Baldwin, I. T. (2015). How scent and nectar influence floral antagonists and mutualists. Elife 4:e07641. doi: 10.7554/eLife.07641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirkness, E. F., Haasa, B. J., Sun, W., Braig, H. R., Perottid, M. A., Clarke, J. M., et al. (2011). Genome sequences of the human body louse and its primary endosymbiont provide insights into the permanent parasitic lifestyle. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 6335–6335. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1103909108

CrossRef Full Text

Klein, A. -M., Vaissière, B. E., Cane, J. H., Steffan-Dewenter, I., Cunningham, S. A., Kremen, C., et al. (2007). Importance of pollinators in changing landscapes for world crops. Proc. Biol. Sci. 274, 66, 95–96, 191. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.3721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klinner, C. F., Koenig, C., Missbach, C., Werckenthin, A., Daly, K. C., Bisch-knaden, S., et al. (2016). Functional olfactory sensory neurons housed in olfactory sensilla on the ovipositor of the hawkmoth Manduca sexta. Front. Ecol. Evol. 4:130. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2016.00130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, C., Hirsh, A., Bucks, S., Klinner, C., Vogel, H., Shukla, A., et al. (2015). A reference gene set for chemosensory receptor genes of Manduca sexta. Insect Biochem. Mol. Biol. 66, 51–63. doi: 10.1016/j.ibmb.2015.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurtovic, A., Widmer, A., and Dickson, B. J. (2007). A single class of olfactory neurons mediates behavioural responses to a Drosophila sex pheromone. Nature 446, 542–546. doi: 10.1038/nature05672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, E., Lopez-Martinez, G., Fane, B., and Davidowitz, G. (2017). Hawkmoths use nectar sugar to reduce oxidative damage from flight. Science 355, 733–735. doi: 10.1126/science.aah4634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, H. H., Cao, D. S„ Sethi, S., Zeng, Z., Chin, J. S. R., Chakraborty, T. S., et al. (2016). Hormonal modulation of pheromone detection enhances male courtship success. Neuron 90, 1272–1285. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, C., Pitts, R. J., Bohbot, J. D., Jones, P. L., Wang, G., and Zwiebel, L. J. (2010). Distinct olfactory signaling mechanisms in the malaria vector mosquito Anopheles gambiae. PLoS Biol. 8:e1000467. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, T., Qiu, Y. T., Wang, G., Kwon, J. Y., Rutzler, M., Kwon, H., et al. (2007). Odor coding in the maxillary palp of the malaria vector mosquito Anopheles gambiae. Curr. Biol. 17, 1533–1544. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Majeed, S., Hill, S. R., Birgersson, G., and Ignell, R. (2016). Detection and perception of generic host volatiles by mosquitoes modulate host preference: context dependence of (R)-1-octen-3-ol. R. Soc. Open Sci. 3:160467. doi: 10.1098/rsos.160467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mansourian, S., and Stensmyr, M. C. (2015). The chemical ecology of the fly. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 34, 95–102. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, S. G., and Hildebrand, J. G. (1981). Olfactory mechanisms in the moth Manduca sexta : response characteristics and morphology of central neurons in the antennal lobes. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 213, 249–277. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1981.0066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMeniman, C. J., Corfas, R. A., Matthews, B. J., Ritchie, S. A., and Vosshall, L. B. (2014). Multimodal integration of carbon dioxide and other sensory cues drives mosquito attraction to humans. Cell 156, 1060–1071. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.12.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Münch, D., and Galizia, C. G. (2016). DoOR 2.0 - Comprehensive mapping of Drosophila melanogaster odorant responses. Sci. Rep. 6, 1–14. doi: 10.1038/srep21841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitts, R. J., Rinker, D. C., Jones, P. L., Rokas, A., and Zwiebel, L. J. (2011). Transcriptome profiling of chemosensory appendages in the malaria vector Anopheles gambiae reveals tissue- and sex-specific signatures of odor coding. BMC Genomics 12:271. doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-12-271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proffit, M., Khallaf, M. A., Carrasco, D., Larsson, M. C., and Anderson, P. (2015). “Do you remember the first time?” Host plant preference in a moth is modulated by experiences during larval feeding and adult mating. Ecol. Lett. 18, 365–374. doi: 10.1111/ele.12419

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeves, W. C. (1951). Field studies on carbon dioxide as a possible host simulant to mosquitoes. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 77, 64–66. doi: 10.3181/00379727-77-18679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reisenman, C. E., Lei, H., and Guerenstein, P. G. (2016). Neuroethology of olfactory-guided behavior and its potential application in the control of harmful insects. Front. Physiol. 7:271. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2016.00271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riabinina, O., Task, D., Marr, E., Lin, C. -C., Alford, R., O'Brochta, D. A., et al. (2016). Organization of olfactory centres in the malaria mosquito Anopheles gambiae. Nat. Commun. 7:13010. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riffell, J. A., Lei, H., Abrell, L., and Hildebrand, J. G. (2013). Neural basis of a pollinator's buffet: olfactory specialization and learning in Manduca sexta. Science 339, 200–204. doi: 10.1126/science.1225483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riffell, J. A., Lei, H., Christensen, T. A., and Hildebrand, J. G. (2009a). Characterization and coding of behaviorally significant odor mixtures. Curr. Biol. 19, 335–340. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riffell, J. A., Lei, H., and Hildebrand, J. G. (2009b). Neural correlates of behavior in the moth Manduca sexta in response to complex odors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 19219–19226. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910592106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riffell, J. A., Shlizerman, E., Sanders, E., Abrell, L., Medina, B., Hinterwirth, A. J., et al. (2014). Flower discrimination by pollinators in a dynamic chemical environment. Science 344, 1515–1518. doi: 10.1126/science.1251041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sachse, S., and Beshel, J. (2016). The good, the bad, and the hungry: how the central brain codes odor valence to facilitate food approach in Drosophila. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 40, 53–58. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smallegange, R. C., Bukovinszkiné-Kiss, G., Otieno, B., MBadi, P. A., Takken, W., Mukabana, W. R., et al. (2012). Identification of candidate volatiles that affect the behavioural response of the malaria mosquito Anopheles gambiae sensu stricto to an active kairomone blend: laboratory and semi-field assays. Physiol. Entomol. 31, 60–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3032.2011.00827.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Späethe, A., Reinecke, A., Haverkamp, A., Hansson, B. S., and Knaden, M. (2013). Host plant odors represent immiscible information entities - blend composition and concentration matter in hawkmoths. PLoS ONE 8:e77135. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sponberg, S., Dyhr, J. P., Hall, R. W., and Daniel, T. L. (2015). Luminance-dependent visual processing enables moth flight in low light. Science 348, 1245–1248. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa3042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steck, K., Veit, D., Grandy, R., Badia, S. B., Mathews, Z., Verschure, P., et al. (2012). A high-throughput behavioral paradigm for Drosophila olfaction - The Flywalk. Sci. Rep. 2:361. doi: 10.1038/srep00361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stensmyr, M. C., Dweck, H. K. M., Farhan, A., Ibba, I., Strutz, A., Mukunda, L., et al. (2012). A conserved dedicated olfactory circuit for detecting harmful microbes in Drosophila. Cell 151, 1345–1357. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.09.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterling, P., and Laughlin, S. B. (2015). Principles of Neural Design. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Thomas, S. J., Harrison, R. R., Leonardo, A., and Reynolds, M. S. (2012). A battery-free multichannel digital neural/EMG telemetry system for flying insects. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Circuits Syst. 6, 424–436. doi: 10.1109/TBCAS.2012.2222881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Touhara, K., and Vosshall, L. B. (2009). Sensing odorants and pheromones with chemosensory receptors. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 71, 307–332. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.010908.163209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Loon, J. J. A., Smallegange, R. C., Bukovinszkiné-Kiss, G., Jacobs, F., De Rijk, M., Mukabana, W. R., et al. (2015). Mosquito attraction: crucial role of carbon dioxide in formulation of a five-component blend of human-derived volatiles. J. Chem. Ecol. 41, 567–573. doi: 10.1007/s10886-015-0587-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhulst, N. O., Weldegergis, B. T., Menger, D., and Takken, W. (2016). Attractiveness of volatiles from different body parts to the malaria mosquito Anopheles coluzzii is affected by deodorant compounds. Sci. Rep. 6:27141. doi: 10.1038/srep27141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wicher, D., Morinaga, S., Halty-deLeon, L., Funk, N., Hansson, B., Touhara, K., et al. (2017). Identification and characterization of the bombykal receptor in the hawkmoth Manduca sexta. J. Exp. Biol. 220, 1781–1786. doi: 10.1242/jeb.154260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, X., Rokas, A., Berger, S. L., Ray, A., and Zwiebel, L. J. (2015). Chemoreceptor evolution in Hymenoptera and its implications for the evolution of eusociality. Genome Biol. Evol. 7, 2407–2416. doi: 10.1093/gbe/evv149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhuang, L., Sun, Y., Hu, M., Wu, C., La, X., Chen, X., et al. (2016). Or47b plays a role in Drosophila males' preference for younger mates. Open Biol. 6, 1–8. doi: 10.1098/rsob.160086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: olfaction, Drosophila, mosquito, moth, labeled line, odor background

Citation: Haverkamp A, Hansson BS and Knaden M (2018) Combinatorial Codes and Labeled Lines: How Insects Use Olfactory Cues to Find and Judge Food, Mates, and Oviposition Sites in Complex Environments. Front. Physiol. 9:49. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.00049

Received: 27 September 2017; Accepted: 16 January 2018;
Published: 01 February 2018.

Edited by:

Shigehiro Namiki, The University of Tokyo, Japan

Reviewed by:

Hong Lei, Arizona State University, United States
Nicolas Durand, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, France

Copyright © 2018 Haverkamp, Hansson and Knaden. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Markus Knaden, mknaden@ice.mpg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.